Abstract
This paper proposes the principle of weakest epenthesis, whereby the quality of the epenthetic vowel is that of the weakest lexical vowel of the language, and weakness is defined phonologically, not phonetically. This principle is first shown to motivate the emergence of [e] as the epenthetic vowel of Modern Hebrew. It is then corroborated by a crosslinguistic survey of 100 languages. Finally, the Modern Hebrew facts are used to illustrate the formalization of this principle using gradient symbolic representations.
Acknowledgments
The author would like to thank the following people for their help: Outi Bat-El, Si Berrebi, Joaquim Brandão de Carvalho, and Vered Silber-Varod and Il-il Yatziv-Malibert, as well as the participants of the Atelier de Phonologie of the CNRS lab “Structures Formelles du Langage” and three anonymous reviewers. Remaining errors and omissions should not be attributed to any of these individuals, but to the author.
Appendix: A crosslinguistic survey of the correlation between vocalic weakness and epenthetic quality
The language survey is based on the list of 100 languages in the WALS online site (https://wals.info/languoid/samples/100). The following substitutions were made (in all cases but one within the same language family), in order to bolster the results: German → Yiddish; Hindi → Domari; Imonda → Kilmeri; Kewa → Komnzo; Kiowa → Kiowa/Tiwa; Koasati → Alabama; Krongo → Lumun; Lango → Lupit; Luvale → Shona; Mangarayi → Enindhilyakwa; Rema → Teribe; Tiwi → Wardaman; Wichita → Caddo; Yaqui → Seri; Nama → Khoekhoegowab; Ngiyambaa → Kisêdjê (not related).
Languages with a weak lexical vowel that is used for epenthesis
Language | Pattern | Source | |
---|---|---|---|
1. | Abkhaz | Two vowel phonemes, /a,ə/. The phone [ə] is used to split inconvenient consonant clusters. Of the two vowels /a,ə/, the latter is clearly weaker. | Chirikba (2003) |
2. | Alamblak | /i,e,a,u,o,ɨ,ə/. Epenthetic [ɨ]. /ɨ/ undergoes full assimilation to some vowels. | Bruce (1984) |
3. | Alabama | /i,a,u/. [i] is used to derive CV-final words (other cases of epenthesis are copy vowels). /i/ is also weak in other senses. | Montler and Hardy (1991) |
4. | Arabic (Palestinian) | /i,u,a,iː,uː,aː,eː,oː/. Epenthetic [i]. Lexical /i/ syncopates in unstressed open syllables. | Personal knowledge |
5. | Basque | /i,u,a,e,o/. /e/ is a phoneme, [e] is epenthetic, [e] undergoes syncope. | Hualde and Ortiz de Urbina (2003) |
6. | Cree | At least /i,a,u/+length. According to Wolfart (1973), /i/ is syncopated in normal speech. And according to Russell (1992), [i] is the epenthetic vowel. | Wolfart (1973), Russell (1992) |
7. | Dakelh | /i,e,o,u,a,ʌ (ə?)/. The epenthetic vowel is [ə/ʌ]. It is the only vowel not to appear in open syllables in verb stems. It does not seem to be possible to reduce all [ə] to epenthesis. | Gessner (2003) |
8. | Domari | /i,u,a,e,o/. [i] epenthesis. /i/ also syncopates in some common verbs. | Matras (2012) |
9. | English | /ə/ (for instance, [ə]loof) is also the epenthetic vowel [kənɪʃ] (Yiddish [kniʃ] ‘sort of dumpling’), and also alternates with zero [k(ə)ríjə] Korea. | Personal knowledge |
10. | Enindhilyakwa | /i,ɛ,a,ə/. Also, “lexical schwa optionally harmonizes to the epenthetic vowel in the next syllable, either by i-umlaut or by regressive vowel harmony” (Van Egmond 2012: 69). Epenthetic [ə] is ignored by stress, lexical /ə/ isn’t. | Van Egmond (2012) |
11. | Fijian | /i,u,a,e,o/+length+diphthongs. Copy epenthesis, but word-finally [i]. Short vowels /i,u/ can be syncopated in unstressed syllables (the consonant lengthens, in the case of /u/ it is homorganic) → for this there are only a few examples in Schütz (1985). | Schütz (1985) |
12. | French | /i e ɛ y œ ø ɔ a ã õ œ̃ ɛ̃/. [œ/ø] is epenthetic. /ø,œ/ are also phonemes. Some [ø]s alternate with Ø [ʃøval, ʃval] ‘horse’. | Personal knowledge |
13. | Gooniyandi | /a,aa,oo,i/. Epenthesis is [i]. Some evidence for weakness of [i]: it is statistically much more common word-finally, and gets lost when a V-initial suffix is added. i → oo/_Coo. | McGregor (1990) |
14. | Greek | / i,u,a,e,o/. Either [e] or [i] is epenthetic (not very clear). In northern dialects, while /e,o/ shift to [i,u] the latter delete in unstressed position. | Markopoulos (2018) |
15. | Hare Slave | /i,u,a,e,o+je/ in Hare. Epenthetic vowel is [e]. A suffix [-e] is deleted upon contact with another vowel. In fact, all kinds of [e] are deleted in the morphology. | Rice (1989) |
16. | Hebrew |
![]() |
Personal knowledge |
17. | Kilmeri | /i,ɪ,ɛ,æ,a,u,ʊ,ɔ/. [ɛ] and [i] undergo lowering (to [a] and [ɛ]) before certain codas. [i] undergoes lexically specific syncope in 2-sided open syllables. [ɛ] undergoes assimilation to following [ɔ]. Some prefixes are analyzed as /C-/, in which case their vowel =V1 unless it is [a], in which case the prefixed vowel is [ɛ]. The epenthetic vowel is probably [ɛ]. | Gerstner-Link (2018) |
18. | Japanese | /i,u,a,e,o/+length. Mostly [u], though sometimes [o] and [i]. All three correspond to phonemes. /u/ and /i/ undergo devoicing. | Labrune (2012) |
19. | Korean | /i,e,ɛ,u,o,ɔ,a,ɨ/. Epenthetic [ɨ], /ɨ/ is weak. | Rhee (2002) |
20. | Koyra Chiini | /i,u,a,e,o/ + length. Hard to tell. French consonant-final loans are adapted with a final high vowel (either i or u). Short vowels are deleted in suffixed CViCViC, [i] is marginally deleted elsewhere. | Heath (2011) |
21. | Lumun | /i,ɪ,u,ʊ,ɛ,ɔ,a,ə/. /ə/ is definitely lexical, definitely the weakest lexical vowel, and definitely the epenthetic vowel. | Smits (2017) |
22. | Lakota | /i,u,a,e,o+ã,ĩ,ũ/. [a] is the epenthetic vowel. It also undergoes deletion in prefixes. | Carter (1974) |
23. | Malagasy (Betsimisrak) | /a,i,e,u/. Epenthetic [a]. O’Neill speaks both of epenthetic [a] (only final) and sometimes of harmonic epenthesis (in same position). Also, “in all but the most formal, careful speech, though, is /a/ susceptible to devoicing, manifesting sometimes in as little as a release of the previous stop (Dziwirek 1989; O’Neill 2015: 70) | O’Neill (2015), Dziwirek (1989) |
24. | Meithei/ Manipuri | According to author, /i,a,u,o,e,ə/. /ə/ tends to disappear when syllabically unnecessary, though. Consonant clusters of the form rC are broken up by the insertion of schwa between the two consonants. If the onset of a syllable is a liquid or a nasal and the vowel of the preceding syllable is a [ə], then that [ə] may delete. In fast speech, [ə] deletes before nasals. Only a vowel can form the nucleus of a Manipuri syllable; in borrowed words [ə] is inserted as the nucleus for words with syllabic nasals or liquids: [filəm] ‘film’, [tebəl] ‘table’ (though possibly there was an intermediate language that already inserted the schwa). | Chelliah (1997) |
25. | Oneida | /i,e,a,o,ʌ̃,ũ/. Looks like [e] is the epenthetic vowel according to Abbott (2000), as well as prosthetic [i] in Michelson (1988). [i] is also deleted before other vowels. | Abbott (2000), Michelson (1988) |
