Home Repetition in discourses across languages and genres
Article Open Access

Repetition in discourses across languages and genres

  • Kamal Abdullayev , Azad Mammadov EMAIL logo , Misgar Mammadov and Shaban Huseynov
Published/Copyright: August 12, 2024
Become an author with De Gruyter Brill

Abstract

Despite the fact that various functions of repetition in discourse have been studied, we have decided to continue our exploration of the role of phonetic and lexical repetitions in the pragmatics and cognition of text and discourse focusing our attention on the epic texts and modern literary discourses across languages. The reason why we have decided to analyze both epic texts and modern literary discourses across languages is our intention to reveal the pragmatic nature of intersubjective behaviors regardless of time and place. In addition, phonetic and lexical repetitions play a crucial role in the cognition of these texts and discourses as they cause associations by stimulating knowledge in the human mind. Thus, our article’s goal is to analyze the ways why and how the sender uses these very important explicit linguistic devices to produce pragmatic and cognitive effects on the audience of the epic texts, as well as on the participants of modern fictional discourses in English, Azerbaijani, and Russian. Throughout the history, phonetic and lexical repetitions have gradually become key devices in producing pragmatic and cognitive effects in literary discourses across languages. The examples from Beowulf, The Book of Dede Korkut, as well as from modern fictional and poetic discourses in Azerbaijani and English clearly demonstrate this trend. We can draw a conclusion that as human’s life experience and his/her intellectual level gradually develops, the functions of these repetitions in discourse expand and gain a new meaning. Repetition is a choice made by the sender of any text, first of all, based on his/her subjective individual rather than social preferences. That is why the use of repetition in discourse motivates intersubjective behaviors regardless of time and place.

1 Introduction: Theoretical framework, methodology and data

1.1 Theoretical framework

Since the subjectivity of discourse depends on discourse participants, intersubjectivity appears to be crucial for mutual understanding and mutual influencing during discourse production. In this connection, the remarks made by Van Dijk are quite symptomatic (2008, 119):

In sum, in this sociocognitive paradigm (and the same is true for a constructionist perspective), there is no such thing as an objective situation, unless I define such objectivity, as I have done for knowledge, as some kind of intersubjective, commonsense definition of the situation, that is, as an understanding shared by the members of a group or community.

In the intersubjective behaviors, repetition is the only instrument for directing the emotional dynamism of subjectivity toward one single way of thinking. The reason is the ability of repetition of the linguistic forms to justify the process of discourse production. During this process, an individual human experience has a specific intellectual power over his/her consciousness. On the other hand, repetition ensures a strategic partnership and mutual agreement during the strong influence of the emotionality of the process of discourse production based on subjectivity and intersubjectivity. Gallagher writes that “[…] intersubjectivity is important for an adequate account of perception” (2008, 163). He further develops this idea referring to the role “of an implicit intersubjectivity built into the perceptual experience” (2008, 171). At the same time, repetition helps to achieve perception, which defines intersubjective acts in discourse. Thus, the context created by intersubjective behaviors can build an impression thanks to the knowledge about semantics and pragmatics of repetition used during discourse production.

Repetition in discourse uses intersubjective behaviors for actualization of what is perceived. It adds an element of subjectivity in the process of concretizing and specifying repetition, resulting in full perception in discourse. Intersubjective behavior creates necessary conditions for cognition during repetition in discourse because it is rich with separate subjective content. Otherwise, subjective behavior will not be able to motivate repetition to play a role in cognitive processes. Intersubjective behavior therefore is one of the general human competencies that cause dynamic changes in the communicative situations during repetition. Since repetition motivates cognitive processes, it can be divided into two types: repetition in mind and repetition in language (Mammadov et al. 2019, 81).

It is obvious that repetition in mind implies reminding. It means that since reminding of the repeated object in different contexts of time and space is characterized by its situational features, the direction of perception is changing in line with the polyphony of consciousness. The polyphonic capacities of consciousness motivate functioning of repetition as an act of mind situationally. That is why repetition is not merely the product of reminding, but also perception of the new situation in line with the logic of time and space changes.

Repetition in language, i.e., direct repetitions of sounds, morphemes, words, phrases and sentences, helps us to find answers to the following two questions: how to perceive and why to perceive during discourse production. The reason is our metaphysic observation of the gestalt principle of mind in the process of perception. Repetitions are linguistic multiplicities, and this multiplicity is accompanied by polyphonic perceptions on the level of consciousness. A linguistic device performs three functions regarding the connection between the real world and cognitive activity: the real world, its linguistic manifestations, and the linguistic reality as a result of the pragmatic attitude of a person to reality. All three functions have a strong link to the stimulus nature of repetition, which predetermines pragmatics and cognition vis-a-vis context.

