Abstract
This article investigates aspects of stress assignment in a Jordanian Arabic dialect (JA). Cases of opaque stress are discussed with reference to Harmonic Serialism (HS) within the framework of Optimality Theory (OT). Two sources of opaque stress are under consideration. On the one hand, a syllable in an antepenultimate position is stressed where a Heavy penultimate is expected to receive stress. On the other hand, a light penultimate syllable is stressed where an antepenultimate Heavy syllable is expected to receive stress. All cases of opaque stress in the dialect involve an epenthetic vowel in the penultimate syllable or in the ultimate syllable in the two structures, respectively. An account of such opaque structures requires faithfulness to the location of stress before epenthesis; a scenario perfectly handled with reference to HS.
1 Introduction
In a quantity-sensitive stress system, syllable weight is crucial in stress placement. In general, heavy syllables are usually considered prominent in their feet and eventually on the metrical grid. Jordanian Arabic (JA) is an example of a quantity-sensitive stress system where stress is assigned to a word-final super heavy (ꞌSH) syllable made of a long vowel and a coda consonant, or a short vowel followed by two consonants. If no such syllable is at the end of a word, stress will fall on a heavy (ꞌH) penult made up of a long vowel, or a short vowel followed by a consonant. Otherwise, stress will fall on the antepenultimate syllable by default.
Despite the rather straightforward stress assignment principles above, JA has two major cases of opaque stress placement. First, a syllable in the antepenultimate position is stressed where a Heavy penultimate is expected to receive stress (ꞌσHσ vs expected σꞌHσ). Second, a light penultimate syllable is stressed where an antepenultime heavy syllable is expected to receive stress (HꞌLσ vs expected ꞌHLσ). All cases of opaque stress in this dialect involve an epenthetic vowel in the penultimate syllable or in the ultimate syllable in the two structures, respectively.
In this article, we propose that HS can account for opaque stress-epenthesis interactions among languages where epenthetic vowels appear to avoid stress. Other proposals within OT for handling cases of opaque stress involve rejecting stress on epenthetic vowels (Kager 1999a), faithfulness to a base (Kager 1999a, b), output–output faithfulness (Benua 1997), or faithfulness to a sympathetic candidate (McCarthy 1999). HS provides a unified account of both cases of opaque stress in JA.
Harmonic Serialism (McCarthy 2000, 2002, 2008a, b, 2009) is a version of Optimality Theory (Prince and Smolensky 1993/2004) combining serial derivation with constraint interaction. HS imposes a requirement on GEN limiting the candidate set generated. A candidate may differ from the input only by the application of a single operation. The optimal candidate in each pass through GEN and EVAL becomes an input to the next pass. This creates a derivation-like sequence of candidates each of which is more harmonic with the final output. The derivation stops when the input is identical to the output. This gradual improvement of candidates towards the optimal enables the grammar to make reference to intermediate stages, base forms, or sympathetic candidates (Kager 1999a, b, McCarthy 1999).
The rest of this article is organized as follows. Section 2 introduces the dialect under scrutiny, and detail stress assignment rules active in the dialect. Section 3 introduces cases of opaque stress in JA and how the phenomenon was handled in the literature. Section 4 presents the basics of HS and proceeds to account for opaque stress in JA. Section 5 offers a summary and concluding remarks.
2 Jordanian Arabic
Arabic is a Central Semitic language of the Afroasiatic language family spoken primarily in the Arab world (Al-Jallad 2018). Arabic spread from the Arabian Peninsula to North Africa and the Levant at the end of the seventh century. In general, Arabic dialects share some cross-dialectal generalizations (Kiparsky 2002). Nevertheless, not all varieties are mutually intelligible. Intelligibility or the lack of it depends on history, geography, and cultural influences (Watson 2002, 2011a, b).
Various distinct dialects are spoken in Jordan forming a dialect continuum mutually understood by most Jordanians. These are traditionally divided into Sedentary and Bedouin dialects. Sedentary dialects belong to the Levantine dialects continuum, while Bedouin dialects belong to either the Northwest Arabian dialects spoken in the Southern parts of Jordan, or to the Najdi dialects spoken in the Northern parts of the country. Sedentary dialects are socially divided into Urban or Rural and are used in major urban centers and surrounding smaller villages and towns. Bedouin dialects are spoken in the far eastern and southern parts of the country (Abdel-Jawad 1986, Al-Khatib 1988, Al-Sughayer 1990, Abu-Abbas 2003, Sakarna 2005). Each dialect may be further divided into distinct varieties the discussion of which is beyond the scope of this research (Zuraiq and Abu-Joudeh 2013, Al-Harahsheh 2014, Abu Ain 2016, Omari and Herk 2016, Alrabab’ah 2018). Jordanian Arabic in this article will be used to refer to a group of dialects spoken in an urban center in the northern part of the country, mainly the city of Irbid and nearby Ajloun and Jerash. These dialects share stress assignment principles.
2.1 Overview of stress assignment in JA
This overview is based on the elaborate discussion of stress in JA by Al-Ghazu (1987) and Abu-Abbas (2003, 2008). In Arabic, CVVC, CVVCC, or CVCC are super-heavy, CVV and CVC syllables are heavy, and a syllable with a short vowel and no coda, i.e., CV is light. In short, a superheavy syllable has three or more moras, a heavy syllable has two, and a light syllable is one that includes a single mora. A short vowel contributes one mora, a long vowel contributes two, a coda consonant contributes one mora, and onset consonants are weightless (Al-Jarrah 2008, Bokhari 2021).