26. | Oromo | /i,u,a,e,o+length/. [i] is the epenthetic vowel, and [i] is the weakest vowel (completely deletes word-finally). | Lloret (1997) |
27. | Paiwan | Looks like /a,i,u,ə/. [ə] is epenthetic. /ə/ is the weakest vowel: stressed only in very extreme conditions. | Ferrell (1982) |
28. | Persian | /i,a,ɑ,u,o,e/. Copy epenthesis except before [a,ɑ] where there is [e] epenthesis. The lexical vowel /e/ is syncopated in several cases, and deleted if first in inter-morphemic hiatus. | Alireza Jaferian (pers. comm.), Rahbar (2012) |
29. | Teribe | /i,e,a,u,o,ɑ+nasality+ɪ,ʊ(no nasal counterparts)/. Epenthesis is [ə]. /a/ in unstressed syllables is also [ə]. | Oakes (2001) |
30. | Seri | /a,e,i,o+length/. Epenthesis is most commonly [i] (though there is also [a]-epenthesis). There is also a rule of /i/ deletion. | Marlett and Stemberger (1983) |
31. | Spanish | /i,e,a,u,o/. Epenthesis is [e]. [e]-deletion in some paradigms. | Personal Knowledge |
32. | Swahili | /i,a,e,o,u/. Epenthesis in loanword adaptation is sometimes copy, but not at word edges, where it is [i] (and [u] after labials). Lexical [i] and [u] are optionally deleted in some prefixes. | Mwita (2009) |
33. | Turkish | /i,a,e,o,u,y,ø,ɨ/. [ɨ] is epenthetic and lexical /ɨ/ is the weakest vowel. | Markus Pöchtrager (p.c.) |
34. | Yagua | /i,u,a,e,o/. Epenthesis of [i] between [r] and [j]. Deletion of [i] between two voiceless C (+ another deletion that is described for all vowels but illustrated only for /i/ and /u/) | Payne (1985) |
35. | Yiddish | /i,u,a,e,o,ɨ/. /ɨ/ must be lexical in words like [χánɨke]. Never stressed, hence weak. Also used as epenthetic in /motl+n/ → [motlɨn]. | Personal knowledge |
36. | Yoruba | Apparently /i,a,e,o,u,ɛ,ɔ + 5 nasals/. Epenthesis in loans (cluster+final) is usually [i] ([u] after labials). Occasionally [i] or [u] through copy. [i] is also sometimes elided word-initially. | Fresco (1970) |
37. | Zoque | /i,u,a,e,o,ɨ/. [ɨ]-epenthesis. /ɨ/ undergoes syncope and harmonizes with [a]. | Knudson (1975) |
38. | Zulu | /i,u,a,e,o/. Epenthetic [i]. Also used in loans, alongside copy. There is a vowel deletion rule that targets /i/. | Cook (2013) |
Languages with a lexical vowel that is used for epenthesis, where weakness cannot be established
Language | Pattern | Source | |
---|---|---|---|
1. | Caddo | /a,i,u/. Probably [i] is sometimes epenthetic (one case cited). No weakness of /i/ (all short vowels delete). | Melnar (1998) |
2. | Canela-Krahô | /i,e,ɛ,u,o,ɔ,a/ + nasal contrast (neutralization in mid vowels) + rounding contrast for back vowels except /a/. Final consonants are released with the quality of preceding vowel, except when it’s an /a/, in which case there’s an [i]. But no evidence of weakness. | Popjes and Popjes (1986) |
3. | Finnish | /i,e,æ,y,ø,u,o,a/. Not so clear, but probably [i] is epenthetic (though morphologically conditioned). However, weakness of /i/ cannot be clearly established. | Karlsson (1999) |
4. | Georgian | /i,u,a,e,o/. Probably /a/ is epenthetic (more common than /i/ in augmenting words to disyllabicity). /a/ (but also /e/ and /o/) syncopates in nominal stems: V[-high] → Ø /_C[+son]-VC. | Butskhrikidze (2002) |
5. | Grebo | /i,em,u,om,e,o,a,ɛ,ɔ/ (m= “muffled”) + nasality for non-muffled vowels. An underspecified non-low vowel in harmony with following vowel. No data for weakness. | Innes (1966) |
6. | Guaraní (Tapiete Guaraní) | /i,a,u,o,e,ɨ/. Often copy, but not of [a]. When no copy is possible, [i,ɨ,e] (all analyzed as phonemic; only the latter word-finally). No sign of weakness of these non-low vowels. | González (2005) |
7. | Hausa | /i,u,a,e,o/+length. Copy, but word-finally mostly [i]. Nothing indicates weakness of /i/. | Newman (2000) |
8. | Indonesian | /i,u,a,e,o/, copy epenthesis, but unmotivated /u/ word-finally. No sign of weakness of /u/. | Batais and Wiltshire (2017) |
9. | Kannada | /i,u,a,e,o/+length. [u] is inserted at the edge of words before pause and consonant-initial suffixes ([i] after [j]). All short vowels delete in colloquial speech. Some evidence that [u] is weak: [avu] → [aː], ija → [eː]. | Bright (1970) |
10. | Kisêdjê | /i,e,a,u,o,ɛ,ɔ,ɨ,ə,ʌ/+nasality (doesn’t combine with high mid and a). Epenthetic vowel is [ɨ] when it is not a copy. Not clear whether it is the weakest. | Beauchamp (2019) |
11. | Shona | /a,i,e,o,u/. Generalization: after labial Cs, /u/ is inserted; after coronal Cs, /i/ is inserted; after dorsal Cs, the previous V is copied if it is /i,o,u/; after /e,a/, however, /i/ is epenthesized. Caveat: after stops, CV-assimilation is dispreferred; instead, we find V-copy if the previous V is /i/ or /u/. | Uffman (2002) |
12. | Mandarin | Possibly the inventory is /i,y,u,ə,a/. Luo reports about loanwords: [i,ɤ,u] depending on preceding C. Natively, no lexical vowel shows more weakness than any other. | Xiaoliang Luo (pers. comm.) |
13. | Mapuche | /i,a,u,o,e,ə/. Epenthesis is [ə]. [ə] is the only vowel that isn’t a suffix. No other evidence for weakness of [ə]. | Moulineaux (2018) |
14. | Otomí | /i,e,ɛ,a,u,o,ɔ,ɨ,ə/, Epenthesis is [ɨ]. No evidence for weakness. | Palancar (2009) |
15. | Pirahã | /i,a,u/. Epenthesis is either [i] or [a], under very strange conditions; no information as to weakness. | Everett (1986) |
16. | Rapanui | Strict CV language, /a,i,u,e,o/. No epenthesis in native words. “Final consonants are not allowed. This is resolved by adding a final vowel, which is either e or identical to the previous vowel” (Kieviet 2017: 59). No data points to weakness. | Kieviet (2017) |
17. | Tagalog | /i,u,a,e,o/, [i] epenthesis before word initial sC. #Cj an #Cw broken by i and u. No real proof found that high vowels are weaker. | Schachter and Otanes (1972) |
Languages for which the source does not point out to an epenthetic vowel
Language | Pattern | Source | |
---|---|---|---|
1. | Apuriña | /i,u,a,e,o,ɨ,ə/+length+nasalization for all vowels. /ɨ/ deletes before a vowel; /ɨ/→[i] after palatal, /ɨ/ is absent from diphthongs. But no information about epenthetic vowels. | Da Silva Facundes (2000) |
2. | Acoma-Keresan | /i,e,ɨ,u,a/+voicing contrast. No data regarding epenthesis/weakness in source. | Maring (1967) |
3. | Asmat | /i,u,a,e,o/. | Voorhoeve (1980) |
4. | Baguirmi | Unreliable data. | Gaden (1909) |
5. | Barasano | Insufficient data. | |
6. | Burmese | /i,u,a,e,o/. No data on epenthesis or weakness. | Soe (1999) |
7. | Burushaski | /i,u,a,e,o/+length. High vowel syncope occurs occasionally. No data about epenthesis. | Anderson (1997) |
8. | Chamorro | /u,e,i,o,a,æ/. No data on epenthesis or weakness. | Topping (1973) |
9. | Daga | /i,u,a,e,o/. No data on epenthesis or weakness. | Murane (1974) |
10. | Dani | /i,u,a,e,o/. No data on epenthesis or weakness. | Bromley (1961) |
11. | Hmong | /i,a,u,o,e,õ,ẽ/. Epenthesis is probably [ə], but in fact hard to tell. Mostly C-deletion in loanwords. | Golston and Yang (2001) |