Phonetic, morphological, lexical, and syntactic repetitions have been traditionally among the most challenging issues in rhetoric (Cockroft et al. 2014, Fahnstock 2011) and stylistics (Simpson 1992). In recent decades, various types of repetitions have also been extensively studied in text linguistics (Halliday and Hasan 1976, Haegemann 1988, Karoly 2003) and discourse studies (Tannen 1989, Hoey 1991, Nascione 2010), which explore them on usage-based methodology (Diessel 2017). This methodology provides an effective tool to study language, its structure, and various linguistic devices belonging to this structure. Moreover, the usage-based methodology can allow the researchers to reveal the functional-pragmatic and cognitive features of language. Since discourse is considered by Semino as “naturally occuring language use: real instances of writing or speech which are produced and interpreted in particular circumstances and for particular purposes” (2008:1), it therefore appears to be an ideal platform for functional-pragmatic and cognitive analysis of various linguistic devices, including repetitions. It therefore appears to be an ideal platform for functional-pragmatic and cognitive analysis of various linguistic devices, including repetitions. The sender of text uses repetitions to deliver his/her pragmatic intention to the receiver of text. On the other hand, these devices have very important cognitive potential. Thus, various structural types of repetitions (phonetic, morphological, lexical, and syntactic) are very important linguistic devices since they are key elements during discourse production.

Obviously, senders of text use different types of repetitions for various purposes in discourse, and therefore, it is difficult to distinguish functions of repetitions in discourse. From a perspective of functional and cognitive richness and potential, phonetic and lexical repetitions have traditionally drawn special attention. Phonetic repetitions, such as alliteration (repetition of initial consonants), play a very important role in text and discourse. There are numerous studies on the poetic and other functions of this type of repetition in literary texts (Jakobson 2010, Roper 2011). Lexical repetitions have also been well-studied from different perspectives (Halliday and Hasan 1976, Hoey 1991, Karoly 2003, etc.). Within text linguistics and discourse analysis, researchers usually analyze the role of lexical repetition as a powerful tool to build cohesion and coherence in text and discourse. Thus, despite these and numerous other studies, the article revisits the role of repetition in discourse focusing on the ways how the combination of phonetic and lexical repetitions functions in literary discourses across languages and genres to produce pragmatic and cognitive effects.

1.2 Methodology and data

Thus, this study is another attempt to explore the role of phonetic and lexical repetitions in the pragmatics and cognition of discourse. For this purpose, the following methods have been used:

  1. the method of discourse analysis;

  2. the traditional method of contrastive analysis to study the direct repetition of sounds and words from the data.

In order to justify this methodology, the article considers repetition in language as one of the core elements in the series of choices senders make based on their individual and social preferences. It therefore aims to explore phonetic and lexical repetitions extensively used in discourses across genres from a perspective of cognitive-pragmatic approach (MacMahon 2007, Schmidt 2012). The analysis of phonetic and lexical repetitions across languages is a productive way to understand better their pragmatic and cognitive nature. In this connection, literary discourse is ideal since it can help to achieve the objective of the study.

In total, more than 1,500 pages from epic texts, as well as twentieth-century fictional and poetic discourses, have been analyzed. They were selected as a result of intensive selective work with relevant material from Azerbaijani (The Book of Dede Korkut and Movlud Suleymanli’s Three novels), British English (Beowulf and Joanne Rowling’s Harry Potter and the Philosopher’s Stone), and American English (E. Hemingway’s For Whom the Bell Tolls and The Old Man and The Sea). Various types of repetitions are commonly used linguistic devices in the literary texts under analysis, and their analysis has convinced us to formulate a theoretical framework and to write this article. The reason why two epics (Beowulf and The Book of Dede Korkut) have been selected for analysis is that these texts can also be considered as discourse based on its definition “of naturally occurring language use: real instances of writing or speech which are produced and interpreted in particular circumstances and for particular purposes” given by Semino (2008, 1). Despite these two epics belonging to different locations, they are in close proximity in terms of time. It gives an argument to suggest that the participants of these discourses had similar linguistic processes in their consciousness. It can also give us an argument to draw some conclusions about the way of thinking of the people of that time. Thus, the comparative study of the role of phonetic and lexical repetitions in the epic and modern literary discourses can help us understand better the complex nature of repetition both in mind and in language.

1.3 Phonetic repetition

One of the crucial issues in the studies of linguistic pragmatic is the analysis of the role of language in the realization of the pragmatic intention of text. İn this connection, the most intriguing and complex issue is the study of the way how various linguistic devices are used to perform pragmatic functions in discourse. From this perspective, phonetic and lexical repetitions draw a special interest among these devices, since they play a very important role in text pragmatics.

