Startseite The syntax of African American English borrowings in the Louisiana Creole tense-mood-aspect system
Artikel Open Access

The syntax of African American English borrowings in the Louisiana Creole tense-mood-aspect system

  • Oliver Mayeux ORCID logo EMAIL logo
Veröffentlicht/Copyright: 25. Juni 2024

Abstract

This paper presents the typologically unusual case of borrowed tense-mood-aspect morphemes. Data are taken from Louisiana Creole, a critically endangered French-lexifier creole. Over the course of its history, Louisiana Creole has been in contact with local varieties of both French and English, including African American English. It will be shown that points of structural congruity between Louisiana Creole and African American English have facilitated the borrowing of two aspect markers for speakers competent in both varieties. African American English stressed BIN has been borrowed and marks remote past habitual, stative, and completive. The adverb still has been borrowed and subsequently has grammaticalized as a continuative marker via spec-to-head reanalysis. These borrowings are integrated into the inflectional domain as functional heads marking aspect. Their ordering constraints are evaluated relative to a previous hierarchy proposed by Rottet. Discussion of contact-induced change in creole languages has typically been confined to examination of interactions with the lexifier, the language which contributes the majority of a creole’s vocabulary (in this case, French). Fewer studies have presented detailed accounts of how creoles behave when in contact with other languages, meaning that this particular contact context remains undertheorized.

1 Introduction

This paper documents two ongoing processes of contact-induced change in Louisiana Creole (LC), a critically endangered French-lexifier creole language spoken mostly in Louisiana in the United States. Many – but not all – speakers of LC are also regularly users of local varieties of African American English (AAE). Contact between these two varieties has resulted in the ongoing incorporation of at least two new morphemes into the LC tense-mood-aspect (TMA) system. The first is a borrowing of AAE stressed BIN, which marks the remote past with a range of aspectual meanings. Some speakers additionally have borrowed the English adverb still, which is grammaticalizing as a marker of continuative aspect.

The borrowing of TMA morphemes is rare in crosslinguistic perspective (Matras 2007) and has not been thoroughly analyzed in LC. Discussion of contact-induced change in creole languages has typically been confined to examination of interactions with the lexifier, the language which contributes the majority of a creole’s vocabulary. Far fewer studies have presented detailed accounts of how creoles behave when in contact with other languages, meaning that this particular contact context remains undertheorized (Aceto 1999; Siegel 2010; Snow 2000).

This paper aims to contribute to the documentation of the first instances of contact-induced change in LC motivated by contact with AAE and, in so doing, to the theory of how creole languages behave when in contact with languages other than their lexifiers. Section 2 presents the necessary background for the study, reviewing the linguistic ecology of LC in Louisiana in historical-sociolinguistic perspective, with a special emphasis on contact with AAE. Section 3 reviews the data used in the study. The analysis itself proceeds in two parts in Section 4, examining how AAE stressed BIN and the adverb still are incorporated into LC. These changes are discussed in broader perspective in Section 5, with special emphasis on how degrees of structural similarity modulate language contact. Concluding remarks are presented in Section 6.

2 Louisiana Creole and African American English in Louisiana

Today, LC is spoken by a dwindling number of elderly people – probably around 6,000 – who are all bilingual in English and have varying levels of competence in local varieties of Louisiana Regional French (Klingler 2003; Klingler and Neumann-Holzschuh 2013; Neumann 1985; Valdman 1997; Valdman et al. 1998). The speech community today is mostly concentrated in rural south Louisiana, although smaller diaspora communities exist especially in east Texas (see Wendte 2020). These rural communities remained insulated from the gradual Americanization of Louisiana which followed the sale of the territory to the United States in 1803, although local bourgeois classes adopted English relatively early (Fortier 1891: 77; Morgan 1970). All this changed, however, in the first half of the twentieth century, when even the most isolated communities underwent a shift to English precipitated by changes in statewide educational and language policy (Dajko 2012; Dubois et al. 2018; Landry 2016).

The Americanization of Louisiana brought with it not only the dominance of English but also the imposition of a Black/White racial binary as part of the antebellum regime of “Jim Crow” racial segregation. This bifurcated rural Creole communities, where the construction of race and ethnicity had previously been negotiated as an interaction between tripartite legal status (blancs, noirs, gens de couleur libres – White, Black, Free People of Color) and a number of phenotypical descriptors (Barthé 2021; Domínguez 1986; Kein 2000; Landry 2016). Race and ethnicity therefore constitute key social constructs for understanding language variation in Louisiana in general, and in LC in particular (Dajko 2012; Klingler 2019; Neumann 1983; Wendte 2020). Previous quantitative studies (Mayeux 2017, 2019; Klingler 2019) have shown that variation and change in LC pattern according to the Anglo-American Black/White racial binary due to differing degrees of contact with French on either side of the Jim Crow divide (see Mayeux 2024).

Race and ethnicity have also shaped the nature of contact with English. It is important to understand that LC exists in contact with local varieties of English which themselves display contact features from LC and Louisiana Regional French. This process of leveling was first noted in publications based on field data collected from older speakers in the 1950s (Morgan 1959, 1970). Studies on English in rural Louisiana identify two distinct varieties based on race (for recent work on urban Louisiana, on the English of New Orleans, see Dajko and Carmichael 2023). Cajun English is the name given to a variety of English spoken by White, Cajun-identified Louisianians from historically French- and LC-speaking communities (Carmichael 2019). Cajun English has been the subject of a number of sociolinguistic studies, many of which comment on the presence of phonological and morphosyntactic features originating in LC and Louisiana Regional French (Carmichael 2013, 2018; Dubois 2002; Dubois and Horvath 1998, 1999, 2001, 2003a). Another variety, which has received less attention, has been termed Creole African American Vernacular English by Dubois and Horvath (2003b). This variety also exhibits various features of contact with LC and Louisiana Regional French which distinguish it as a subvariety of AAE (Dubois and Horvath 2003b; see also Oetting and Garrity 2006). AAE, and its local incarnations, has had an important presence in Louisiana since at least the early-to-mid nineteenth century. Picone (2003) provides the most detailed historical-sociolinguistic overview, demonstrating that the arrival of enslaved and formerly enslaved anglophones in the plantation societies of Louisiana exerted considerable pressure from below in encouraging shift to English. This also resulted in the introduction of contact features into local AAE, for example pronominal tagging (e.g., I got no chance, me; Picone 2003: 421).