Syllable quantity plays a major role in stress assignment in all Arabic dialects including JA (Al-Jarrah 2002, Abu-Abbas 2003, 2008, Huneety 2015). Stress is assigned to the rightmost superheavy (SH) syllable provided that it is not separated from the right edge of the word by more than two syllables, i.e., preantepenultimate syllables are never stressed in JA. In the absence of a (super)heavy syllable under the condition above, i.e., in the ultimate or penultimate syllable, the antepenultimate is stressed. Consider the examples in (1) below. These examples include various possible structures in JA starting from monosyllabic words all the way to words with five syllables:
(1) stress in JA
a | Monosyllabic words | Gloss |
ꞌmaat | died | |
ꞌle:l | night | |
ꞌkul | eat (imperative) | |
ꞌkalb | a dog | |
ꞌktaab | a book | |
ꞌrbaaʕ | quarters | |
b | Disyllabic words | Gloss |
initial stress in words ending with CV | ||
ꞌsa.wa | together | |
ꞌʔa.na | I | |
ꞌblaad.na | our countries | |
initial stress in words ending with CVC | ||
ꞌʔi.sim | name | |
ꞌmak.bas | stapler | |
ꞌnaa.jim | asleep | |
ꞌʤaar.hum | their neighbor | |
ꞌʔa.kil | food | |
ꞌkalb.hum | their dog | |
initial stress in words ending with CVV | ||
ꞌfaa.tuu | they entered | |
ꞌba.nuu | they built | |
final stress in words ending with CVVC | ||
da.ꞌmaar | destruction | |
sa. ꞌlaam | peace | |
mis.ꞌmaar | a nail | |
baa.ꞌbe:n | two doors | |
waag.ꞌfaat | standing up (fem. pl.) | |
final stress in words ending with CVCC [1] | ||
ka. ꞌtabt | I wrote | |
ra. ꞌfaʕt | I lifted | |
c | Trisyllabic words | Gloss |
initial stress in words ending with CV.CV(C) | ||
ꞌsa.ma.ka | a fish | |
ꞌwa.ra.ga | a piece of paper | |
ꞌʔa.la.mak | your pain | |
ꞌwa.la.duh | his son | |
ꞌmuħ.ta.ram | respectable | |
initial stress in words ending with CV.CVV [2] | ||
ꞌmak.ta.bii | my office | |
ꞌra.ħa.luu | they moved (left) | |
ꞌka.ta.buu | they wrote | |
ꞌra.sa.muu | they drew | |
penultimate stress in words ending with CVV.CV(C) | ||
ma.ꞌkaa.tib | offices | |
ma.ꞌraa.sim | ceremonies | |
mun.ꞌʃaa.rah | a saw | |
ʔi.ꞌʃaa.rah | a sign | |
penultimate stress in words ending with CVC.CV(V,C) | ||
xaa.ꞌtim.hum | their ring | |
mal.ꞌʕab.naa | our playing ground | |
fi.ꞌhim.naa | he understood us | |
gaa.ꞌwam.hum | he resisted them | |
mus.ꞌtaʕ.ʤal | urgent | |
la. ꞌbint.hum | for their daughter | |
final stress in words ending with CVV(C) | ||
ma.saa.ꞌʤiin | prisoners | |
ħa.ra.ꞌkaat | moves | |
miʤ.tam.ꞌʕiin | gathered | |
wa.la.ꞌde:n | two boys | |
mil.taz.ꞌmaat | committed (fem. pl.) | |
d | Words with more than three syllables | Gloss |
muħ.ꞌta.ra.ma | respectable (fem.) | |
maz.ꞌra.ʕa.tuh | his farm | |
mu. ꞌbaa.ra.zeh | sword play | |
mu.raa.sa.ꞌlaat | correspondences | |
ma.daa.ꞌris.kum | your schools | |
muʃ.ta.ra.ꞌjaat | purchases | |
mu.baa.ra.ꞌjaat | competitions | |
ʔa.maa.ꞌkin.hum | their places | |
mus.taʕ.ꞌma.ra.tii | my colony | |
ʔis.tiʕ.laa.ꞌmaat.hum | their inquiries |
Data show that onsets are obligatory while codas are optional. The dialect tolerates CV, CVC, CCV, CVCC, CVV, CVVC, and CCVVC syllable structures. Within a syllable, long vowels are never followed by a CC cluster, i.e., CVVCC syllables are prohibited. Initial and final CCC clusters are also prohibited. Initial CC clusters are allowed irrespective of sonority (/ktaab/‘a book’,/rbaaʕ/‘quarters’). Final CC clusters are allowed if they have a falling sonority value (/kalb/‘a dog’). Otherwise, an epenthetic vowel will break the cluster (/ꞌʔa.kil/‘food’ from underlying/ꞌʔa.kil/). Medial CVCC syllables are permitted when the two consonants have falling sonority values (la. ꞌbint.hum ‘for their daughter’. Otherwise, an epenthetic vowel is added to break the cluster (/fi.kir.hum/‘their thought’ from/fikr.hum/) (Na’eem et al. 2020).
The data in (1) confirm the stress assignment rules in JA. A preantepenultimate syllable is never stressed. This leaves the last three syllables from the right edge as stress bearers in the dialect. A word-final superheavy syllable is stressed. If one is not found, a penultimate heavy syllable is stressed. Otherwise, the antepenultimate syllable receives stress by default.