12. | Jacaltec | /i,a,u/, among which /i/ most frequently undergoes syncope. But no evidence for epenthesis. | Craig (1977) |
13. | Kiowa/Tiwa | /i,e,a,u,o,ɔ/. There is possibly [e] epenthesis in Tiwa, but /e/ does not seem to be a lexical vowel in this language. If anything [ɔ] is the weakest vowel (gets deleted occasionally). | Sutton (2014) |
14. | Kutenai | /i,u,a/. Little evidence of weakness or of which vowel is epenthetic. | Morgan (1991) |
15. | Lavukaleve | /i,u,a,e,o/. Not enough data. [i] or [e] as epenthesis alongside copy. | Terrill (2003) |
16. | Lezgian | /i,e,a,æ,y,u/. “Loss of pretonic high vowels after voiceless obstruents” (Haspelmath 1993: 36); “epenthetic [i]” reported by Scheffler (2005) | Haspelmath (1993) |
17. | Martuthunira | /i,a,u/+length. No data about epenthesis (“epenthetic syllable pa”). Mention of epenthetic [u] but in one very specific rounding-inducing environment. No vowel weakness. | Dench (1987) |
18. | Maung | /i,u,a,ɛ,ɔ/. No evidence of epenthesis. | Capell and Hinch (1970) |
19. | Mixtec | /i,ɨ,a,u,o,e/. Possibly copy. Some loanwords add [i] or [a] at the right edge, possibly depending on the stem vowel; but there is insufficient data. | Macaulay (1996) |
20. | Quechua | /i,a,u/. No data. | Cole (1982) |
21. | Russian | /i,e,u,o,a,ɨ/. No data for epenthetic vowel. | Personal knowledge |
22. | Sanuma | /i,ɨ,e,ə,a,u/+nasal set (somewhat reduced). No information regarding epenthesis. | Borgman (1990) |
23. | Thai | /i,ɯ,u,e,ɤ,o,ɛ,a,ɔ/. In some English loanwords one finds [a] breaking up impermissible clusters, but this accommodation looks like more than just a pronunciation thing. Another paper says that /thr/ is sometimes pronounced [thər]. | Nacaskul (1979) |
24. | Wardaman | No data found in these two grammars. | Merlan (1994), Cook (1987) |
25. | Tukang Besi | No data. | Donohue (1999) |
26. | Vietnamese | Looks like /i,e,ɛ,u,o,ɔ,ɨ,ə/+complications. In the adaptation of French consonant clusters into Vietnamese, epenthesis applies far less frequently than deletion, attested only in onset clusters. Three epenthetic vowels are attested in the corpus, [a,i,ɤ]; of these, [ɤ] has the highest frequency. It seems possible that the place of articulation of the preceding consonant is one of the factors that determine the choice of the low, high, or mid vowel. Additional data is needed to confirm this hypothesis. |
Nguyen and Dutta (2017) |
27. | Wari | /i,a,e,o,y,ø/. No real information about epenthesis. | Everett and Kern (1997) |
Languages with a non-lexical epenthetic vowel
Language | Pattern | Source | |
---|---|---|---|
1. | Berber (Tamazight) | /i,a,u/. Non-lexical epenthesis [ɨ]. | Abdel-Massih (1971) |
2. | Chukchi | /i,e,u/. Non-lexical epenthesis [ə]. | Dunn (1999) |
3. | Hixkaryana | /i,a,u,o,e/. /i/ and /u/ regularly delete word-finally. Epenthetic vowel described as non-lexical [ə] or [ɪ]. | Derbyshire (1979) |
4. | Komnzo | /i,a,e,u,ʊ,ɔ/. Non-lexical epenthetic [ə] ([i] and [u] around [j] and [w]). | Döhler (2018) |
5. | Khalakh Mongolian | /i,y,a,æ,e,œo,u/. Non-lexical epenthetic [ə], influenced by environment (sometimes harmonizes with preceding vowel). | A. M. Karlsson (2005) |
6. | Maricopa | /i,u,a,e,o/. Epenthetic [ə] ([ɪ/i] after [j], [u] after labialized C). No evidence for weakness of high vowels. | Gordon (1986) |
7. | Maybrat | According to author, /i,u,a,e,o,ə/. Epenthetic [ə] (very widespread). But the phoneme /ə/ only occurs in a handful of words word-initially or preceded by [ʔ], and even in these cases it is optional. I would conclude that there is non-lexical epenthesis. | Dol (1999) |
Languages with copy epenthesis
Language | Pattern | Source | |
---|---|---|---|
1. | Amele | /i,u,a,e,o/. Copy epenthesis. | Roberts (2016) |
2. | Arapesh | /i,e,a,æ,u,o,ɨ,ə/. /ɨ/ undergoes syncope. But copy epenthesis. | Conrad and Wogiga (1991) |
3. | West Greenlandic | /i,a,u/. Copy epenthesis in loans. Names are Greenlandized by adding a final [i]. No evidence of weakness. | Fortescue (1984) |
4. | Karuk | /i,u,a,iː,uː,aː,eː,oː/. Epenthesis is a copy vowel, but it must be short so it can’t be [e] or [o]. Short vowels are deleted word-finally. | Sandy (2017) |
5. | Kayardild | /a,i,u/. Copy epenthesis (found in loans in Evans’s list). Short vowels, and high ones are susceptible to reduction. | Evans (1995) |
6. | Khoekhoegowab | /i,u,a,e,o/. In loans, copy of adjacent vowel. | Haacke (2015) |
7. | Lupit | /a,i,ɪ,e,ɛ,o,ɔ,u,ʊ/. Copy (only in some loanwords). | Billington (2017) |
8. | Makah | /i,u,a,e,o/. Copy epenthesis. | Davidson (2002) |
9. | Sango | /i,e,ɛ,a,u,o,ɔ/. For epenthesis, not a lot of evidence. It seems that there is copy epenthesis in French loans. | Samarin (1967) |
10. | Supyire | /a,e,i,o,u,ɛ,ɔ/+nasals (no õ,ẽ). Vowel elision only affects /i/. Many CV suffixes where V is a complete copy of preceding vowel. /i/→[u]/_labial. Copy epenthesis seems to be the rule in loans (though not many examples). | Carlson (1994) |
11. | Warao | CVCV language, /i,u,a,e,o/. Copy epenthesis (in loans), with occasional unmotivated [i]. | Romero-Figueroa (1997) |
References
Asherov, Daniel & Evan Cohen. 2019. A phonetic description of Modern Hebrew consonants and vowels. Brill’s Journal of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 11. 3–27. https://doi.org/10.1163/18776930-01101002.Search in Google Scholar
Bat-El, Outi. 2008. Morphologically conditioned V–Ø alternation in Hebrew: Distinction among nouns, adjectives and participles, and verbs. In Sharon Armon-Lotem, Gabi Danon & Susan Rothstein (eds.), Generative approaches to Hebrew linguistics, 27–60. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/la.134.02morSearch in Google Scholar
Benua, Laura. 1997. Transderivational identity: Phonological relations between words. Amherst: University of Massachusetts Amherst doctoral dissertation. (Available on Rutgers Optimality Archive, ROA-259. Published in 2000 as Phonological relations between words, New York: Garland.).Search in Google Scholar
Berrebi, Si, Noa Bassel & Roey J. Gafter. 2022. Hearing Hebrew pharyngeals: Experimental evidence for a covert phonemic distinction. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 28(2). 12–20.Search in Google Scholar
Bolozky, Shmuel. 1991. Casual and formal vowel deletion in Modern Hebrew. In Alan S. Kaye (ed.), Semitic studies in honor of Wolf Leslau, vol. 1, 189–198. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz.Search in Google Scholar
Bolozky, Shmuel. 2009. Colloquial Hebrew imperatives revisited. Language Sciences 31. 136–143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2008.12.008.Search in Google Scholar
Bolozky, Shmuel. 2019. The phonology of connected speech in Israeli Hebrew. Brill’s Journal of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 11(1). 201–225. https://doi.org/10.1163/18776930-01101013.Search in Google Scholar