Traditionally, phonetic repetition is considered one of the most productive devices to arouse poetic effect in literary discourse. Recent studies explore this function of phonetic repetitions from the perspective of cognitive pragmatics (Pilkington 2000, MacMahon 2007). One of the most extensively studied types of phonetic repetition is alliteration (repetition of the initial consonant sound). It is common in the various genres of literary texts, especially in the epic texts. Kendall suggests that “Beowulf is a poem of infinite variety. It we cannot know precisely what it may have meant to its original, probably monastic, audience, it certainly speaks to modern audiences in many modes and moods” (1991, 1). Based on these remarks, we can conclude that Beowulf and other epics can be considered as discourses with strong pragmatics and cognition. On the other hand, discourse analysis of old texts can raise questions about the definition of discourse given by Enkvist: “discourse implies text + context,” which contains a situational component (1989, 379). In this connection, the following remarks made by Warwik are interesting (2013, 217):

Likewise in analysing the Old English texts selected for the present study, it was problematic to distinguish between certain unused elements, inferrables and situationally evoked elements. References to priests, churches and other religious and ecclesiastical items could in particularly all cases be assumed as familiar, but it was not possible to determine whether this was because they were part of the assumed background knowledge of the audience of the sermon, or if they were inferrable from elements already familiar on the basis of the preceding discourse, or if they were part of the situational context of the sermon.

It is really hard to reveal the real reasons behind the referential use of lexical repetitions to draw certain conclusions about pragmatic intentions in epic discourses. In any case, the fact of the use of repetitions demonstrates certain pragmatic intentions of the sender. Repetition of the different units of language is the reconstruction of what is in the language in discourse, which means the creativity of the sender. It serves to the delivery of ideas by the author in this context as one of the tools of realization of text pragmatics. In this connection, the repetition of sounds draws special attention. Goering writes (2020, 909):

Line 1122 of Beowulf represents a problem where the findings of metrics, historical phonology, and the reading of the manuscript are in conflict with one another. I revive and adapt Tolkien’s proposal to emend lāðbite līċes līġ ealle forswealg to lāðbite līġes līċ eall forswealg “the cruel bite of fire swallowed up the entire bodies”. This emended reading allows the line to be scanned in accordance with Kaluza’s law without any complications to the standard historical phonology of Old English, and furthermore improves both the syntax and semantics of the overall passage.

The author focuses on the role of repetition of the consonant sound l in line 1122 from Beowulf from the perspective of syntax and semantics in the whole passage. The remarks made by Naciscione explicitly show this role of repetition in the construction of meaning in discourse (2010, 122):

In discourse analysis, another role of repetition is revealed: that of creating relationships and new meaning in a discourse world.

Despite the fact that basically the instances of the use of the repetitions of sounds do not differ from the repetitions of other linguistic units (grammatical and lexical); meantime, they still have some specific features typical to the units of this level. The repetitions of sounds have the same potential and the most important issue here is the innovative way of thinking of the sender reflecting his/her ability to reconstruct meaning goal-orientedly. Abdullayev discusses the instances of the systemic use of repetitions in The Book of Dede Korkut (2017, 206):

The issue of the instances of repetitions in the Book of Dede Korkut as a poetic-stylistic device, especially their primitive literary poetic feature draws a special attention. The repetition of sounds, words, phrases, sentences and even situations is the linguistic-stylistic phenomenon, which has a systemic nature in this epic text.

It is worth noting that phonetic repetitions are typical for epic texts across languages. For example, alliteration is so common in Beowulf that the researchers suggest that this epos was constructed based on this type of repetition (Roper 2011). Especially alliteration is considered an archetype of rhyme by replacing it in the sense of contemporary studies. Thus, another function of alliteration becomes obvious in this epos. The repetition of the initial consonant in the stressed syllabus existed not just as a crucial factor in the construction of epic poetry, but also in the realization of the pragmatic intention of the narrator. The main issue here is the choice of those consonant sounds, which are suitable in terms of alliterative verse and the pragmatics of the text under analysis. In this connection, the following pieces of text from Beowulf are symptomatic:

Heorogar, Hroðgar,

Halga til.

Hyrde ic þæt ides

wæs æþelan cwen,

Heaðo-Scilfingas

healsgebedda.

(Kiernan Kevin, 2015, Electronic Beowulf, Fourth Edition

http://ebeowulf.uky.edu/ebeo4.0/CD/main.html)

Heorogar, Hrothgar, the good Halga,

And a daughter, I have heard who was Onela’s Queen

A balm in bed to the battle-scarred Swede.