2.2 Active constraints
Accounting for the stress patterns in (1), the following constraints are assumed active in the dialect.[3]
(2) Active constraints
a. Trochaic (TF) | Prince and Smolensky (1993/2004) | |
Feet are left headed | ||
b. WSP | Prince and Smolensky (1993/2004) | |
Heavy syllables are stressed. | ||
c. FOOT BINARITY (FTBIN) | Prince and Smolensky (1993/2004) | |
Feet are binary at some level of analysis (μ, σ) | ||
d. NONFINALITY (NONFIN) | Prince and Smolensky (1993/2004) | |
No head of Prosodic Word is final in Prosodic Word *ꞌF *ꞌσ | ||
e. EDGEMOST | ||
A peak of prominence lies at the right edge of the word. (Prince and Smolensky, 1993/2004) | ||
f. PARSE σ > μμ | Al-Jarrah (2008) | |
A syllable that weighs more than two moras must be parsed into a higher prosodic constituent | ||
g. PARSE σ ≤ μμ | Al-Jarrah (2008) | |
A syllable that weighs two moras or less must be parsed into a higher prosodic constituent | ||
h. LAPSE-R | Al-Jarrah (2008) | |
Successive unparsed syllables at the right edge of a word in not allowed |
All feet are trochaic, and the following constraint hierarchy holds:
(3) Constraint hierarchy
FTBIN ≫ PARSE σ > μμ ≫ LAPSE ≫ WSP ≫ NONFIN ≫ PARSE σ ≤ μμ ≫ EDGEMOST
The interaction of the constraints accounts for all stress patterns in the data in (1) as exemplified in tableaux (4–12) all of which are self-explanatory.
(4) LꞌSH
Input: damaar | FTBIN | WSP | PARSE σ > μμ | NONFIN(ꞌF, ꞌσ) | PARSE σ ≤ μμ |
a. ☞da(ꞌmaar) | *ꞌF and * ꞌσ | * | |||
b. (ꞌda.maar) | *! | *ꞌF | |||
c. (ꞌda)maar | *! | * | * |
Candidates (4b,c) violate the higher-ranked WSP and FTBIN, respectively, while candidate (4a) satisfies both and is thus optimal.
(5) HHꞌSH
Input:/miʤ.tam.ꞌʕiin/ | FTBIN | WSP | PARSE σ > μμ | NONFIN | PARSE σ ≤ μμ |
a. ☞ miʤ.tam(ꞌʕiin) | *ꞌF and *ꞌσ | ** | |||
b. miʤ(ꞌtam)ʕiin | !* | ** | |||
c. (ꞌmiʤ)tam.ʕiin | !* | * |
Candidate (5a) is optimal since a final superheavy syllable is stressed. The other two candidates fail to parse the final syllable which has more than two moras.
(6) LꞌHL
Input:/fi.him.na/ | FTBIN | WSP | PARSE σ > μμ | NONFIN | PARSE σ ≤ μμ |
a. ☞ fi(ꞌhim)na | ** | ||||
b. fi(him.ꞌna) | *! | *ꞌF and *ꞌσ | * | ||
c. fi. (ꞌhim.na) | *ꞌF | ** | |||
d. (ꞌfi.him)na | *! | * |
A heavy penultimate syllable is stressed in (6a). The closest rival is (6b) which loses due to a violation of NONFIN since a final foot is stressed.
(7) HꞌHH
Input: xaatimhum | FTBIN | WSP | PARSE σ > μμ | NONFIN | EDGEMOST | PARSE σ ≤ μμ |
a.☞xaa(ꞌtim)hum | * | ** | ||||
b. (ꞌxaa).tim.hum | **! | ** | ||||
c. xaa(ꞌtim. hum) | * ꞌF | * | ||||
d. xaa.tim. (ꞌhum) | * ꞌF | ** |
With multiple heavy syllables, EDGEMOST will determine the winning candidate. Candidates (7a, b) are equally optimal without EDGEMOST which will favor ((7a).
(8) ꞌHLL
Input: maktabi | FTBIN | LAPSE | WSP | NONFIN | EDGEMOST |
a.☞(ꞌmak.ta)bi | ** | ||||
b. mak(ꞌta.bi) | *! | *ꞌF | * | ||
c. (ꞌmak).ta.bi | *! | ** |
Candidate (8a) is optimal with a disyllabic foot. The closest rival is (8c) which is ruled out by a fatal violation of LAPSE.
(9) LLL
Input: samaka | FTBIN | WSP | PARSE σ > μμ | NONFIN | PARSE σ ≤ μμ | EDGEMOST |
a.☞ (ꞌsa.ma)ka | * | ** | ||||
b.sa(ꞌma.ka) | *ꞌF | * | * |
With all light syllables, NONFIN will favor (9a).
(10) HꞌLLL
Input: muħtarama | LAPSE | WSP | NONFIN | EDGEMOST |
a.☞muħ(ꞌta.ra)ma | * | σσ# | ||
b. (ꞌmuħ.ta)rama | *! | |||
c. muħ.ta(ꞌra.ma) | *! | * | *ꞌF | σ# |
LAPSE is crucially ranked over WSP to favor (10a) over (10b).
(11) HHꞌH
Input: miʤtamʕiin | PARSE σ > μμ | LAPSE | WSP | NONFIN | PARSE σ ≤ μμ |
a.☞miʤ.tam(ꞌʕiin) | * | *ꞌF and *ꞌσ | ** | ||
b. miʤ(ꞌtam)ʕiin | !* | * | |||
c. (ꞌmiʤ)tam.ʕiin | !* | * | * |
The need to parse syllables with more than two moras into feet is crucially ranked higher than NONFIN.