Contact with local varieties of English has exerted extensive influence on LC. Best documented are the widespread processes of lexical borrowing and code-switching. The impact on the grammar of LC, however, has so far been subject only to preliminary investigation. Instead, much of the focus has been on the results of contact with French. This follows from an emphasis in Creole Studies on decreolization, the process of contact between a creole and its lexifier, which may result in the former becoming more similar to the latter (Bickerton 1980; DeCamp 1971; Holm 2000; Whinnom 1971). Decreolization has been roundly critiqued (see the comprehensive critical review in Siegel 2010). For our purposes, an important shortcoming of decreolization is that it excludes contexts where creoles are in contact with languages other than their lexifiers (Aceto 1999), as is the case in Louisiana. It is perhaps for this reason that this kind of contact remains under-researched (Bartens 2002; Snow 2000).

3 Data

Data are taken from a diachronic corpus of LC (Mayeux 2019), which combines historical language documentation with fieldwork recordings from 2017. Although the whole corpus was examined for the variables discussed here, no occurrences in historical texts were found. The variables were only attested in recordings made in 2017, which comprise sociolinguistic interviews from 27 speakers in St. Martin and St. James Parishes, Louisiana. Samples averaging 30 min were transcribed and analyzed for each of the speakers represented. Occurrences of the variables in this study – bɪn (Section 4.1) and stɪl (Section 4.2) – were coded. LC examples are transcribed using the International Phonetic Alphabet; English insertions are transcribed orthographically. Corpus examples are followed by 2017XX, where XX is a two-letter code identifying the speaker. Syntax trees were generated using RSyntaxTree (Hasebe 2023).

4 Analysis

Like both other French-lexifier creoles (see Syea 2017) and AAE (see Green 2002), LC encodes TMA through a series of preverbal function words usually referred to as particles or markers (Klingler 2003; Klingler and Neumann-Holzschuh 2013; Neumann 1985). This paper builds on the analysis in Rottet (1992), who finds that TMA and the negator p(a) follow strict ordering before the verb, given in (1). Rottet (1992: 275) analyzes each marker as the spell-out of a functional head, deriving the tense phrase (TP) structure in (2).

(1)
Tense > Mood > Negation > Aspect > Verb
(2)
[TP [MoodP [NegP [AspP [VP ]]]]] (Rottet 1992: 275)

Following Henri (2016; see also Henri and Klingler 2014), this paper adopts a preliminary analysis of the negator p(a) as a clitic. The analysis also incorporates Rottet’s suggestion that LC aspectual heads can be “stacked” (1992: 275, n. 12), that is, that more than one AspP may appear in a given TP. This notion is compatible with the hierarchy of clausal functional projections in Cinque (1999). Elements of this cartographic approach are drawn upon here to extend Rottet’s account of the LC inflectional domain.

The present study treats two new borrowings into this system, which both have their origins in AAE: bɪn (< stressed BIN; Section 4.1) and stɪl (<still; Section 4.2). The sociolinguistic and linguistic factors involved in this contact-induced change are discussed in Section 5.

4.1 Remote past bɪn

The presence of the form [bɪn] in LC is attested in Neumann (1985: 211) and Klingler (2003: 262–263). Both grammars of LC classify bɪn as a borrowing of English perfective been (Klingler 2003: 262–263; Neumann 1985: 211). Rottet (1992: 270–272) follows these descriptions. In this section, bɪn is shown to originate from the phonologically identical AAE stressed BIN, which in fact can be straightforwardly incorporated into the phrase structure proposed by Rottet (1992). AAE BIN is a “camouflaged” (Spears 1982) AAE feature, that is, one which is phonologically similar to, but syntactically and semantically distinct from, a feature in other varieties of English. This may explain why LC bɪn has been mistaken as a borrowing of perfective been.

First analyzed as a remote past marker by Labov (1972) and Rickford (1975), AAE stressed BIN indicates that “the time period referred to is longer than normal for an activity, or … that a state has indeed held for a long [period] of time” (Green 2002: 54). Incidentally, this resembles the description for LC bɪn in Klingler (2003: 263) as encoding “an action or state that began before, and continued up to, a subsequent point in time, which may be the moment of utterance or a point prior to it”. The form bɪn is distributed categorically across speakers in the corpus and in other available data (Klingler 2003; Neumann 1985), attested only amongst Black speakers who are proficient in AAE.

AAE BIN is normally transcribed in all capitals, a convention which denotes that it typically bears prosodic stress, unlike its perfective counterpart been (Green 2002: 54). When it is used, bɪn bears prosodic stress as in (3) (⟨ˈˈ⟩ indicates prosodic stress). No counterexample was heard during fieldwork.[1]

(3)
⟨Vitamin C⟩ ˈˈbɪn la!
Vitamin C remp cop there
‘Vitamin C has been around for a long time (and still is)!’
(2017EJ)

AAE BIN has varied aspectual readings distinct from those of perfective been. Green (1998a; see also Green 2002: 55–59) finds BIN can encode stative aspect, as in (4a); habitual aspect, as in (5a); and completive aspect, as in (6a). All three are attested for contemporary LC bɪn, as seen in (4b)–(6b).