(Beowulf, translated by Seamus Heaney, W.W. Norton & Company, 2000)

And:

leoman to leohte

landbuendum.

gefrætwade

foldan sceatas,

leomum leafum,

lif eac gesceop

(Kiernan Kevin, 2015, Electronic Beowulf, Fourth Edition

http://ebeowulf.uky.edu/ebeo4.0/CD/main.html)

To be earth’s lamplight, lanterns for men,

And filled the broad lap of the world

With branches and leaves; and quickened life

(Beowulf, translated by Seamus Heaney, W.W. Norton & Company, 2000)

The analysis of the repetitions of the consonant sounds h and l suggests that an alliterative verse existed in Beowulf as a lingua-poetic device. It made the language of the epos rhythmic and musical, which is so important in terms of text pragmatics. Thus, alliteration performs not only a lingua-poetic function, but also a pragmatic function in this epos. The reason is the richness of the poetic mind of the narrator. The analysis of alliteration in The Book of Dede Korkut suggests that this phenomenon produces the same pragmatic effect in this epos:

Mərə, dini yoq, əqlsiz kafər!

Ussı yoq dərnəksiz kafər!

Qarşu yatan qarlu qara tağlar,

Qarıyıbdır, otı bitməz.

Qanlu-qanlu ırmaqlar

Qurıyıbdur suyı gəlməz.

Şahbaz-şahbaz atlar

Qarıyıbdur qulun verməz.

Qızıl-qızıl dəvələr

Qarıyıbdur köşək verməz.

Mərə kafər, Qazanın. Anası

Qarıyubdur oğul verməz.

(Kitabi – Dədə Qorqud, Bakı Yazıçı 1988, 49)

Godless brainless infidel

Witless disordered infidel!

Yonder snowcapped black mountains have grown old,

No grass grows on them.

The blood-red rivers have grown old,

No water flows in them.

The falcon-swift horses have grown old,

They give no foals.

The red camels have grown old,

They give no young.

Infidel! Kazan’s mother has grown old,

She gives no sons,

(The Book of Dede Korkut, Translated with Introduction and Notes by Jeoffry Lewis, Penguin Books, 1974, 54–55).

Alliteration in this epic text is not a poetic verse such as in Beowulf, but rather a very strong stylistic-pragmatic device. The repetition of the consonant sound Q in this text is in semantic harmony with the word yoq (does not have) used in the first two lines. It refers to the notion lack in the whole piece of this text. In this sense, the hate against the enemy is gradually strengthened by the repetition of the sound Q. By this, the pragmatics of the text is fully realized.

The resemblances of alliteration in these two epic texts give us an argument to suggest that in the early stages of the cultural development of the world population, alliterative verse as a complex mechanism was the main form of the language of any epos, reflecting an internal rhyme and harmony. In the tenth–eleventh centuries, alliterative verse lost its dominance as a device for the construction of poetic texts and a traditional rhyme was introduced and developed. It coincided with the period when The Book of Dede Korkut was recorded in written form (Sumer 1972). Despite the dominance of the rhyme in the epic texts, alliteration was still preserved and used as a stylistic and pragmatic device by the narrators. It reflects a new stage in the development of alliteration as a model of consciousness.

Alliteration used in combination with lexical repetition is one of the most powerful tools in the realization of pragmatic intention in modern literary discourses. Based on the individual properties of the authors, repetitions are constructed and used so creatively that they become central in terms of pragmatics and author’s style. In this connection, the following piece of text from E. Hemingway’s For Whom the Bell Tolls is quite symptomatic:

He lay flat on the brown, pine-needled floor of the forest, his chin on his folded arms, and high overhead the wind blew in the tops of the pine-trees. The mountainside sloped gently where he lay; but below it was steep and he could see the dark of the oiled road winding through the pass. There was a stream alongside the road and far down the pass he saw a mill beside the stream and the falling water of the dam, while in the summer sunlight.

“Is that the mill?” he asked.

“Yes.”

“I do not remember it. It was built since you were here. The old mill is farther down; much below the pass.”

He spread the photostated military map out on the forest floor and looked at it carefully. The old man looked over his shoulder. He was a short and solid old man in a black peasant’s smock and grey iron-stiff trousers and he wore rope-soled shoes. He was breathing heavily from the climb and his hand rested on one of the two heavy packs they had been carrying (For Whom the Bell Tolls, Ernest Hemingway, Triad/Panther Books, 1976, 1).