(12) HHꞌLLL
Input: mustaʕmarati | PARSE σ > μμ | LAPSE | WSP | NONFIN | PARSE σ ≤ μμ |
a.☞mus.taʕ(ꞌma.ra)ti | * | *** | |||
b. mus.(ꞌtaʕ.ma)ra.ti | !* | *** | |||
c. mus.taʕ.ma(ꞌra.ti) | * | !*ꞌF | *** |
Longer words further establish the need to rank LAPSE higher than WSP. In candidate (12b), a heavy syllable is stressed but leaves two unparsed syllables at the right edge of a word. Candidate (12a) is optimal although a heavy syllable is not stressed. Unparsed syllables at the left edge are tolerated in the dialect.
Thus far we have seen that in JA stress falls on one of the last three syllables. The final syllable is stressed only if it is superheavy. This is a result of having two PARSE constraints targeting syllables with more than two moras and syllables with two or less moras. These two constraints are split by a NONFINALITY constraint that penalizes final stressed syllables and final stressed feet (5–7). It was also essential to introduce a LAPSE constraint which penalizes two successive unparsed syllables at the right edge of the word. This constraint is dominated by PARSE σ > μμ (11).
3 Problematic data
Despite the discussion presented in the previous section, JA still exhibits stress patterns that require further stipulations. Consider data in (13):
(13) Opaque stress
Input | Output | Gloss | |
a | saaʕad-at | ꞌsaa. ʕa.dat | she helped |
saʔal-ku | sa. ꞌʔal.ku | he asked you (pl.) | |
katab-na | ka. ꞌtab.na | we wrote | |
ʔakal-na | ʔa. ꞌkal.na | we ate | |
gaawam-at | ꞌgaa.wa.mat | she resisted | |
b | bint-na | ꞌbi.nit.na | our daughter |
ʔakl-na | ꞌʔa.kil.na | our food | |
ʔi.sm.na | ꞌʔi.sim.na | our name | |
c | gaawam-t | gaa.ꞌwa.mit | I resisted |
saa.ʕad-t | saa.ꞌʕa.dit | I helped |
Data in (13a) are accounted for by the constraint hierarchy established thus far for JA as exemplified in (14 keeping in mind that feet in JA are trochaic (TF))
(14) katab-na
katab-na | TF | PARSE σ > μμ | LAPSE | WSP | NONFIN | PARSE σ ≤ μμ |
a.☞ka(ꞌtab).na | ||||||
b.(ꞌka.tab)na | !* | ** | ||||
c.(ka.ꞌtab)na | !* |
However, data in (13b, c) cannot be accounted for by the hierarchy established so far. In (13b), a light antepenultimate syllable is stressed where a heavy penultimate syllable is optimized by the hierarchy as (15) shows:
(15) Incorrect optimal candidate for ʔism-na
ʔism-na | PARSE σ > μμ | LAPSE | WSP | NONFIN | PARSE σ ≤ μμ |
a.☞ ʔi. (ꞌsim).na | ** | ||||
b. (ꞌʔi.sim).na | !* | ** |
A suboptimal (15a) wins over the optimal (15b).
In (13c), a light penultimate syllable is stressed where an antepenultimate syllable is optimized by the hierarchy as shown in (16):
(16) Incorrect optimal candidate for gaawam-t
gaawam-t | PARSE σ > μμ | LAPSE | WSP | NONFIN | PARSE σ ≤ μμ |
a.☞ (ꞌgaa.wa).mit | * | ||||
b. gaa. (ꞌwa.mit) | !* | * | * |
A suboptimal (16a) wins over the optimal (16b).
The constraint hierarchy will predict the correct optimal candidate only if the epenthetic vowel is ignored for the purposes of stress assignment. In other words, what we need is a theory that allows faithfulness to the stressed vowel in a form prior to epenthesis, i.e., an intermediate stage in the derivation.
3.1 Proposals with traditional OT
Different theoretical tools have been proposed to account for the opaque interaction of epenthesis and stress. To solve the problem posed by examples in (13b), Kager (1999a, b) proposes a constraint that would ban assigning stress to an epenthetic vowel. Note that the heavy syllables in /ꞌbi.nit.na/ from /bint.na/, /ꞌʔi.sim.na/ from/ʔism-na/, and /ꞌʔa.kil.na/ from /ʔakl-na/ all have an epenthetic vowel that is inserted to break up an otherwise illicit consonant cluster. The constraint proposed by Kager is formalized in (17):
(17) HEAD-DEP(IO)
Every vowel in the output prosodic head has a correspondent in the input.