(4)
Stative
a.
He BIN running.
‘He’s been running for a long time and still is.’
(Green 1998a: 117)
b.
li bɪn muri ɛ̃?
3s remp.stat dead eh
‘He has been dead for a long time (and still is), eh?’
(2017EO)
(5)
Habitual
a.
Bruce BIN running.
‘Bruce started running some time ago and he still runs from time to time.’
(Green 2002: 57)
b.
mo bɪn ole ⟨retire⟩
1s remp.hab want retire
‘I’ve been wanting to retire for a long time (and still want to).’
(2017ML)
(6)
Completive
a.
Bruce BIN ate those potato chips.
‘Bruce ate those potato chips a long time ago (and is no longer doing so).’
(Green 1998a: 130)
b.
n t ape kite, nu bɪn ape ⟨pack up⟩ no zafɛ
1pl ant prog leave 1pl remp.cmpl prog pack up 1pl.poss thing
‘We were leaving, we had been packing up our things for a long time (and no longer were).’
(2017BM)

Further similarities are found in the domain of negation. As in AAE (Green 1995), bɪn cannot take sentence-level negation, as shown in (7a). AAE negative auxiliaries ain’t or haven’t can be used in metalinguistic negation and ellipsis contexts (e.g., He ain’t BIN running; he only just started; see Green et al. 2022: 964). This may be possible in LC, as seen in (7b), though no examples of this kind of metalinguistic negation are attested in the corpus. When the negator p(a) is placed after bɪn in (7c), another constructed example, the result is a change of scope.

(7)
a.
*li pa= bɪn muri
3s neg remp.stat die
Intended: ‘He has not been dead for a long time.’
b.
li pa= bɪn muri; li ʒɪʃ sɔr muri
3s neg remp.stat die 3s just aux die.
‘He hasn’t been dead for a long time; he only just died.’
cf. AAE He ain’t BIN dead; he only just died. (after Green et al. 2022: 964)
c.
li bɪn pa= muri
3s remp.stat neg die
‘He has been not-dead for a long time (i.e., he has been alive for a long time and still is).’

Finally, English perfective been is not rendered in LC using bɪn. Example (8a) contains an extended code-switch into English where the perfective been is used (we’ve been there). This is rendered in LC with a plain verb kuri ‘go’. With bɪn, the sentence would produce a different interpretation, denoting the remote past with stative aspect rather than a perfective aspect (i.e., ‘we’ve been there and still are there’). A similar case can be found in Neumann (1985), reproduced in (8b). The example shows an insertion of been in a calque on the English have been to V construction associated with motion events (see Gesuato 2012).

(8)
a.
paske ⟨we’ve been there⟩ [.] no kuri la
because we’ve been there 1pl go there
‘Because we’ve been there. We’ve been there (and no longer are).’
(2017LD)
b.
m o te pa= ⟨been⟩ wa twa lɔ̃tɔ̃ e tɔ̃t?
1s pst neg been see 2s long.time eh aunt
‘I have not been to see you in a long time, eh Auntie?’
(Neumann 1985: 211, n. 1)

Given its syntactic and semantic properties, I posit that remote past bɪn can be incorporated into Rottet’s (1992) phrase structure as a functional head which lexicalizes stative, habitual, or completive aspects, as in (4)–(6). This is the same position it occupies in AAE (Green 1998b). This AspSTAT/HAB/CMPL is higher than the progressive AspPROG, since bɪn must precede the progressive marker where the two co-occur, as in (6b) (though see footnote 5 for discussion of another potential solution for future investigation). This structure is schematized in (9).

(9)

This analysis allows for tense markers to appear under T. This structure is not found in my data from St. Martin or St. James Parishes. In the LC of Pointe Coupee Parish, however, two examples where anterior te precedes bɪn are recorded by Klingler (2003: 263). This possible regional variation, which should be subject to further investigation, may result from differing sociolinguistic dynamics in that region. By way of summary, I present one of these examples from Klingler (2003: 263) in (10a) and (10b), which shows a more fully elaborated structure.

(10)
a.
nu te bɪn e vole
1pl ant remp.hab prog steal
‘We had been stealing (peaches) for a long time.’
(Klingler 2003: 263)[2]
b.

4.2 Continuative stɪl

In another incipient process, an English-origin temporal adverb may be undergoing grammaticalization as preverbal marker for the continuative aspect. The adverb [stɪ(l)] ‘still’ (< English still) is recorded as an alternative to the adverb tuʒu ‘always; still’ (Klingler 2003: 343; Neumann 1985: 27). Today, speakers who also know AAE have replaced tuʒu with stɪl for the sense ‘still’, as in (11). For the sense ‘always’, tuʒu is used, as in (12). I submit that this change has occurred due to reanalysis of this adverb as an aspect marker on the basis of structural similarities between preverbal TMA elements in both AAE and LC.

(11)
no stɪl mãʒ pul
1pl cont eat chicken
‘We still eat chicken.’
(2017BB)
(12)
to kɔ̃nɛ̃ to tuʒu bɛzɛ̃n kɛkʃɔʒ
2s know 2s always need something
‘You know, you always need something.’
(2017BB)

Since adverbs can encode aspectual information and may act as the specifiers of functional heads (see Cinque 1999: 95), they represent good candidates for grammaticalization as aspect markers. In languages where adverbials share a similar distribution to TMA markers – as is the case in both LC and AAE – their reanalysis and grammaticalization is robustly attested (Abunya and Amfo 2013; Bybee et al. 1994: 240; Marchese 1986). In such cases, another sign of the grammaticalization of adverbs as TMA markers is phonological reduction (cf. Bybee et al. 1994), as seen, for example, in Nyabo kɛ́ɛ̄té ‘today’ > kɛ̀ɛ̄ prs (Marchese 1986: 258) and Kaakyi ɔ̀kɛ́ ‘tomorrow’ > kɛ̀ fut (Abunya and Amfo 2013). LC continuative stɪl also occurs in a phonologically reduced form [stɪ ∼ sti], even where followed by a vowel, as in (13).

(13)
me le piti astœr je sti ole!
but pl child now 3pl cont want
‘But the children nowadays, they keep wanting [things].’
(2017MY)

The first stage in this process of grammaticalization was the substitution of the borrowed adverb stɪl for tuʒu in cases where the latter adverb had a continuative reading (‘still’). Here, stɪl took on the special distribution of tuʒu, which belongs to a closed class of adverbials which can appear preceding the verb (Neumann 1985: 296), as in (14) (cf. example [12]). Other adverbials, for example manner adverbs such as dusmã ‘slowly’, cannot, as is shown in (15a)–(15b).