This example illuminates how the author uses alliteration and lexical repetition to produce a pragmatic effect. In this connection, the consonant sound H + the repetition of he and his frequently used in this piece of fictional text are key. E. Hemingway used them at the beginning of his novel before introducing their names to the reader. Probably he tried to use phonetic and lexical repetitions to draw the attention of the reader to the main characters of the novel with an element of secrecy, which always motivates curiosity. In this connection, the reader is looking for the answers to his/her questions and therefore follows the development of the plot. At a certain point, the author decides to reveal the name of his main character. The same is true in this piece of fictional text from J.F. Rowling’s Harry Potter and the Philosopher’s Stone:

Mr. and Mrs. Dursley, of number four, Privet Drive, were proud to say that they were perfectly normal, thank you very much. They were the last people you’d expect to be involved in anything strange or mysterious, because they just didn’t hold with such nonsense.

Mr. Dursley was the director of a firm called Grunnings, which made drills. He was a big beefy man with hardly any neck, although he did have a very large mustache. Mrs. Dursley was thin and blonde and had nearly twice the usual amount of neck, which came in very useful as she spent so much of her time craning over garden fences, spying on the neighbors. The Dursleys had a small boy called Dudley and in their opinion there was no finer boy anywhere.

The Dursleys had everything they wanted, but they also had a secret and their greatest fear was that somebody would discover it. They didn’t think they could bear it if anyone found out about the Potters. Mrs. Potter was Mrs. Dursley’s sister, but they hadn’t met for several years; in fact, Mrs. Dursley pretended she didn’t have a sister, because her sister and her good-for-nothing husband were as unDursleyish as it was possible to be. The Dursleys shuddered to think what would the neighbors say if the Potters arrive in the street. The Dursleys knew that the Potters had a small son, too, but they had never ever seen him. This boy was another good reason for keeping the Potters away, they didn’t want Dudley mixing with a child like that.

When Mr. and Mrs. Dursley woke up on the dull, gray Tuesday, our story starts, there was nothing about the cloudy sky outside to suggest that strange and mysterious things would soon be happening all over the country. Mrs. Dursley hummed as he picked out his most boring tie for work, and Mrs. Dursley gossiped away happily as she wrestled a screaming Dudley into his high chair (Harry Potter and the Philosopher’s Stone, J.F. Rowling, Bloomsbury, 1997, 1–2).

At the beginning of her story, the author uses alliteration (the repetition of the initial consonant sound D and later P) in combination with the direct repetition of the words Dursley, Dudley and Potter as a powerful instrument to introduce the main characters. These two types of repetitions used in the first two pages of the novel have strong pragmatic potential.

2 Lexical repetition

The use of repetitions of words to fill the semantic gap in the text is a signal of the subjective culture. On the other hand, the cultural richness that is the result of subjective activity stimulated by lexical repetition determines the directions of cognitive behavior. The cognitive activity stimulated by this repetition is a device of creative imagination. Thus, the cognitive aspects of the context constructed by lexical repetition are diverse and profound. For example:

Oral uşağının gözləri qaynayıb oyur-oyur oynayırdı. Böyüyün də, kiçiyinin də gözləri tək özündən baxmırdı, bir adamın gözündən, elə bil, yüz-yüz adamın gözü baxırdı (Üç roman, Mövlud Suleymanlı, 2004, 180).

The eyes of the guys of Oral were excitedly playing. The eyes of the adult and the child didn’t merely watch through their own eyes. It seemed that the eyes of the hundreds watched through the eyes of a person.

İn this piece of fictional text in the Azerbaijani language, the repetition of the word göz eye in different morphological modifications motivates the construction of the images associated with the concept eye. The main cognitive goal of the creative activity in the context constructed by repetition is the delivery of what is perceived to the general mechanism of perception using repetition. It relies on the role of cognitive communication, which varies from culture to culture during deliveries of the cognitive behavior created by repetition toward the general cognitive direction. The function of the subjective culture in the context constructed by lexical repetition in the intersubjective relations ensures mutual perception.

We take into account the differences between intersubjective way of thinking and the attempts of the universal perception for communication in the context constructed by repetition. In this connection, the cultural properties of the mind aimed at understanding meaning are actually subjective culture. The existence of subjective culture within universal cultural values is the central issue in ethnopsychology.

The role of lexical repetition in the construction of the element of culture in intersubjective relations is its ability to be a tool for perception. The cognitive style as a result of the impact of repetition on the mind in the intersubjective relations is the subjective cultural form of the way of thinking and also the cognitive linguistic representation of personality.

It is consciousness that converts linguistic acts into meaning in intersubjective relations. Since lexical repetition is a subjective linguistic intervention, it represents expressiveness accompanied by certain semantic properties in each subjective behavior. For example:

I know now, anyway, he thought. I wish I could show him what sort of man I am. But then he would see the cramped hand. Let him think I am more than I am and I will be so. I wish I was the fish, he thought, with everything he has against only my will and my thought (The Old Man and The Sea, Ernest Hemingway, 53–54).