What this constraint demands is for every stressed vowel in the output there must be a corresponding vowel in the input. This constraint is responsible for ruling out output form like /bi.ꞌnit.na/ since the stressed vowel in the heavy syllable is epenthetic and thus has no correspondence in the input. This constraint must necessarily dominate WSP as shown in (18) for /ʔism.na/:
(18) HEAD-DEP(IO) and /bint.na/
ʔism-na | PARSE σ > μμ | LAPSE | Head-Dep I/O | WSP | NONFIN | PARSE σ ≤ μμ |
a. ʔi. (ꞌsim).na | !* | ** | ||||
b. ☞ (ꞌʔi.sim).na | * | ** |
Although appealing HEAD-DEP(IO) will not be able to account for the stress pattern in (13c) since stress does not fall on the syllable with the epenthetic vowel. To solve the cases of opaque stress encountered in (13c), we need stress assignment to disregard the epenthetic vowel as a whole and not simply avoiding assigning stress to such vowels. In /ʔa.ꞌka.lit/ and /gaa.ꞌwa.mit/, disregarding the epenthetic vowel means that we have a final heavy syllable in both examples. According to the stress assignment rules in JA, these heavy vowels will attract stress. What we need then is a mechanism formalized as a constraint that would trigger a correspondence relation between the actual output and a related form of the output before epenthesis, i.e., what we need is a correspondence relation between /ʔa.ꞌka.lit/ and /gaa.ꞌwa.mit/ on the one hand and /ʔa.ꞌkalt/ and /gaa.ꞌwamt/ on the other, respectively. This correspondence relation will have to preserve the stressed vowel between the two output forms. The forms /ʔa.ꞌkalt/ and /gaa.ꞌwamt/ cannot be called bases of the output forms since they are not found as separate output forms in the dialect.
McCarthy (1997, 1999) proposes a theory of Sympathy to account for the above opaque cases of stress. Sympathy constraints require faithfulness to a failed candidate ‘sympathy candidate’ marked by (), which is defined as the optimal candidate that obeys a designated faithfulness constraint called ‘the selector constraint’ marked by (✬). The selector constraint is language-specific. Faithfulness then plays two roles in the theory of Sympathy. The failed candidate, which is the object of sympathy, is selected by an IO faithfulness constraint. And this candidate’s effect on the outcome is mediated by inter-candidate faithfulness (McCarthy 1999, 336).
Relating sympathy theory to our data in (13c), what we need is a correspondence relation that would be responsible for retaining the stressed syllable in the input forms before epenthesis could take place, i.e., we need a correspondence relation between /ʔa.ꞌkal-t/ and /gaa.ꞌwam-t/ on the one hand and the actual output forms on the other. In his development of an argument against sympathy, Kiparsky (2000) suggests that to account for opaque stress in Arabic, it would be necessary to introduce IDENT-STRESS that requires faithfulness to a candidate that has no epenthesis (/ʔa.ꞌkalt/ and /gaa.ꞌwamt/. These are the candidates that satisfy the selector constrain that bans epenthesis (19):
(19) ✬IDEP-IO (V)
Every vowel in the output has a corresponding vowel in the input (no epenthesis).
IDENT-STRESS necessarily dominates WSP as tableaux (20–21) exemplify, where a constraint against complex margins (*CM) is necessary to prevent the sympathetic candidate from surfacing.
(20)
Input: ʔakl-na | *CM |
![]() |
WSP | ✬IDEP-IO (V) |
a. ☞ ꞌʔa.kil.na | * | * | ||
b. ʔa.ꞌkil.na | *! | * | ||
c. ![]() |
*! |
According to (20), candidate (20a) surfaces as the optimal output since it satisfies the dictates of the IDENT-STRESS in having its stress on the syllable that corresponds to the syllable in the sympathetic candidate /ꞌʔakl.na/ which was chosen by the selector constraint IDEP-IO (V). The closest rival to (20a) is (20b) which loses despite its satisfaction of WSP.
(21)
Input: ʔakal-t | *CM |
![]() |
WSP | ✬IDEP-IO (V) |
a. ☞ ʔa.ꞌka.lit | * | |||
b. ꞌʔa.ka.lit | *! | * | ||
c. ![]() |
*! |
According to (21), candidate (21a) surfaces as the optimal output since its closest rival (21b) violates the higher-ranked IDENT-STRESS by having stress on the first syllable rather than the penultimate syllable which corresponds to the stressed syllable of the sympathetic candidate (21c) which itself is ruled out due to a fatal violation of the higher ranked *CM. It is worth noting here that candidate (21b) bests the optimal candidate (21a) in terms of the dictates of NONFINALITY. This suggests that
IDENT-STRESS must dominate the NONFINALITY constraint. Candidate (21a) has a final stressed foot while (21b) does not violate this constraint. In terms of the EDGEMOST constraint, the optimal candidate in (21a) bests its rival (21b) since only one syllable separates the stressed syllable in (21a) from the right edge of the word, while two syllables separate the stressed syllable in (21b) from the right edge of the word.
Sympathy theory was severely criticized by Idsardi (1997) and Kiparsky (2000). Idsardi (1997) notes that the theory suffers several severe shortcomings. First, the theory cannot handle the normal application of some processes. Second, chaos ensues when sympathy is added to OT; non-problematic data suddenly become problematic. This has an adverse effect on learnability. Third, adding sympathy vastly increases the grammar space in unpredictable ways. Kiparsky (2000) notes that sympathy misses the generalization that epenthesis is invisible to all word phonology. The same Selector constraint will choose a different sympathy candidate to handle other processes that involve epenthesis. Kiparsky (2006) also shows that sympathy predicts non-occurring types of constraint interactions (such as mutual non-bleeding), that it cannot characterize certain actually occurring types of constraint interactions, and that it is incompatible with Richness of the Base.
What both Head-Dep and Sympathy require is faithfulness to a feature in a sub-optimal candidate. This idea is developed in HS.