(14)
no tuʒu mãʒ pul
1pl still eat chicken
‘We still eat chicken.’
(15)
a.
no pal dusmã
1pl speak slowly
‘We speak slowly.’
b.
*no dusmã pal
1pl slowly speak
Intended: ‘We speak slowly.’

The second stage in the process concerns how the borrowed adverb stɪl was grammaticalized. I propose that stɪl goes from being the AdvP specifier of AspCONT to being the head, as in (16). This change proceeds by spec-to-head reanalysis in accordance with the Head Preference Principle, which holds that grammars will prefer heads rather than a phrases (van Gelderen 2004).

(16)

Within this analysis, stɪl conforms to the principles of ordering outlined in Rottet (1992) and must appear following tense elements such as te, as in (17), or the future a, in (18). Example (18) also shows that, as for bɪn, more than one aspect marker may co-occur, though with ordering constraints. In (18), stɪl precedes the progressive marker ape and may not follow it. The architecture of examples such as (18) can be schematized as in (19) according to Rottet’s analysis.

(17)
li te stɪl dɑ̃ lɛkɔl
3s ant still in school
‘She was still in school.’
(2017MM)
(18)
n a stɪl ape pale krejɔl
1s fut cont prog speak Creole
‘We will still be speaking Creole.’
(2017GB)
(19)

Finally, as for bɪn, the presence of multiple aspectual heads can also allow for contrastive negative scope (via different merging positions of Neg as per Cinque 1999: 140–141). In (20a), the negator p(a) intervenes between continuative stɪl and progressive ape. Sentences such as (20b) are also judged as acceptable, albeit with a different reading. Sentences such as (20c) may represent the adverbial adjunction of stɪl to the TP (Rottet 1992: 285–286), possibly for focus purposes.

(20)
a.
li te stɪl p= ape pale frãse
3s pst cont neg prog speak French
‘She still was not speaking French.’
= ‘It remained the case that she was not speaking French.’
(2017GB)
b.
li te pa= stɪl ape pale frãse
3s pst neg cont prog speak French
‘She was not still speaking French.’
= ‘It did not remain the case that she was speaking French.’
c.
?li stɪl te p= ape pale frãse
3s cont pst neg prog speak French
Intended: ‘Still, she was not speaking French.’

It is my hypothesis that this reanalysis of stɪl has been facilitated in particular by contact with AAE, explaining why nearly all speakers who employ this feature are proficient in that variety. This is because AAE, like LC, features a distributionally similar set of preverbal aspectual heads. LC-AAE bilinguals are primed to interpret the aspectual semantics on adverbials preceding the verb; since stɪl shares both the distribution and semantics of a TMA marker, it is vulnerable to grammaticalization as part of this system by means of spec-to-head reanalysis in accordance with the Head Preference Principle (van Gelderen 2004).

5 Discussion

To understand the changes-in-progress examined in this study, we must draw on a framework of contact-induced change which acknowledges the interplay between sociolinguistic context and linguistic change, including language-internal mechanisms of reanalysis (Chamoreau and Léglise 2012; Farrar and Jones 2002; Thomason and Kaufman 1988; Weinreich 1953).

The results of the analysis can be understood from a historical-sociolinguistic point of view as the result of differing levels of contact with different superstrates on either side of the Jim Crow divide in the areas where I have conducted fieldwork. In communities which were racialized as Black, LC took on an important role as a marker of ethnolinguistic identity (Maguire 1987). In White communities, though, LC speakers in St. Martin Parish were under pressure to dissociate themselves from Blackness and therefore to accommodate to Louisiana Regional French (Neumann 1983; Tentchoff 1977; see Mayeux 2024). Therefore, although both Black and White communities were in intensive contact with English, it is in Black communities that LC was maintained for longest in St. Martin Parish. One result of this maintenance is the increased presence of local English features – and, in particular, local AAE features – in the speech of Black speakers of this variety of LC.[3]

The linguistic consequences of these sociolinguistic dynamics are evidenced in the borrowing of bɪn and stɪl and their incorporation into LC as aspect markers. This represents a case of the borrowing of morphosyntactic matter, resulting from the prolonged intensive contact described above. However, the borrowing of TMA morphemes is rare even in situations of intensive contact (Matras 2007: 44). I suggest that the borrowing of bɪn and stɪl has been facilitated not only by the intensive contact between LC and AAE, but by structural congruity between these two systems. Alongside the intensity of contact, structural and lexical congruity have long been known to modulate borrowing (Thomason and Kaufman 1988; Weinreich 1953). For instance, Jones (2015) compares contact between Mainland Norman and French, two very similar varieties, to that between Insular Norman and English, which are more distantly related. She finds the close relationship between Mainland Norman and French facilitates the transfer of grammatical material. The result is that changes in Mainland Norman driven by contact with French are more fully “worked through” than those in Insular Norman driven by contact with English (Jones 2015: 142).

LC and AAE share similar syntactic structures in their inflectional domains, with both marking aspect on functional heads. The borrowings bɪn and stɪl share near-identical distributions in both LC and AAE. Compare the LC sentences in (21a) and (22a) with their AAE renderings in (21b) and (22b).[4]

(21)
bɪn
a.
li bɪn muri!
3s remp.stat dead
(2017EO)
b.
He BIN dead!
3s remp.stat dead
‘He’s been dead for a long time!’
(22)
stɪl
a.
li stɪl ɛ̃ hɔg.
3s cont cop indef hog
(2017EO)
b.
He s till a hog.
3s.m cont cop indef hog
‘He still is a hog (promiscuous male).’

In the case of bɪn, this represents a more straightforward case of the borrowing of structurally congruent material. The case of stɪl involves multiple stages, beginning with the borrowing of the adverb still (which has no special distribution in AAE). Once borrowed into LC, stɪl was subsequently grammaticalized via reanalysis from adverb specifier to head of AspCONTP in accordance with the Head Preference Principle (van Gelderen 2004; see Section 4.2). In both cases, LC-AAE bilinguals employ similar structures across both linguistic systems which facilitates transfer of material and its grammaticalization.