In this piece of text from E. Hemingway’s The Old Man and The Sea, the repetition of I in all five sentences draws a special interest. The gradual extension from its original referential function (referring to first person singular) to figurative meaning (first to hyperbole I am more than I am and finally to the metaphor I wish I was the fish) has a strong cognitive potential. The same is true in another piece of text from the same novel:

Only I have no luck any more. But who knows? Maybe today. Every day is a new day. It is better to be lucky. But I would rather be exact. Then when luck comes you are ready (The Old Man and The Sea, Ernest Hemingway, 25).

The author repeated luck three times in this piece of text. The last repeated luck was used metonymically when luck comes, which brings an element of additional cognitive effect on the audience. The following piece of the fictional text in the Azerbaijani language is also symptomatic from this perspective:

Bir vaxtlar içlərində bayraqlar yellənirdi. Atların belində olanda rüzgarlar əsib bayraqları yelləndirirdi. Dünyanın boyu uzunu bayraqların havasına gedirdilər. Bir adam lazım idi bayraq kimi, qabağa çıxaydı (Üç roman, Mövlud Suleymanlı, 2004, 124).

Once upon a time the flags were flying inside them. When they were on the horses, the winds were blowing the flags. Across the world, they were following the flags. One person was needed to take a step forward as a flag.

In this piece of fictional text in the Azerbaijani language, the word bayraq flag repeated in different morphological modifications in all four sentences signals the following semantic properties:

  1. the word flag encourages us to fight and to win;

  2. flag on the horses as the symbol of heroism, bravery, and courage, which motivates them to join the battle and win;

  3. flag refers to unity, greatness, and highness;

  4. perception of the functions of the concept flag using repetition as a result of metaforization of all above-mentioned properties – man as a flag.

Thus, metaforization using repetition completes cognition of the image of the concept flag and therefore facilitates perception.

The analysis of the examples suggests that perception of what is unperceived depends on the context of creative activity, which is crucial during repetition. The situation constructed by context creates a favorable condition for repetition in terms of its impact on the receiver. The reason why the emotional impact of repetition in language gains strength depending on the situation is the complex nature of a linguistic sign. The repetition of this sign implements the determining role of what is psychic depending on the context. Vygotsky noted that symbolic reflections are “the psychological means of expression of psychic processes” (1982, 41).

Within the psycholinguistic hierarchy of perception, knowing and remembering are two early stages of the mind. Remembering is a cognitive act. Meanwhile, as the examples under analysis suggest memory–mind relations construct the cognitive connexity. Memorizing for remembering depends on repetition, which conditions cognition. It motivates the transfers from mind to memory and from memory to mind and thus constructs the linguistic representations of cognition. Repetition in language causes psycholinguistic connexity, which leads to the construction of cognitive processes and eventually to perception. The role of repetition in remembering is the necessary mnemonic rule. The function of repetition in remembering is perception, and its psycholinguistic interpretation is the linguistic motivation of the same sense using repetition in the intersubjective behaviors.

Focusing attention on an idea using repetition in the intersubjective behaviors helps to accumulate this idea. It includes thinking of positive semantic nuances based on subjective creative activity produced by repetitions. Thus, repetition implies changing of subjective attitude of a person towards an event during creative activity on the one hand, and motivation, on the other. Since intersubjective communication is an extremely dynamic process, repetition is an issue of time in terms of observing perception. The large number of repetitions is an irreplaceable linguistic time leading to the flexibility of semantization. In general terms, focusing attention on an idea using repetition both conditions cognition and enables stimulus for further behavior.

3 Conclusion

The results of our analysis suggest that phonetic repetitions especially alliteration play a very important role in epic texts. Beowulf was written in alliterative verse, which makes the text of Beowulf stylistically rich. This factor is crucial in the pragmatics of this text. In contrast, alliteration is not so functionally rich in The Book of Dede Korkut and except few cases, we can state that alliterative verse as a device to construct poetic text does not exist in this epos. But it is still strong stylistically and pragmatically. Phonetic repetitions were used in the epic texts as a lingua-poetic device reflecting internal rhyme. Starting from the tenth–eleventh centuries towards modern times, internal rhyme has been replaced by external rhyme thanks to alliteration. As a result, a new system of rhyme has been established in poetry.

Phonetic and lexical repetitions stimulate cognitive meaning and build a link between poetic image and meaning. By doing it, they construct coherence based on the harmony of image, meaning, and word. The aim is to achieve perception. The components (phonetic and lexical repetitions) of the alliterative image complete the emotionality in text and discourse from the cognitive point of view. Emotions are among the most important attempts, which increase creative activity during the construction of meaning in the alliterative images.