Boula de Mareüil, Philippe & Martine Adda-Decker. 2002. Studying pronunciation variants in French by using alignment techniques. In John H. L. Hansen & Bryan L. Pellom (eds.), Proceedings of the International Conference on Spoken Language Processing (ICSLP), Denver, 2273–2276. Denver: International Speech Communication Association.10.21437/ICSLP.2002-622Search in Google Scholar
Bradley, Travis G. 2009. On the syllabification of prevocalic /w/ in Judeo-Spanish. In Pascual J. Masullo, Erin O’Rourke & Chia-Hui Huang (eds.), Romance Linguistics 2007: Selected papers from the 37th Linguistic Symposium on Romance Languages, 51–67. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/cilt.304.05braSearch in Google Scholar
Bürki, Audrey, Myriam Ernestus, Cédric Gendrot, Cécile Fougeron & Ulrich Frauenfelder. 2011. What affects the presence versus absence of schwa and its duration: A corpus analysis of French connected speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 130. 3980–3991. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3658386.Search in Google Scholar
Cooper, Robert. 1990. Language planning and social change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511620812Search in Google Scholar
Côté, Marie-Hélène & Geoffrey S. Morrison. 2004. Experimental evidence and the nature of the schwa/zero alternation in French. Abstract from the 9th Conference on Laboratory Phonology. Available at: http://www.linguistics.uiuc.edu/labphon9/AbstractPDF/cote.pdf.Search in Google Scholar
Côté, Marie-Hélène & Geoffrey S. Morrison. 2007. The nature of the schwa/zero alternation in French clitics: Experimental and non-experimental evidence. Journal of French Language Studies 17(2). 159–186. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0959269507002827.Search in Google Scholar
Dupoux, Emannuel, Erika Parlato, Sonia Frota, Yuki Hirose & Sharon Peperkamp. 2011. Where do illusory vowels come from? Journal of Memory and Language 64(3). 199–210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.12.004.Search in Google Scholar
Faust, Noam. 2019. Vowel alternation in Modern Hebrew. Brill’s Journal of Afro-Asiatic Language and Linguistics 11(1). 119–134. https://doi.org/10.1163/18776930-01101009.Search in Google Scholar
Guevara-Rukoz, Adriana, Erika Parlato-Oliveira, Shi Yu, Yuki Hirose, Sharon Peperkamp & Emannuel Dupoux. 2017. Predicting epenthetic vowel quality from acoustics. Proceedings of Interspeech 2017. 596–600. https://doi.org/10.21437/Interspeech.2017-1735.Search in Google Scholar
Hall, Nancy. 2017. Phonetic neutralization in Palestinian Arabic vowel shortening, with implications for lexical organization. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 2(1). 1–23. https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.257.Search in Google Scholar
Harmati, Shlomo. 2000a. Hebrew – a spoken language [in Hebrew]. Israel: Modan.Search in Google Scholar
Harmati, Shlomo. 2000b. The pioneer teachers in Eretz Israel [in Hebrew]. Israel: The Ministry of Defense.Search in Google Scholar
Harmati, Shlomo. 2003. Jewish teachers in the diaspora [in Hebrew]. Israel: The Ministry of Defense.Search in Google Scholar
Hume, Elisabeth. 2003. Language specific markedness: The case of place of articulation. Studies in Phonetics, Phonology and Morphology 9(2). 295–310.Search in Google Scholar
Jacobs, Neil G. 2005. Yiddish: A linguistic introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Khan, Geoffrey. 2004. The Jewish Neo-Aramaic dialect of Sulemaniyya and Halabja. Boston: Brill.10.1163/9789047413585Search in Google Scholar
Klamer, Marian. 1998. A grammar of Kambera. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110805536Search in Google Scholar
Lee, Juhee. 2008. Salience and typology of epenthetic vowels: The case from loanword adaptation. Linguistic Research 25(1). 83–101. https://doi.org/10.17250/khisli.25.1.200806.005.Search in Google Scholar
Lacy, Paul de. 2006. Markedness: Reduction and preservation in phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Lombardi, Linda. 2003. Markedness and the typology of epenthetic vowels. Rutgers Optimality Archive 578. https://roa.rutgers.edu/article/view/588 (accessed 18 April 2024).Search in Google Scholar
Morag, Shelomo. 2007. Pronunciations of Hebrew. In Encyclopedia Judaica, 2nd edn., vol. 16, 547–562. New York: Thomson Gale.Search in Google Scholar
Morley, Rebecca L. 2015. Deletion or epenthesis? On the falsifiability of phonological universals. Lingua 154. 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2014.11.002.Search in Google Scholar
Ofer, Yosef. 2008. The beginning of the Israeli accent [in Hebrew]. In Aaron Maman, Samuel Fassberg & Yohanan Broyer (eds.), Shearey lashon: Studies in Aramaic and in the languages of the Jews offered to Moshe Bar-Asher, vol. 3, 166–172. Jerusalem: Bialik Institute.Search in Google Scholar
Rabin, Chaim. 1973. A short history of the Hebrew Language. Jerusalem: The Jewish Agency.Search in Google Scholar
Rabin, Chaim. 1999. What was the revival of the Hebrew language? [in Hebrew]. In Chaim Rabin (ed.), Linguistic Studies: Collected papers in Hebrew and Semitic languages, 359–376. Jerusalem: Academy of the Hebrew Language and the Bialik Institute.Search in Google Scholar
Smolensky, Paul & Matthew Goldrick. 2016. Gradient symbolic representations in grammar: The case of French liaison. Rutgers Optimality Archive 1286. https://roa.rutgers.edu/content/article/files/1552_smolensky_1.pdf (accessed 18 April 2024).Search in Google Scholar
Smolensky, Paul & Géraldine Legendre. 2006. The harmonic mind: From neural computation to optimality-theoretic grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar
Steriade, Donca. 2009. The phonology of perceptibility effects: The P-map and its consequences for constraint organization. In Kristin Hanson & Sharon Inkelas (eds.), The nature of the word: Studies in honor of Paul Kiparsky, 151–179. Massachusetts: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/7894.003.0011Search in Google Scholar
Tzenker, Osnat & Noam Amir. 2017. Acoustic reduction of Hebrew vowels in spontaneous speech [in Hebrew]. In Einat Gonen (ed.), Te’uda XXVII: Studies in Spoken Hebrew, 199–227. Tel Aviv: Tel Aviv University.Search in Google Scholar
Zimmermann, Eva. 2018. Gradient symbolic representations in the output: A case study from Moses Columbian Salishan stress. In Sherry Hucklebridge & Max Nelson (eds.), Proceedings of NELS 48, GLSA, Amherst, 275–284. CreateSpace.Search in Google Scholar
Zimmermann, Eva. 2019. Gradient symbolic representations and the typology of ghost segments. In Katherine Hout, Anna Mai, Adam McCollum, Sharon Rose & Matthew Zaslansky (eds.), Proceedings of AMP 2018, LSA.10.3765/amp.v7i0.4576Search in Google Scholar
References for appendix
Abbott, Clifford. 2000. Oneida (Languages of the World/Materials 301). Munich: Lincom Europa.Search in Google Scholar
Abdel-Massih, Ernest T. 1971. A reference grammar of Tamazight (Middle Atlas Berber). Ann Arbor: Center for Near Eastern and North African Studies, University of Michigan.Search in Google Scholar