Finally, I propose a revision to the hierarchy in (1) and (2) (see Section 4) proposed by Rottet (1992; cited in Cinque 1999: 164). The present analysis takes up Rottet’s suggestion that aspectual heads may be “stacked” (1992: 275, n. 12), a notion compatible with the approach in Cinque (1999). The first task is to account for the ordering of aspect markers. It has been shown that bɪn and stɪl must both precede, not follow, the progressive ape. Since no co-occurrences of bɪn and stɪl are attested, their relative ordering constraints are unclear. I provisionally place them within a single point on the hierarchy, pending further research.[5] Cinque (1999: 140–141) proposes that negation may merge in various positions depending on language- and context-specific considerations. In LC, Henri’s (2016) analysis of p(a) as a clitic allows its insertion in various positions to obtain different scopal readings, as in (20) in Section 4.2. Nevertheless, it must occur below mood (Rottet 1992). These observations are summarized in (23).

(23)
Tense > Mood > (Negation) > Aspect {STAT, HAB, CMPL, CONT} > (Negation) > Aspect PROG > Verb

6 Conclusions

This study has documented two novel aspect markers, bɪn and stɪl, which have emerged in LC due to that language’s intensive contact with AAE. This relatively rare case of morphosyntactic borrowing has been facilitated in linguistic terms by the syntactic congruity between LC and AAE, both of which employ preverbal aspect markers subject to similar ordering constraints. Now that these novel aspect markers have been documented, future research should seek to uncover the finer details of the LC inflectional domain, notably negation, and situate these findings in crosslinguistic perspective. Future work should also seek to investigate variation in different varieties of LC, notably that of Pointe Coupee Parish, as well as the extent to which AAE contact features are found in Louisiana Regional French.

The borrowing of bɪn and stɪl constitutes a case of contact-induced change which involves the reanalysis and grammaticalization of borrowed material, emphasizing the need to consider multiple causation in language contact (Farrar and Jones 2002; Mougeon and Beniak 1991; Thomason and Kaufman 1988). The results of this study confirm a need for further examination of contact between creoles and languages other than their lexifiers (Aceto 1999; Snow 2000), which may present valuable data for the comparative study of language contact. Creoles and their lexifiers hold a particular sociohistorical relationship and, by definition, share most of their lexical material. On the other hand, creoles and non-lexifier languages share much less lexical material and diverge structurally. The comparison of creole/lexifier and creole/non-lexifier contact may thus yield important insights into the borrowability of material as a function of linguistic similarity however defined (see also Mayeux 2019; Thomason and Kaufman 1988; Yakpo 2021).


Corresponding author: Oliver Mayeux, 2152 University of Cambridge , Cambridge, UK, E-mail:

Award Identifier / Grant number: 1687496

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to two anonymous reviewers for their careful, constructive comments that significantly improved the analysis and discussion in this paper. Special thanks to Kim Groothuis for helpful feedback on an earlier draft, to Ștefania Costea and Sara Cardullo for their impromptu syntax supervision at the pub, and to Theresa Biberauer for valuable input and encouragement. Thanks also to the members of the University of Cambridge SyntaxLab, where an earlier version of this analysis was presented in 2018, and to Mari C. Jones for comments on preliminary work. Data collection for this work was part funded by UKRI grant 1687496 and by residencies at NUNU Arts and Culture Collective, Arnaudville, Louisiana. For the purpose of Open Access, the author has applied a Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising.

References

Abunya, Levina Nyameye & Nana Aba Appiah Amfo. 2013. Grammaticalization in Kaakyi: From a temporal adverb to a future tense marker. Acta Linguistica Hafniensia 45(1). 126–139. https://doi.org/10.1080/03740463.2014.897813.Suche in Google Scholar

Aceto, Michael. 1999. Looking beyond decreolization as an explanatory model of language change in creole-speaking communities. Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages 14(1). 93–119. https://doi.org/10.1075/jpcl.14.1.04ace.Suche in Google Scholar

Bartens, Angela. 2002. Another short note on creoles in contact with non-lexifier prestige languages. Journal of Pidgin and Creole Languages 17(2). 273–278. https://doi.org/10.1075/jpcl.17.2.08bar.Suche in Google Scholar

Barthé, Darryl. 2021. Becoming American in Creole New Orleans, 1896–1949. Baton Rouge, LA: Louisiana State University Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Bickerton, Derek. 1980. Decreolization and the creole continuum. In Albert Valdman & A. Highfield (eds.), Theoretical orientations in Creole Studies, 109–128. New York, NY: Academic Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Bybee, Joan L., Revere D. Perkins & William Pagliuca. 1994. The evolution of grammar: Tense, aspect, and modality in the languages of the world. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Carmichael, Katie. 2013. The performance of Cajun English in Boudreaux and Thibodeaux jokes. American Speech 88(4). 377–412. https://doi.org/10.1215/00031283-2691415.Suche in Google Scholar

Carmichael, Katie. 2018. Cajuns as Southe(r)ne(r)s? An examination of variable r-lessness in Cajun English. In Jeffrey Reaser, Eric Wilbanks, Karissa Wojcik & Walt Wolfram (eds.), Language variety in the new South: Contemporary perspectives on change and variation, 135–152. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Carmichael, Katie. 2019. Cajun English in Louisiana: A linguistic and cultural profile. In Nathalie Dajko & Shana Walton (eds.), Languages in Louisiana. Jackson, MS: University Press of Mississippi.10.14325/mississippi/9781496823854.003.0011Suche in Google Scholar

Chamoreau, Claudine & Isabelle Léglise. 2012. A multi-model approach to contact-induced language change. In Claudine Chamoreau & Isabelle Léglise (eds.), Dynamics of contact-induced language change, 1–16. Berlin: De Gruyter.10.1515/9783110271430.1Suche in Google Scholar

Cinque, Guglielmo. 1999. Adverbs and functional heads: A cross-linguistic perspective (Oxford Studies in Comparative Syntax). Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oso/9780195115260.001.0001Suche in Google Scholar