Throughout history, different types of repetitions have gradually become key devices in producing pragmatic and cognitive effects in literary discourses across languages. The examples from Beowulf, The Book of Dede Korkut, as well as from modern fictional and poetic discourses in Azerbaijani and English clearly demonstrate this trend. We can draw the conclusion that as human’s life experience and his/her intellectual level gradually develops, the functions of repetitions in discourse expand and gain a new meaning. Repetition is a choice made by the sender of any text, first of all, based on his/her subjective individual rather than social preferences. That is why the use of repetition in discourse motivates intersubjective behaviors regardless of time and place.

Acknowledgements

We extend our sincere gratitude to the editors for all their efforts to help us in the process of reviewing and publication of this article. We are also very grateful to our reviewers for their constructive comments which were instrumental in the process of revision of our manuscript.

  1. Funding information: Authors state no conflict of interest.

  2. Author contributions: All co-authors have accepted responsibility for the entire content of the manuscript and consented to its submission, reviewed all the results, and approved the final version of the manuscript. All co-authors equally contributed to the manuscript based on their field of expertise. KA, AM and MM developed a theoretical framework from the linguistic and psychological perspectives. KA and SHH focused on the analysis of The Book of Dede Korkut and other literary texts in Azerbaijani, whereas AM focused on the analysis of Beowulf and other literary texts in English.

  3. Conflict of interest: All co-authors state that no conflict of interest exists.

  4. Data availability statement: Data sharing is not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.

References

Abdullayev, Kamal. 2017. Introduction to Poetics of the Book of Dede Korkut. Baku: R.S. Poligraph.Search in Google Scholar

Cockroft, Robert, Susan Cockroft, Craig Hamilton, Downing, Laura Hidalgo. 2014. Persuading People: An Introduction to Rhetoric. 3rd ed. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.10.1007/978-1-137-05527-9_1Search in Google Scholar

Diessel, Holger. 2017. “Usage-based Linguistics.” In Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics, edited by Mark Aronoff, 1–24. New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.363Search in Google Scholar

Fahnstock, Jeanne. 2011. Rhetorical Style: The Uses of Language in Persuasion. New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199764129.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Gallagher, Shaun. 2008. “Intersubjectivity in perception.” Continental Philosophy Review 41: 163–78.10.1007/s11007-008-9075-8Search in Google Scholar

Goering, Nelson. 2020. “The Terrible Bite of Fire: Metre, Sound Change, and Emendation in Beowulf 1122.” English Studies 101 (8): 909–20.10.1080/0013838X.2020.1847918Search in Google Scholar

Haegemann, Lilianne. 1988. “Parallel Structures in Syntax.” Lingua 75 (2): 275–87. Amsterdam: Elsevier.10.1016/0024-3841(88)90037-XSearch in Google Scholar

Halliday, Michael and Rugayya Hasan. 1976. Cohesion in English. London: Longman.Search in Google Scholar

Hoey, Michael. 1991. Patterns of Lexis in Discourse. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Jakobson, Roman. 2010. Linguistics and Poetics. Volume III Poetry of Grammar and Grammar of Poetry, 18–51. Berlin, Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Search in Google Scholar

Karoly, Krisztia. 2003. Lexical Repetition in Text: A Study of the Text-Organizing Function of Lexical Repetition in Foreign Language Argumentative Discourse (Metalinguistica). Frankfurt-am-Main: Peter Lang.Search in Google Scholar

Kendall, Calvin. 1991. The Metrical Grammar of Beowulf. Cambridge Studies in Anglo-Saxon England. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

MacMahon, Barbara. 2007. “The Effects of Sound Patterning in Poetry: A Cognitive Pragmatic Approach.” Journal of Literary Semantics 36 (2): 103–20.10.1515/JLS.2007.006Search in Google Scholar

Mammadov, Azad, Misgar Mammadov, and Jamila Rasulova. 2019. Repetition in Discourse. Munchen: LINCOM GmbH.Search in Google Scholar

Nascione, Anita. 2010. Stylistic Use of Phraseological Units in Discourse. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/z.159Search in Google Scholar

Pilkington, Adrian. 2000. Poetic Thoughts and Poetic Effects: A Relevance Theory Perspective. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/pbns.75Search in Google Scholar

Roper, Jonathan. 2011. “Introduction: Key Topics in the Study of Alliteration.” In Alliteration in Culture, edited by Jonathan Roper, 1–20. London: Palgrave Macmillan.10.1057/9780230305878_1Search in Google Scholar