Anderson, Gregory D. S. 1997. Burushaski phonology. In Alan S. Kaye (ed.), Phonologies of Asia and Africa 2, 1021–1041. Winona Lake: Eisenbrauns.Search in Google Scholar
Batais, Saleh & Caroline Wiltshire. 2017. Indonesian borrowing as evidence for Harmonic Grammar. Journal of Linguistics 54(2). 231–262. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0022226717000317.Search in Google Scholar
Beauchamps, Jérémie. 2019. Echo and default epenthesis in Kĩsêdjê. In Daniel K. E. Reisinger & Roger Yu-Hsiang Lo (eds.), Proceedings of the Workshop on the Structure and Constituency of Languages of the Americas 23. Vancouver, BC: UBCWPL.Search in Google Scholar
Billington, Rosey. 2017. The phonetics and phonology of the Lopit language. Melbourne: University of Melbourne PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Borgman, Donald M. 1990. Sanuma. In Desmond C. Derbyshire & Geoffrey K. Pullum (eds.), Handbook of Amazonian languages 2, 15–248. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar
Bright, William. 1970. Phonological rules in literary and colloquial Kannada. Journal of the American Oriental Society 90. 140–144. https://doi.org/10.2307/598435.Search in Google Scholar
Bromley, H. Myron. 1961. The phonology of Lower Grand Valley Dani: A comparative structural study of skewed phonemic patterns (Verhandelingen van het Koninklijk Instituut voor Taal, Land en Volkenkunde 34). s-Gravenhage: Martinus Nijhoff.Search in Google Scholar
Bruce, Les. 1984. The Alamblak language of Papua New Guinea (East Sepik) (Pacific Linguistics Series C-81). Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar
Butskhrikidze, Marika. 2002. The consonant phonotactics of Georgian. Utrecht: LOT.Search in Google Scholar
Capell, Arthur & H. E. Hinch. 1970. Maung grammar (Janua Linguarum, Series Practica 98). The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar
Carlson, Robert. 1994. A grammar of Supyire (Mouton Grammar Library 14). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar
Carter, Richard T. 1974. Teton Dakota phonology (University of Manitoba Anthropology Papers 10). Winnipeg: Department of Anthropology, University of Manitoba.Search in Google Scholar
Chelliah, Shobhana Lakshmi. 1997. A grammar of Meithei (Mouton Grammar Library 17). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar
Chirikba, Viacheslav A. 2003. Abkhaz. Munich: Lincom.Search in Google Scholar
Cole, Peter. 1982. Imbabura Quechua (Lingua Descriptive Studies 5). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Search in Google Scholar
Conrad, Robert J. & Kepas Wogiga. 1991. An outline of Bukiyip grammar (Pacific Linguistics Series C-113). Canberra: Australian National University.Search in Google Scholar
Cook, Anthony R. 1987. Wagiman Matyin: A description of the Wakiman language of the Northern Territory. Melbourne: La Trobe University PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Cook, Toni. 2013. Morphological and phonological structure in Zulu reduplication. Pittsburg: University of Pennsylvania PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Craig, Colette Grinevald. 1977. The structure of Jacaltec. Austin: University of Texas Press.Search in Google Scholar
Da Silva Facundes, Sidney. 2000. The language of the Apuriña people of Brazil. Buffalo: University of New York at Buffalo PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Davidson, Matthew. 2002. Studies in southern Wakashan (Nootkan) grammar. Buffalo: University of Buffalo PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Dench, Alan Charles. 1987. Martuthunira: A language of the Pilbara region of Western Australia (Pacific Linguistics Series C-125). Canberra: Australian National University.Search in Google Scholar
Derbyshire, Desmond C. 1979. Hixkaryana (Lingua Descriptive Studies 1). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Search in Google Scholar
Döhler, Christian. 2018. A grammar of Komnzo (Studies in Diversity Linguistics 22). Berlin: Language Science Press.Search in Google Scholar
Dol, Philomena. 1999. A grammar of Maybrat: A language of the Bird’s Head, Irian Jaya, Indonesia. Leiden: Leiden University.Search in Google Scholar
Donohue, Mark. 1999. A grammar of Tukang Besi (Mouton Grammar Library 20). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar
Dunn, Michael. 1999. A grammar of Chukchi. Canberra: Australian National University PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Dziwirek, Katarzyna. 1989. Malagasy phonology and morphology. Linguistic Notes from La Jolla 15. 1–30.Search in Google Scholar
Evans, Nicholas. 1995. A grammar of Kayardild: With historical-comparative notes on Tangkic (Mouton Grammar Library 15). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar
Everett, Daniel L. 1986. Pirahã. In Desmond C. Derbyshire & Geoffrey K. Pullum (eds.), Handbook of Amazonian languages 1, 200–325. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar
Everett, Daniel L. & Barbara Kern. 1997. Wari: The Pacaas Novos language of western Brazil (Descriptive Grammar Series). London: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar
Ferrell, Raleigh. 1982. Paiwan dictionary (Pacific Linguistics Series C-73). Canberra: Australian National University.Search in Google Scholar
Fortescue, Michael. 1984. West Greenlandic (Croom Helm Descriptive Grammars). London: Croom Helm.Search in Google Scholar
Fresco, Edward Max. 1970. Topics in Yorùbá dialect phonology (Studies in African Linguistics 1). Los Angeles: University of California.Search in Google Scholar
Gaden, Henri. 1909. Essai de grammaire de la langue baguirmienne: Suivi de textes et de vocabulaires baguirmien-français et français-baguirmien. Paris: E. Leroux.Search in Google Scholar
Gerstner-Link, Claudia. 2018. A grammar of Kilmeri. Munich: De Gruyter Mouton.Search in Google Scholar
Gessner, Suwanne C. 2003. The prosodic system of the Dakelh (Carrier) language. Vancouver: University of British Columbia PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Golston, Chris & Phong Yang. 2001. White Hmong loanwords phonology. Proceedings of HILP 5. Available at: http://hdl.handle.net/10211.3/179358.Search in Google Scholar
González, Hebe Alicia. 2005. A grammar of Tapiete (Tupi-Guarani). Pittsburg: University of Pittsburgh PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Gordon, Lynn. 1986. Maricopa morphology and syntax (University of California Publications in Linguistics 108). Berkeley: University of California Press.Search in Google Scholar
Haacke, W. H. G. 2015. Lexical borrowing by Khoekhoegowab from Cape Dutch and Afrikaans. Stellenbosch Papers in Linguistics Plus 47. 59–74. https://doi.org/10.5842/47-0-653.Search in Google Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin. 1993. A grammar of Lezgian (Mouton Grammar Library 9). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar
Heath, Jeffrey. 2011. A grammar of Koyra Chiini: The Songhay of Timbuktu. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar
Hualde, José Ignacio & Jon Ortiz de Urbina (eds.). 2003. A grammar of Basque. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar
Innes, Gordon. 1966. An introduction to Grebo. London: School of Oriental and African Studies, University of London.Search in Google Scholar
Karlsson, Fred. 1999. Finnish: An essential grammar. London: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar
Karlsson, Anastasia Mukhanova. 2005. Vowels in Mongolian speech: Deletions and epenthesis. Lund University, Department of Linguistics Working Papers 51. 105–123.Search in Google Scholar
Kieviet, Paulus. 2017. A grammar of Rapa Nui (Studies in Diversity Linguistics 12). Berlin: Language Science Press.Search in Google Scholar
Knudson, Lyle M. 1975. A natural phonology and morphophonemics of Chimalapa Zoque. Papers in Linguistics 8. 283–346. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351817509370404.Search in Google Scholar
Labrune, Laurence. 2012. The Phonology of Japanese. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Lloret, Maria-Rosa. 1997. Oromo phonology. In Alan S. Kaye (ed.), Phonologies of Asia and Africa, vol. 1, 493–519. Winona Lake: Eisenbrauns.Search in Google Scholar
Macaulay, Monica. 1996. A grammar of Chalcatongo Mixtec (University of California Publications in Linguistics 127). Berkeley: University of California Press.Search in Google Scholar
Maring, Joel M. 1967. Grammar of Acoma Kerean. Bloomington: Indiana University PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Markopoulos, Giorgos. 2018. Phonological realization of morphological features. Thessaloniki: Aristotle University of Thessaloniki PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Marlett, Stephen A. & Joseph Paul Stemberger. 1983. Empty consonants in Seri. Linguistic Inquiry 14(4). 617–639.Search in Google Scholar
Matras, Yaron. 2012. A grammar of Domari. Göttingen: De Gruyter Mouton.Search in Google Scholar
McGregor, William. 1990. A functional grammar of Gooniyandi. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Search in Google Scholar
Melnar, Lynette R. 1998. Caddo verb morphology. Chicago: University of Chicago PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Merlan, Francesca C. 1994. A grammar of Wardaman. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar
Michelson, Karen. 1988. A comparative study of Lake-Iroquoian accent. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.Search in Google Scholar
Montler, Timothy R. & Heather K. Hardy. 1991. The phonology of negation in Alabama. International Journal of American Linguistics 57(1). 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1086/ijal.57.1.3519711.Search in Google Scholar
Morgan, Lawrence Richard. 1991. A description of the Kutenai language. Berkeley: University of California Berkeley PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Moulineaux, Benjamin. 2018. The life cycle of Mapudungun epenthetic vowels. Paper presented at the 26th Manchester Phonology Meeting, University of Manchester, 28 May.Search in Google Scholar
Murane, Elizabeth. 1974. Daga grammar: From morpheme to discourse (Summer Institute of Linguistics Publications in Linguistics and Related Fields 43). Norman: Summer Institute of Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar
Mwita, Leonard Chacha. 2009. The adaptation of Swahili loanwords from Arabic: A constraint-based analysis. Journal of Pan African Studies 8(2). 46–61.Search in Google Scholar
Nacaskul, K. 1979. A note on English loanwords in Thai. In Theraphan L. Thongkum (eds.), Studies in Tai and Mon-Khmer phonetics and phonology in honour of Eugénie J. A. Henderson, 151–162. Bangkok: Chulalongkorn University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Newman, Paul. 2000. The Hausa language: An encyclopedic reference grammar. New Haven: Yale University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Nguyen, Huynh Trang & Hemanga Dutta. 2017. The adaptation of French consonant clusters in Vietnamese phonology: An OT account. Journal of Universal Language 18(1). 69–103. https://doi.org/10.22425/jul.2017.18.1.69.Search in Google Scholar
Oakes, Terry J. 2001. A description of Teribe Phonology. SIL International. https://www.sil.org/system/files/reapdata/14/03/32/140332481674964318063764980597949308448/SILEWP2001_003.pdf (accessed 18 April 2024).Search in Google Scholar
O’Neill, Timothy. 2015. The phonology of Betsimisrak Malagasy. Newark: University of Delaware PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Palancar, Enrique. 2009. Gramática del Otomí de San Ildefonso Tultepec. Querétaro: Universidad Autónoma de Querétaro.Search in Google Scholar
Payne, Doris. 1985. Aspects of the grammar of Yagua: A typological approach. Los Angeles: University of California, Los Angeles PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Popjes, Jack & Jo Popjes. 1986. Canela-Krahô. In Desmond C. Derbyshire & Geoffrey K. Pullum (eds.), Handbook of Amazonian languages 1, 128–199. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar
Rahbar, Elham Rohany. 2012. Aspect of Persian phonology and morpho-phonology. Toronto: University of Toronto PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Rhee, Sang Jik. 2002. Empty nuclei in Korean. Utrecht: LOT.Search in Google Scholar
Rice, Keren. 1989. A grammar of Slave (Mouton Grammar Library 5). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar
Roberts, John R. 2016. Amele RRG grammatical sketch. SIL International. https://www.sil.org/resources/archives/68531 (accessed 18 April 2024).Search in Google Scholar
Romero-Figueroa, A. 1997. A reference grammar of Warao (Lincom Studies in Native American Linguistics 6). Munich: Lincom Europa.Search in Google Scholar
Russell, Kevin. 1992. Palatalization and epenthesis in Plains Cree. In Carrie Dyck, Jila Ghomeshi & Tom Wilson (eds.), Proceedings of the 1992 annual conference of the Canadian Linguistic Association, 269–284. Toronto: Linguistic Graduate Course Union.Search in Google Scholar
Samarin, William J. 1967. A grammar of Sango. The Hague: Mouton.Search in Google Scholar
Sandy, Claire S. 2017. Prosodic prominence in Karuk. Berkeley: University of California, Berkeley PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Schachter, Paul & Fé T. Otanes. 1972. Tagalog reference grammar. Berkeley: University of California Press.Search in Google Scholar
Scheffler, Tatjana. 2005. A stem-less description of Lezgian inflectional morphology. Unpublished manuscript, University of Pensylvania. https://www.ling.upenn.edu/∼tatjana/papers/lezgian.pdf (accessed 18 April 2024).Search in Google Scholar
Schütz, Albert J. 1985. The Fijian language. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.Search in Google Scholar
Smits, Helena Johanna. 2017. A grammar of Lumun, a Kordofanian language of Sudan. Leiden: Leiden University PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Soe, Myint. 1999. A grammar of Burmese. Eugene: University of Oregon.Search in Google Scholar