Dajko, Nathalie. 2012. Sociolinguistics of ethnicity in francophone Louisiana: Language and ethnicity in French Louisiana. Language and Linguistics Compass 6(5). 279–295. https://doi.org/10.1002/lnc3.333.Suche in Google Scholar

Dajko, Nathalie & Katie Carmichael. 2023. Plus ça change…Perceptions of New Orleans English before and after the storm. Journal of English Linguistics 51(2). 95–132. https://doi.org/10.1177/00754242231163849.Suche in Google Scholar

DeCamp, David. 1971. Towards a generative analysis of a post-creole speech continuum. In Dell Hymes (ed.), Pidginization and creolization of languages, 349–370. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Domínguez, Virginia R. 1986. White By definition: Social classification in Creole Louisiana. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Dubois, Sylvie. 2002. Sounding Cajun: The rhetorical use of dialect in speech and writing. American Speech 77(3). 264–287. https://doi.org/10.1215/00031283-77-3-264.Suche in Google Scholar

Dubois, Sylvie & Barbara M. Horvath. 1998. Let’s tink about dat: Interdental fricatives in Cajun English. Language Variation and Change 10(3). 245–261. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954394500001320.Suche in Google Scholar

Dubois, Sylvie & Barbara M. Horvath. 1999. When the music changes, you change too: Gender and language change in Cajun English. Language Variation and Change 11(3). 287–313. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954394599113036.Suche in Google Scholar

Dubois, Sylvie & Barbara M. Horvath. 2001. Do Cajuns speak Southern English? Morphosyntactic evidence. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 7(3). 27–41.Suche in Google Scholar

Dubois, Sylvie & Barbara M. Horvath. 2003a. Verbal morphology in Cajun Vernacular English: A comparison with other varieties of Southern English. Journal of English Linguistics 31(1). 34–59. https://doi.org/10.1177/0075424202250296.Suche in Google Scholar

Dubois, Sylvie & Barbara M. Horvath. 2003b. The English vernacular of the Creoles of Louisiana. Language Variation and Change 15(3). 255–288. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095439450315301X.Suche in Google Scholar

Dubois, Sylvie, Emilie Gagnet Leumas & Malcolm Richardson. 2018. Speaking French in Louisiana, 1720–1955: Linguistic practices of the Catholic Church. Baton Rouge, LA: Louisiana State University Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Farrar, Kimberley & Mari C. Jones. 2002. Introduction. In Mari C. Jones & Edith Esch (eds.), Language change: The interplay of internal, external and extra-linguistic factors. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.10.1515/9783110892598.1Suche in Google Scholar

Fortier, Alcée. 1891. The Acadians of Louisiana and their dialect. PMLA 6(1). 64–94. https://doi.org/10.2307/456297.Suche in Google Scholar

Gelderen, Elly van. 2004. Grammaticalization as economy. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Suche in Google Scholar

Gesuato, Sara. 2012. Semantic patterns of HAVE been to V: Corpus data and elicited data. In Sebastian Hoffman, Paul Rayson & Geoffrey Leech (eds.), English corpus linguistics: Looking back, moving forward, 57–78. Leiden: Brill.10.1163/9789401207478_006Suche in Google Scholar

Green, Lisa J. 1995. Study of verb classes in African American English. Linguistics and Education 7(1). 65–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/0898-5898(95)90020-9.Suche in Google Scholar

Green, Lisa J. 1998a. Remote past and states in African-American English. American Speech 73(2). 115–138. https://doi.org/10.2307/455736.Suche in Google Scholar

Green, Lisa J. 1998b. Aspect and predicate phrases in African-American vernacular English. In Salikoko S. Mufwene, John R. Rickford, Guy Bailey & John Baugh (eds.), African-American English, 37–68. New York, NY: Routledge.Suche in Google Scholar

Green, Lisa J. 2002. African American English: A linguistic introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511800306Suche in Google Scholar

Green, Lisa J., Kristine M. Yu, Anissa Neal, Ayana, Whitmal, Tamira Powe & Deniz Özyıldız. 2022. Range in the use and realization of BIN in African American English. Language and Speech 65(4). 958–1006. https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309221111201.Suche in Google Scholar

Hasebe, Yoichiro. 2023. RSyntaxTree: A graphical syntax tree image generator. Available at: https://yohasebe.com/rsyntaxtree.Suche in Google Scholar

Henri, Fabiola. 2016. Conditioned reordering in Louisiana Creole: The case of sentential negation. Paper presented at the Society of Pidgin and Creole Languages Conference, Washington DC, 8–10 January.Suche in Google Scholar

Henri, Fabiola & Thomas A. Klingler. 2014. If you look closely: Inflectional morphology in Louisiana Creole. Paper presented at the Workshop on Creole Complexity, University of Kentucky, 8 March.Suche in Google Scholar

Holm, John. 2000. An introduction to pidgins and creoles. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781139164153Suche in Google Scholar

Jones, Mari C. 2015. Variation and change in Mainland and Insular Norman: A study of superstrate influence. Leiden: Brill.10.1163/9789004257139Suche in Google Scholar

Kein, Sybil. 2000. Introduction. In Sybil Kein (ed.), Creole: The history and legacy of Louisiana’s free people of color, xiii–xxiv. Baton Rouge, LA: Louisiana State University Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Klingler, Thomas A. & Ingrid Neumann-Holzschuh. 2013. Louisiana Creole structure dataset. In Susanne Maria Michaelis, Philippe Maurer, Martin Haspelmath & Magnus Huber (eds.), Atlas of pidgin and creole language structures online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Available at: http://apics-online.info/contributions/53.Suche in Google Scholar

Klingler, Thomas A. 1998. Français cadien, créole des Blancs et créole des Noirs en Louisiane. In Patrice Brasseur (ed.), Français d’Amérique: Variation, créolisation, normalisation, 205–215. Avignon: Centre d’Études Canadiennes.Suche in Google Scholar