Schmidt, Hans-Yorg. ed. 2012. Cognitive Pragmatics. Berlin: De Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar

Semino, Elena. 2008. Metaphor in Discourse. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511816802.015Search in Google Scholar

Simpson, Paul. 1992. “Teaching Stylistics: Analyzing Cohesion and Narrative Structure in a Short Story by Ernest Hemingway.” Language and Literature 1 (1): 47–67. London: Sage Publications.10.1177/096394709200100104Search in Google Scholar

Sumer, Faruk. 1972. The Book of Dede Korkut A Turkish Epic. University of Texas Press.Search in Google Scholar

Tannen, Debora. 1989. Talking Voices: Repetition, Dialogue and Imagery in Conversational Discourse. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Van Dijk, Teun. 2008. Discourse and Context. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Vygotsky, Lev. 1982. Collection of Works. Moskva: Nauka.Search in Google Scholar

Warvik, Brita. 2013. “Participant continuity and narrative structure: Defining discourse marker functions in Old English.” Folia Linguistica Historica 47: 209–42.10.1515/flih.2013.008Search in Google Scholar

Received: 2022-10-18
Revised: 2024-06-04
Accepted: 2024-06-24
Published Online: 2024-08-12

© 2024 the author(s), published by De Gruyter

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Articles in the same Issue

  1. Research Articles
  2. Describing smell: A comparative analysis of active smell lexicon in Estonian and German
  3. The sound of the Italian comic book: Representing noises, senses, and emotions across 80 years
  4. Framing victimhood, making war: A linguistic historicizing of secessionist discourses
  5. Under pressure: Exploring the impact of cognitive factors on clitics placement in L2 Slovak
  6. The syntax of non-canonical coordination in Jordanian Arabic: An experimental investigation
  7. Suffixation in Zhangzhou
  8. Alignment in Vamale, South Oceanic: Diachrony and contact influence
  9. A corpus-based study of epicene pronouns used by Macedonian learners of English
  10. Iconicity as the motivation for morphophonological metathesis and truncation in Nigerian Pidgin
  11. ‘Little Arabia’ on Buddhist land: Exploring the linguistic landscape of Bangkok’s ‘Soi Arab’ enclave
  12. Mother tongue in Serbia: A speakers’ perspective on the meaning of the concept
  13. Role of six turn-initial demonstrative and emotive particles in Lithuanian
  14. Verbal numeral classifiers in languages of Eastern Eurasia: A typological survey
  15. The multilingual repertoire of the Haitian community in Chapecó (SC, Brazil): Patterns of linguistic evolution in a South–South migration context
  16. ‘Aquí toman mucho sopa’: Linguistic variables as predictors of non-standard gender agreement production in Basque Spanish
  17. Data-driven identification of situated meanings in corpus data using Latent Class Analysis
  18. Adverbs and adverbials in contemporary Arabic syntax: A phase-based account
  19. Spatial effects with missing data
  20. Instability of interactives: The case of interjections in Gorwaa
  21. Linguistic explication of rational and irrational knowledge types in the content of toponyms (on the material of the linguocultural dictionary)
  22. Review Articles
  23. Discourse-related expletives: Challenges and opportunities
  24. Vietnamese tense marking since the seventeenth century: A historiographical analysis
  25. Special Issue: Subjectivity and Intersubjectivity in Language, edited by Külli Habicht, Tiit Hennoste, Helle Metslang, and Renate Pajusalu - Part II
  26. Editorial: Exploring subjectivity and intersubjectivity in language
  27. Two past forms inducing conjectural or non-intrusive questions
  28. A typological approach to intersubjective uses of the Finnish clitic markers =hAn and =se from the perspectives of engagement and their interrelations with subject person
  29. Repetition and variation in a Finnish music-related discourse: A case study
  30. Biased interrogatives in Camuno
  31. On the overlapping discourse functions of Spanish ‘cómo que’ and French ‘comment ça’ interrogatives
  32. Repetition in discourses across languages and genres
  33. Reducing the severity of incidents or emergency in Estonian emergency calls
  34. Special Issue: Request for confirmation sequences across ten languages, edited by Martin Pfeiffer & Katharina König - Part I
  35. Request for confirmation sequences in Mandarin Chinese
  36. Request for confirmation sequences in Korean
  37. Request for confirmation sequences in British and American English
  38. Request for confirmation sequences in German
  39. Request for confirmation sequences in Low German
  40. Request for confirmation sequences in Egyptian Arabic
  41. Request for confirmation sequences in Yurakaré
  42. Request for confirmation sequences in Hebrew
  43. Request for confirmation sequences in Czech
Downloaded on 10.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/opli-2024-0020/html
Scroll to top button