Sutton, Logan Dean. 2014. Kiowa-Tanoan: A synchronic and diachronic study. Bloomington: Indiana University PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Terrill, Angela. 2003. A grammar of Lavukaleve. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar
Topping, Donald M. (with the assistance of Bernadita C. Dungca). 1973. Chamorro reference grammar. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.Search in Google Scholar
Uffman, Christian. 2002. A typology of epenthetic vowels in loanwords. Unpublished handout. http://www.ai.mit.edu/projects/dm/featgeom/uffmann-epenthLoanwords.pdf (accessed 18 April 2024).Search in Google Scholar
Van Egmond, M.-E. 2012. Enindhilyakwa phonology, morphosyntax and genetic position. Sydney: University of Sydney PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Voorhoeve, Clemens L. 1980. The Asmat languages of Irian Jaya. Canberra: Australian National University.Search in Google Scholar
Wolfart, H. Christoph. 1973. Plains Cree: A grammatical study (Transactions of the American Philosophical Society, n.s., 63.5). Philadelphia: American Philosophical Society.Search in Google Scholar
© 2024 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston
Articles in the same Issue
- Frontmatter
- Editorial
- Editorial 2024
- Phonetics & Phonology
- The role of recoverability in the implementation of non-phonemic glottalization in Hawaiian
- Epenthetic vowel quality crosslinguistically, with focus on Modern Hebrew
- Japanese speakers can infer specific sub-lexicons using phonotactic cues
- Articulatory phonetics in the market: combining public engagement with ultrasound data collection
- Investigating the acoustic fidelity of vowels across remote recording methods
- The role of coarticulatory tonal information in Cantonese spoken word recognition: an eye-tracking study
- Tracking phonological regularities: exploring the influence of learning mode and regularity locus in adult phonological learning
- Morphology & Syntax
- #AreHashtagsWords? Structure, position, and syntactic integration of hashtags in (English) tweets
- The meaning of morphomes: distributional semantics of Spanish stem alternations
- A refinement of the analysis of the resultative V-de construction in Mandarin Chinese
- L2 cognitive construal and morphosyntactic acquisition of pseudo-passive constructions
- Semantics & Pragmatics
- “All women are like that”: an overview of linguistic deindividualization and dehumanization of women in the incelosphere
- Counterfactual language, emotion, and perspective: a sentence completion study during the COVID-19 pandemic
- Constructing elderly patients’ agency through conversational storytelling
- Language Documentation & Typology
- Conative animal calls in Macha Oromo: function and form
- The syntax of African American English borrowings in the Louisiana Creole tense-mood-aspect system
- Syntactic pausing? Re-examining the associations
- Bibliographic bias and information-density sampling
- Historical & Comparative Linguistics
- Revisiting the hypothesis of ideophones as windows to language evolution
- Verifying the morpho-semantics of aspect via typological homogeneity
- Psycholinguistics & Neurolinguistics
- Sign recognition: the effect of parameters and features in sign mispronunciations
- Influence of translation on perceived metaphor features: quality, aptness, metaphoricity, and familiarity
- Effects of grammatical gender on gender inferences: Evidence from French hybrid nouns
- Processing reflexives in adjunct control: an exploration of attraction effects
- Language Acquisition & Language Learning
- How do L1 glosses affect EFL learners’ reading comprehension performance? An eye-tracking study
- Modeling L2 motivation change and its predictive effects on learning behaviors in the extramural digital context: a quantitative investigation in China
- Ongoing exposure to an ambient language continues to build implicit knowledge across the lifespan
- On the relationship between complexity of primary occupation and L2 varietal behavior in adult migrants in Austria
- The acquisition of speaking fundamental frequency (F0) features in Cantonese and English by simultaneous bilingual children
- Sociolinguistics & Anthropological Linguistics
- A computational approach to detecting the envelope of variation
- Attitudes toward code-switching among bilingual Jordanians: a comparative study
- “Let’s ride this out together”: unpacking multilingual top-down and bottom-up pandemic communication evidenced in Singapore’s coronavirus-related linguistic and semiotic landscape
- Across time, space, and genres: measuring probabilistic grammar distances between varieties of Mandarin
- Navigating linguistic ideologies and market dynamics within China’s English language teaching landscape
- Streetscapes and memories of real socialist anti-fascism in south-eastern Europe: between dystopianism and utopianism
- What can NLP do for linguistics? Towards using grammatical error analysis to document non-standard English features
- From sociolinguistic perception to strategic action in the study of social meaning
- Minority genders in quantitative survey research: a data-driven approach to clear, inclusive, and accurate gender questions
- Variation is the way to perfection: imperfect rhyming in Chinese hip hop
- Shifts in digital media usage before and after the pandemic by Rusyns in Ukraine
- Computational & Corpus Linguistics
- Revisiting the automatic prediction of lexical errors in Mandarin
- Finding continuers in Swedish Sign Language
- Conversational priming in repetitional responses as a mechanism in language change: evidence from agent-based modelling
- Construction grammar and procedural semantics for human-interpretable grounded language processing
- Through the compression glass: language complexity and the linguistic structure of compressed strings
- Could this be next for corpus linguistics? Methods of semi-automatic data annotation with contextualized word embeddings
- The Red Hen Audio Tagger
- Code-switching in computer-mediated communication by Gen Z Japanese Americans
- Supervised prediction of production patterns using machine learning algorithms
- Introducing Bed Word: a new automated speech recognition tool for sociolinguistic interview transcription
- Decoding French equivalents of the English present perfect: evidence from parallel corpora of parliamentary documents
- Enhancing automated essay scoring with GCNs and multi-level features for robust multidimensional assessments
- Sociolinguistic auto-coding has fairness problems too: measuring and mitigating bias
- The role of syntax in hashtag popularity
- Language practices of Chinese doctoral students studying abroad on social media: a translanguaging perspective
- Cognitive Linguistics
- Metaphor and gender: are words associated with source domains perceived in a gendered way?