Klingler, Thomas A. 2003. If I could turn my tongue like that: The Creole language of Pointe Coupee Parish, Louisiana. Baton Rouge, LA: Louisiana State University Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Klingler, Thomas A. 2019. La variation ethnolinguistique en créole louisianais au cours du XXe siècle. In Edith Szlezák & Klara Stephanie Szlezák (eds.), Sprach- und Kulturkontaktphänomene in der Romania – Phénomènes de contact linguistique et culturel dans la Romania Festschrift für Ingrid Neumann-Holzschuh zum 65. Geburtstag, 53–68. Berlin: Erich Schmidt.Suche in Google Scholar

Labov, William. 1972. Language in the inner city: Studies in the Black English vernacular. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Landry, Christophe. 2016. A Creole melting pot: The politics of language, race, and identity in southwest Louisiana, 1918–1945. Brighton: University of Sussex doctoral thesis. Available at: http://sro.sussex.ac.uk/id/eprint/86158/.Suche in Google Scholar

Maguire, Robert E. 1987. Hustling to survive: Social and economic change in a South Louisiana Black Creole community. Montréal: McGill University doctoral thesis.Suche in Google Scholar

Marchese, Lynell. 1986. Tense/aspect and the development of auxiliaries in Kru languages. Dallas, TX: Summer Institute of Linguistics.Suche in Google Scholar

Matras, Yaron. 2007. The borrowability of structural categories. In Yaron Matras & Jeanette Sakel (eds.), Grammatical borrowing in cross-linguistic perspective, 31–74. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110199192.31Suche in Google Scholar

Mayeux, Oliver. 2017. Agreement in Louisiana Creole: Quantitative sociolinguistics in a diachronic corpus. Poster presented at the Cambridge Language Sciences Symposium, University of Cambridge, UK, 21 November. http://www.academia.edu/35214652/Agreement_in_Louisiana_Creole_Quantitative_sociolinguistics_in_a_diachronic_corpus (accessed 10 January 2024).Suche in Google Scholar

Mayeux, Oliver. 2019. Rethinking decreolization: Language contact and change in Louisiana Creole. Cambridge: University of Cambridge PhD thesis.Suche in Google Scholar

Mayeux, Oliver. 2024. Racial segregation and language variation in Louisiana Creole: Social meaning in language loss. Sociolinguistica 38(1). 27–52. https://doi.org/10.1515/soci-2023-0006.Suche in Google Scholar

Morgan, RaleighJr. 1959. Structural sketch of Saint Martin Creole. Anthropological Linguistics 1(8). 20–24.Suche in Google Scholar

Morgan, RaleighJr. 1970. Dialect leveling in non-English speech of southwest Louisiana. In Glenn G. Gilbert (ed.), Texas studies in bilingualism, 50–62. Berlin: De Gruyter.10.1515/9783110845297.50Suche in Google Scholar

Mougeon, Raymond & Edouard Beniak. 1991. Linguistic consequences of language contact and restriction: The case of French in Ontario, Canada. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oso/9780198248279.001.0001Suche in Google Scholar

Neumann, Ingrid. 1983. Le créole des Blancs en Louisiane. Études Créoles 6(1–2). 63–78.Suche in Google Scholar

Neumann, Ingrid. 1985. Le créole de Breaux Bridge, Louisiane: Étude morphosyntaxique, textes, vocabulaire. Hamburg: Helmut Buske.Suche in Google Scholar

Oetting, Janna B. & April Wimberly Garrity. 2006. Variation within dialects: A case of Cajun/Creole influence within child SAAE and SWE. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 49(1). 16–26. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/002).Suche in Google Scholar

Picone, Michael D. 2003. Anglophone slaves in francophone Louisiana. American Speech 78(4). 404–433. https://doi.org/10.1215/00031283-78-4-404.Suche in Google Scholar

Rickford, John R. 1975. Carrying the new wave into syntax: The case of Black English BÍN. In Ralph W. Fasold & R. Shuy (eds.), Analyzing variation in language, 162–183. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Rizzi, Luigi & Guglielmo Cinque. 2016. Functional categories and syntactic theory. Annual Review of Linguistics 2(1). 139–163. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011415-040827.Suche in Google Scholar

Rottet, Kevin J. 1992. Functional categories and verbal movement in Louisiana Creole. Probus 4(3). 261–289.10.1515/prbs.1992.4.3.261Suche in Google Scholar

Siegel, Jason F. 2010. Decreolization: A critical review. Indiana Working Papers in Linguistics 9. 83–98.Suche in Google Scholar

Snow, Peter. 2000. Language variation in Caribbean Creole/non-lexifier contact situations: Continua or diglossia? In Proceedings from the Eight Annual Symposium about Language and Society, 148–162. Austin, TX: University of Texas.Suche in Google Scholar

Spears, Arthur K. 1982. The Black English semi-auxiliary come. Language 58(4). 850–372. https://doi.org/10.2307/413960.Suche in Google Scholar

Syea, Anand. 2017. French Creoles: A comprehensive and comparative grammar (Routledge Comprehensive Grammars). London: Routledge.10.4324/9781315388588Suche in Google Scholar

Tentchoff, Dorice. 1977. Speech in a Louisiana Cajun community. Cleveland, OH: Case Western Reserve University PhD thesis.Suche in Google Scholar

Thomason, Sarah Grey & Terence Kaufman. 1988. Language contact, creolization and genetic linguistics. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.10.1525/9780520912793Suche in Google Scholar

Valdman, Albert. 1997. French and Creole in Louisiana. New York: Plenum Press.10.1007/978-1-4757-5278-6Suche in Google Scholar

Valdman, Albert, Thomas A. Klingler, Margaret M. Marshall & Kevin J. Rottet. 1998. Dictionary of Louisiana Creole. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Weinreich, Uriel. 1953. Languages in contact. New York, NY: Linguistic Circle of New York.Suche in Google Scholar

Wendte, Nathan A. 2020. “Creole” – a Louisiana label in a Texas context. New Orleans, LA: Lulu.Suche in Google Scholar