4 Harmonic serialism
4.1 Theoretical background
In Classic Parallel Optimality Theory (P-OT) (Prince and Smolensky 1993/2004, McCarthy and Prince 1993a, b), the generative component GEN produces the candidate set. The set of candidates is infinite because candidates can differ from the input in many different ways; which means one or more operations can be applied to a given input to produce each candidate. Harmonic Serialism (HS) (McCarthy 2000, 2002, 2006, 2010a, b, 2016) is a serial take on the traditional (P-OT), fundamentally different in the idea of gradualness. In P-OT, Gen is able to create candidates that are different from the input and from each other in many ways. All of these candidates are then submitted to EVAL to optimize one of them based on the hierarchy of constraints active in the dialect. HS reduces the power of GEN so that only candidates that are minimally different from the input are produced. These candidates are then evaluated by EVAL to pick an optimal candidate. Every candidate deviates from the input in only one property. Each pass through GEN and EVAL is called a step. The candidate set at each step includes the unchanged input to that step and all of the ways of making a single change in that input. This optimal candidate will then become the input of the same constraint hierarchy and a new set of candidates will be produced by GEN with only a single deviation from the new input. These new candidates are then evaluated by the constraint hierarchy and an optimal candidate will be chosen in this new step. This will continue until the optimal candidate chosen by a step can no longer be modified as it is identical to the actual final optimal candidate. This is when the derivation is said to converge.
What this process achieves is gradual harmony. After each step, the optimal candidate is more similar to the final optimal candidate. In any given step, there can be only one unfaithful operation but as many faithful operations as possible. A general outline of HS is in (22)
(22) Structure of HS
A set of output candidates is generated by GEN. These candidates are minimally different from the input and from each other. A set of ranked constraints will pick an optimal candidate in what is called (step1). If this optimal candidate is identical to the final output in the language, convergence takes place and the derivation is complete. If the optimal candidate in (step 1) is not identical to the final output, it re-enters GEN and a new set of candidates is generated and evaluated by the constraint set in what is called (step 2). These passes from GEN to EVAL continue until the output of the step is identical to the final output. The optimal candidate chosen in a step will always be more harmonic to the final output compared to the previous step.
4.2 Application of HS to JA opaque stress
Opaque stress in JA (13b, c) results from the need to assign stress before epenthesis. In other words, epenthesis is intrinsically ordered after stress assignment. Two processes are intrinsically ordered if the proper applicability of one depends on the prior application of the other (McCarthy 2016). In HS, this occurs when the markedness constraint implicated in the second process (epenthesis) is not violated until the first process (stress) has been applied. Epenthesis in JA is needed to break illicit coda clusters (*COMPLEX CODA, *COMPCoda) in violation of DEP-IO which militates against epenthesis.
Opaque stress in (13b) involves two steps before convergence (23.1-3). The first step creates feet and assigns stress. The second splits the illicit coda cluster before convergence.
(23.1) bint-na ⇨ ꞌbi.nit.na step 1
/bint-na/ | PARSE σ > μμ | LAPSE | *COMPCoda | WSP | NONFIN | PARSE σ ≤ μμ | DEP-IO |
a. ☞ (ꞌbint).na | * | * | |||||
b. (ꞌbint.na) | * | !* | |||||
c. bint.na | * | !* | ** |
The first pass through GEN and EVAL creates feet and assigns stress which could actually be split into two steps. The first creates feet and the other assigns stress. Accordingly, (23.1a) is chosen as the intermediate optimal candidate. Candidates with an epenthetic vowel (ꞌbi.nit).na or bi.(ꞌnit).na are not produced by GEN since they involve two changes from the input: foot formation and epenthesis.
(23.2) bint-na ⇨ ꞌbi.nit.na step 2
(ꞌbint).na | PARSE σ > μμ | LAPSE | *COMPCoda | WSP | NONFIN | PARSE σ ≤ μμ | DEP-IO |
a. ☞ (ꞌbi.nit).na | * | * | |||||
b. (ꞌbint.na) | !* | !* |
From step 1, (ꞌbint).na becomes the input of step 2. Foot structure and stress are already part of the input. Changes in foot structure and stress location will no longer be suggested by GEN as potential candidates. The hierarchy will only make one change to this new input which is epenthesis. Candidate (23.2a) is optimal since it satisfies *COMPCoda.
(23.3) bint-na ⇨ ꞌbi.nit.na step 3 (convergence)
/ꞌbi.nit.na/ | PARSE σ > μμ | LAPSE | *COMPCoda | WSP | NONFIN | PARSE σ ≤ μμ | DEP-IO |
a. ☞ (ꞌbi.nit).na | * | * | |||||
b. ꞌbint.na | !* |
The steps converge, and the input is identical to the optimal output. No more gradual harmony is possible.
Opaque stress in (13c) also involves the same two steps before convergence (24.1-3).
(24.1)/gaawam-t/ ⇨ gaa.(ꞌwa.mit) step 1
gaawam-t | PARSE σ > μμ | LAPSE | *COMPCoda | WSP | NONFIN | PARSE σ ≤ μμ |
a.☞ gaa.(ꞌwamt) | * | * | ** | * | ||
b. (ꞌgaa).wamt | !* | * | * | * | * | |
c. gaa.wamt | !* | * | ** | * |
The first pass through GEN and EVAL creates feet and assigns stress. Candidate (24.1a) is the intermediate optimal output since the competitors violate the higher-ranked PARSE σ > μμ. Again, GEN does not produce candidates with epenthetic vowels since that will involve two changes to the input.