- Crossmodal correspondence between lexical tones and visual motions: a forced-choice mapping task on Mandarin Chinese
Articles in the same Issue
- Frontmatter
- Editorial
- Editorial 2024
- Phonetics & Phonology
- The role of recoverability in the implementation of non-phonemic glottalization in Hawaiian
- Epenthetic vowel quality crosslinguistically, with focus on Modern Hebrew
- Japanese speakers can infer specific sub-lexicons using phonotactic cues
- Articulatory phonetics in the market: combining public engagement with ultrasound data collection
- Investigating the acoustic fidelity of vowels across remote recording methods
- The role of coarticulatory tonal information in Cantonese spoken word recognition: an eye-tracking study
- Tracking phonological regularities: exploring the influence of learning mode and regularity locus in adult phonological learning
- Morphology & Syntax
- #AreHashtagsWords? Structure, position, and syntactic integration of hashtags in (English) tweets
- The meaning of morphomes: distributional semantics of Spanish stem alternations
- A refinement of the analysis of the resultative V-de construction in Mandarin Chinese
- L2 cognitive construal and morphosyntactic acquisition of pseudo-passive constructions
- Semantics & Pragmatics
- “All women are like that”: an overview of linguistic deindividualization and dehumanization of women in the incelosphere
- Counterfactual language, emotion, and perspective: a sentence completion study during the COVID-19 pandemic
- Constructing elderly patients’ agency through conversational storytelling
- Language Documentation & Typology
- Conative animal calls in Macha Oromo: function and form
- The syntax of African American English borrowings in the Louisiana Creole tense-mood-aspect system
- Syntactic pausing? Re-examining the associations
- Bibliographic bias and information-density sampling
- Historical & Comparative Linguistics
- Revisiting the hypothesis of ideophones as windows to language evolution
- Verifying the morpho-semantics of aspect via typological homogeneity
- Psycholinguistics & Neurolinguistics
- Sign recognition: the effect of parameters and features in sign mispronunciations
- Influence of translation on perceived metaphor features: quality, aptness, metaphoricity, and familiarity
- Effects of grammatical gender on gender inferences: Evidence from French hybrid nouns
- Processing reflexives in adjunct control: an exploration of attraction effects
- Language Acquisition & Language Learning
- How do L1 glosses affect EFL learners’ reading comprehension performance? An eye-tracking study
- Modeling L2 motivation change and its predictive effects on learning behaviors in the extramural digital context: a quantitative investigation in China
- Ongoing exposure to an ambient language continues to build implicit knowledge across the lifespan
- On the relationship between complexity of primary occupation and L2 varietal behavior in adult migrants in Austria
- The acquisition of speaking fundamental frequency (F0) features in Cantonese and English by simultaneous bilingual children
- Sociolinguistics & Anthropological Linguistics
- A computational approach to detecting the envelope of variation
- Attitudes toward code-switching among bilingual Jordanians: a comparative study
- “Let’s ride this out together”: unpacking multilingual top-down and bottom-up pandemic communication evidenced in Singapore’s coronavirus-related linguistic and semiotic landscape
- Across time, space, and genres: measuring probabilistic grammar distances between varieties of Mandarin
- Navigating linguistic ideologies and market dynamics within China’s English language teaching landscape
- Streetscapes and memories of real socialist anti-fascism in south-eastern Europe: between dystopianism and utopianism
- What can NLP do for linguistics? Towards using grammatical error analysis to document non-standard English features
- From sociolinguistic perception to strategic action in the study of social meaning
- Minority genders in quantitative survey research: a data-driven approach to clear, inclusive, and accurate gender questions
- Variation is the way to perfection: imperfect rhyming in Chinese hip hop
- Shifts in digital media usage before and after the pandemic by Rusyns in Ukraine
- Computational & Corpus Linguistics
- Revisiting the automatic prediction of lexical errors in Mandarin
- Finding continuers in Swedish Sign Language
- Conversational priming in repetitional responses as a mechanism in language change: evidence from agent-based modelling
- Construction grammar and procedural semantics for human-interpretable grounded language processing
- Through the compression glass: language complexity and the linguistic structure of compressed strings
- Could this be next for corpus linguistics? Methods of semi-automatic data annotation with contextualized word embeddings
- The Red Hen Audio Tagger
- Code-switching in computer-mediated communication by Gen Z Japanese Americans
- Supervised prediction of production patterns using machine learning algorithms
- Introducing Bed Word: a new automated speech recognition tool for sociolinguistic interview transcription
- Decoding French equivalents of the English present perfect: evidence from parallel corpora of parliamentary documents
- Enhancing automated essay scoring with GCNs and multi-level features for robust multidimensional assessments
- Sociolinguistic auto-coding has fairness problems too: measuring and mitigating bias
- The role of syntax in hashtag popularity
- Language practices of Chinese doctoral students studying abroad on social media: a translanguaging perspective
- Cognitive Linguistics
- Metaphor and gender: are words associated with source domains perceived in a gendered way?
- Crossmodal correspondence between lexical tones and visual motions: a forced-choice mapping task on Mandarin Chinese