Whinnom, Keith. 1971. Linguistic hybridization and the “special case” of pidgins and creoles. In Dell Hymes (ed.), Pidginization and creolization of languages, 91–115. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Yakpo, Kofi. 2021. Two types of language contact involving English Creoles: Why Krio (Sierra Leone) has evolved more towards English than its relative Pichi (Equatorial Guinea) towards Spanish. English Today 39(1). 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0266078421000146.Suche in Google Scholar

Received: 2023-10-04
Accepted: 2024-02-27
Published Online: 2024-06-25

© 2024 the author(s), published by De Gruyter, Berlin/Boston

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Artikel in diesem Heft

  1. Frontmatter
  2. Editorial
  3. Editorial 2024
  4. Phonetics & Phonology
  5. The role of recoverability in the implementation of non-phonemic glottalization in Hawaiian
  6. Epenthetic vowel quality crosslinguistically, with focus on Modern Hebrew
  7. Japanese speakers can infer specific sub-lexicons using phonotactic cues
  8. Articulatory phonetics in the market: combining public engagement with ultrasound data collection
  9. Investigating the acoustic fidelity of vowels across remote recording methods
  10. The role of coarticulatory tonal information in Cantonese spoken word recognition: an eye-tracking study
  11. Tracking phonological regularities: exploring the influence of learning mode and regularity locus in adult phonological learning
  12. Morphology & Syntax
  13. #AreHashtagsWords? Structure, position, and syntactic integration of hashtags in (English) tweets
  14. The meaning of morphomes: distributional semantics of Spanish stem alternations
  15. A refinement of the analysis of the resultative V-de construction in Mandarin Chinese
  16. L2 cognitive construal and morphosyntactic acquisition of pseudo-passive constructions
  17. Semantics & Pragmatics
  18. “All women are like that”: an overview of linguistic deindividualization and dehumanization of women in the incelosphere
  19. Counterfactual language, emotion, and perspective: a sentence completion study during the COVID-19 pandemic
  20. Constructing elderly patients’ agency through conversational storytelling
  21. Language Documentation & Typology
  22. Conative animal calls in Macha Oromo: function and form
  23. The syntax of African American English borrowings in the Louisiana Creole tense-mood-aspect system
  24. Syntactic pausing? Re-examining the associations
  25. Bibliographic bias and information-density sampling
  26. Historical & Comparative Linguistics
  27. Revisiting the hypothesis of ideophones as windows to language evolution
  28. Verifying the morpho-semantics of aspect via typological homogeneity
  29. Psycholinguistics & Neurolinguistics
  30. Sign recognition: the effect of parameters and features in sign mispronunciations
  31. Influence of translation on perceived metaphor features: quality, aptness, metaphoricity, and familiarity
  32. Effects of grammatical gender on gender inferences: Evidence from French hybrid nouns
  33. Processing reflexives in adjunct control: an exploration of attraction effects
  34. Language Acquisition & Language Learning
  35. How do L1 glosses affect EFL learners’ reading comprehension performance? An eye-tracking study
  36. Modeling L2 motivation change and its predictive effects on learning behaviors in the extramural digital context: a quantitative investigation in China
  37. Ongoing exposure to an ambient language continues to build implicit knowledge across the lifespan
  38. On the relationship between complexity of primary occupation and L2 varietal behavior in adult migrants in Austria
  39. The acquisition of speaking fundamental frequency (F0) features in Cantonese and English by simultaneous bilingual children
  40. Sociolinguistics & Anthropological Linguistics
  41. A computational approach to detecting the envelope of variation
  42. Attitudes toward code-switching among bilingual Jordanians: a comparative study
  43. “Let’s ride this out together”: unpacking multilingual top-down and bottom-up pandemic communication evidenced in Singapore’s coronavirus-related linguistic and semiotic landscape
  44. Across time, space, and genres: measuring probabilistic grammar distances between varieties of Mandarin
  45. Navigating linguistic ideologies and market dynamics within China’s English language teaching landscape
  46. Streetscapes and memories of real socialist anti-fascism in south-eastern Europe: between dystopianism and utopianism
  47. What can NLP do for linguistics? Towards using grammatical error analysis to document non-standard English features
  48. From sociolinguistic perception to strategic action in the study of social meaning
  49. Minority genders in quantitative survey research: a data-driven approach to clear, inclusive, and accurate gender questions
  50. Variation is the way to perfection: imperfect rhyming in Chinese hip hop
  51. Shifts in digital media usage before and after the pandemic by Rusyns in Ukraine
  52. Computational & Corpus Linguistics
  53. Revisiting the automatic prediction of lexical errors in Mandarin
  54. Finding continuers in Swedish Sign Language
  55. Conversational priming in repetitional responses as a mechanism in language change: evidence from agent-based modelling
  56. Construction grammar and procedural semantics for human-interpretable grounded language processing
  57. Through the compression glass: language complexity and the linguistic structure of compressed strings
  58. Could this be next for corpus linguistics? Methods of semi-automatic data annotation with contextualized word embeddings
  59. The Red Hen Audio Tagger
  60. Code-switching in computer-mediated communication by Gen Z Japanese Americans
  61. Supervised prediction of production patterns using machine learning algorithms
  62. Introducing Bed Word: a new automated speech recognition tool for sociolinguistic interview transcription
  63. Decoding French equivalents of the English present perfect: evidence from parallel corpora of parliamentary documents
  64. Enhancing automated essay scoring with GCNs and multi-level features for robust multidimensional assessments
  65. Sociolinguistic auto-coding has fairness problems too: measuring and mitigating bias
  66. The role of syntax in hashtag popularity
  67. Language practices of Chinese doctoral students studying abroad on social media: a translanguaging perspective
  68. Cognitive Linguistics
  69. Metaphor and gender: are words associated with source domains perceived in a gendered way?
  70. Crossmodal correspondence between lexical tones and visual motions: a forced-choice mapping task on Mandarin Chinese
Heruntergeladen am 8.9.2025 von https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/lingvan-2023-0148/html
Button zum nach oben scrollen