(24.2)/gaawam-t/ ⇨ gaa.(ꞌwa.mit) step2
gaa.(ꞌwamt) | PARSE σ > μμ | LAPSE | *COMPCoda | WSP | NONFIN | PARSE σ ≤ μμ | DEP-IO |
a. ☞ gaa.(ꞌwa.mit) | * | * | * | * | |||
b. gaa.(ꞌwamt) | !* | ** | * |
From step (1), gaa.(ꞌwamt) becomes the input of step (2) with stress already assigned. Epenthesis will split the coda cluster, and (24.2a) is the optimal candidate.
(24.3)/gaawam-t/ ⇨ gaa.(ꞌwa.mit) step 3
gaa.(ꞌwa.mit) | PARSE σ > μμ | LAPSE | *COMPCoda | WSP | NONFIN | PARSE σ ≤ μμ | DEP-IO |
a.☞ gaa.(ꞌwa.mit) | * | * | * | * | |||
b. gaa.(ꞌwamt) | !* | ** | * |
The steps converge, and the input is identical to the optimal output. No more gradual harmony is possible.
The fact that in JA stress assignment takes place before epenthesis implies that some other languages will have the opposite ranking. In Egyptian Arabic (Farwaneh 1995), stress epenthesis interaction is opposite to that in JA. Epenthesis in Egyptian takes place prior to stress assignment.
(25) Egyptian Arabic (Farwaneh 1995, 134): Penultimate stress
/madrasa/ | mad.rá.sa | school |
/martaba/ | mar.tá.ba | mattress |
In words with sequences of three medial consonants (CCC), a vowel is epenthesized following C2. If the epenthetic vowel is penultimate, it is stressed just like an underlying vowel:
(26) Egyptian Arabic: Epenthesis in medial CCC clusters (Farwaneh 1995, 135)
/bint-na/ | bin.ꞌti.na | our daughter |
/ʔardʕ-na/ | ʔar.ꞌdʕi.na | our land |
/katabt-lu/ | ka.tab.ꞌti,lu | I wrote to him |
In (26), a syllable with an epenthetic vowel is stressed in support of the fact that epenthesis takes place before stress assignment.
5 Conclusion
Stress–epenthesis interaction in JA results in opaque surface stress patterns. Two cases were under investigation in this article. Stressing an antepenultimate syllable where a heavy penultimate is expected, and stressing a light penultimate syllable where an antepenultimate syllable is expected to receive stress. Both cases involve an epenthetic vowel required by the dialect to break illicit coda clusters. Reference to an intermediate stage where stress is assigned prior to epenthesis was critical for a proper account of these cases of opaque stress. Harmonic Serialism is outlined and proven adequate for a proper account for opaque stress in JA. The theory of HS is a crossover between traditional parallel OT and derivational, rule-based theory that is still gaining grounds particularly due to its ability to account for opaque structures.
-
Funding information: The authors state no funding involved.
-
Author contributions: All authors have accepted responsibility for the entire content of this manuscript and consented to its submission to the journal, reviewed all the results, and approved the final version of the manuscript. KA and AA prepared the data and literature review. KA suggested the theoretical framework and prepared the first draft. KA and AA prepared the final version of the manuscript.
-
Conflict of interest: The authors state no conflict of interest.
References
Abdel-Jawad, Hassan. 1986. “The Emergence of an Urban Dialect in the Jordanian Urban Centers.” International Journal of the Sociology of Language 16: 53–63. 10.1515/ijsl.1986.61.53.Suche in Google Scholar
Abu-Abbas, Khaled. 2003. “Topics in the Phonology of Jordanian Arabic: An Optimality Theory Perspective.” PhD diss., University of Kansas. https://www.proquest.com/openview/4781eda16d550bab4c1f5493504934c0/1?pqorigsite=gscholar&cbl=18750&diss=y.Suche in Google Scholar
Abu-Abbas, Khaled. 2008. “Introducing Weight-Sensitive Edgemost.” SKY Journal of Linguistics 21: 11–36. https://www.linguistics.fi/julkaisut/SKY2008/AbuAbbas_NETTIVERSIO.pdf.Suche in Google Scholar
Abu-Abbas, Khaled. 2024. “Free Variation in Epenthesis and Syncope in a Jordanian Arabic Dialect: An Optimality Theory Perspective.” Jordan Journal of Modern Languages and Literatures 16 (1): 147–60. 10.47012/jjmll.16.1.8.Suche in Google Scholar
Abu Ain, Noora. 2016. “A Sociolinguistic Study in Saḥam, Northern Jordan.” PhD diss., University of Essex. https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:135284361.Suche in Google Scholar
Al-Ghazu, Mohammad. 1987. “Syncope and Epenthesis in Levantine Arabic: A Non-Linear Approach.” PhD. diss., University of Illinois, Urbana. https://www.proquest.com/openview/016f9746df456e8319fbe003f6e8ce08/1?pq-origsite=gscholar&cbl=18750&diss=y.Suche in Google Scholar
Al-Harahsheh, Ahmad. 2014. “Language and Gender Differences in Jordanian Spoken Arabic: A Sociolinguistics Perspectives.” Theory and Practice in Language Studies 4 (5): 872–82. 10.4304/tpls.4.5.872-882.Suche in Google Scholar
Al-Jallad, Ahmad. 2018. “The Earliest Stages of Arabic and its Linguistic Classification.” In The Routledge Handbook of Arabic Linguistics, edited by Elabbas Benmamoun and Reem Bassiouney, 315–31. Routledge. https://www.academia.edu/18470301/Al_Jallad_2018_The_earliest_stages_of_Arabic_and_its_linguistic_classification?auto=download.Suche in Google Scholar
Al-Jarrah, Rasheed. 2002. “An Optimality-Theoretic Analysis of Stress in the English of Native Arabic Speakers.” PhD diss., Ball State University. https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:60706939.Suche in Google Scholar
Al-Jarrah, Rasheed. 2008. “Interaction of Weight Effects with Extrametricality in Cairene Arabic: A Constraint-Based Analysis.” Jordan Journal of Modern Languages and Literature 1 (1): 43–60. https://journals.yu.edu.jo/jjmll/Issues/Vo1No1_2008PDF/INTERACTIONT[1].pdf.Suche in Google Scholar
Al-Khatib, Mahmoud. 1988. “Sociolinguistic Change in an Expanding Urban Context: A Case Study of Irbid City.” PhD diss., University of Durham. https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:131966779.Suche in Google Scholar
Alrabab’ah, Sharif. 2018. “Rural and Urban Dialects in Contact in Jordan: The Case of [tʃ] De affrication in the Rural Dialect of Irbid Suburbs.” MA thesis, University of Canterbury. https://ir.canterbury.ac.nz/server/api/core/bitstreams/841204c1-ae92-40bf-83e8-67adeda4d49c/content.Suche in Google Scholar
Al-Sughayer, Khalil. 1990. “Aspects of Comparative Jordanian and Modern Standard Arabic Phonology.” PhD diss., Michigan State University. 10.25335/es6q-5t40.Suche in Google Scholar
Benua, Laura. 1997. “Transderivational Identity: Phonological Relations between Words.” PhD diss., University of Massachusetts at Amherst. 10.1002/9780470756171.ch22.Suche in Google Scholar
Bokhari, Hassan. 2021. “On the Treatment of Super-Heavy Syllables in Arabic Dialects: An Optimality Theoretic Approach to Historical Typology.” Papers in Historical Phonology 6: 33–67. 10.2218/pihph.6.2021.6668.Suche in Google Scholar
Farwaneh, Samira. 1995. “Directionality Effects in Arabic Syllable Structure.” PhD diss., University of Utah. https://www.proquest.com/openview/2a20b87c8854ad03124ae67fbad0a6de/1?pq-origsite=gscholar&cbl=18750&diss=y.Suche in Google Scholar
Huneety, Anas. 2015. “The Phonology and Morphology of Wadi Mousa Arabic.” PhD diss., University of Salford, UK. https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:148809051.Suche in Google Scholar
Idsardi, William. 1997. “Sympathy Causes Chaos.” MS. University of Delaware. https://terpconnect.umd.edu/∼idsardi/papers/1998sympathy.pdf.Suche in Google Scholar
Kager, Rene. 1999a. Optimality Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511812408.Suche in Google Scholar
Kager, Rene. 1999b. “Surface Opacity of Metrical Structure in Optimality Theory.” In The Derivational Residue in Phonological Optimality Theory, edited by Ben Hermans and Marc van Oostendorp. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. https://api.semanticscholar.org/CorpusID:122526095.Suche in Google Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul. 2000. “Opacity and Cyclicity.” The Linguistic Review 17: 351–67. 10.1515/tlir.2000.17.2-4.351.Suche in Google Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul. 2002. “Syllables and moras in Arabic.” In The Syllable in Optimality Theory, edited by Caroline Fery and Ruben van de Vijver, 147–82. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://web.stanford.edu/∼kiparsky/Papers/syll.pdf.Suche in Google Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul. 2006. Paradigms and Opacity. Stanford: Center for the Study of Language and Information. https://press.uchicago.edu/ucp/books/book/distributed/P/bo3535536.html.Suche in Google Scholar
Na’eem, Hani, Mujdey Abudalbuh, and Aziz Jaber. 2020. “Medial Tri-Consonantal Clusters in Urban Jordanian Arabic.” Jordan Journal of Modern Languages and Literatures 12 (1): 45–56. https://journals.yu.edu.jo/jjmll/Issues/vol12no12020/Nom4.pdf.Suche in Google Scholar
McCarthy, John. 1997. “Process-Specific Constraints in Optimality Theory.” Linguistic Inquiry 28: 231–51. https://www.jstor.org/stable/4178976.Suche in Google Scholar
McCarthy, John. 1999. “Sympathy and Phonological Opacity.” Phonology 16: 331–99. 10.1017/S0952675799003784.Suche in Google Scholar
McCarthy, John. 2000. “Harmonic Serialism and Parallelism.” NELS 30: 501–24. https://core.ac.uk/download/pdf/334982125.pdf.Suche in Google Scholar
McCarthy, John. 2002. A Thematic Guide to Optimality Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511613333.Suche in Google Scholar
McCarthy, John. 2005. “The Length of Stem-Final Vowels in Colloquial Arabic.” Perspectives on Arabic linguistics XVII–XVIII, 1–26. https://core.ac.uk/reader/13602290.Suche in Google Scholar
McCarthy, John. 2006. “Restraint of Analysis.” In Freedom of Analysis, edited by Martin Krämer, Sylvia Blaho, and Patrik Bye, 203–31. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/8j31x9md.Suche in Google Scholar
McCarthy, John. 2008a. “The Serial Interaction of Stress and Syncope.” NLLT 26: 499–546. 10.1007/s11049-008-9051-3.Suche in Google Scholar
McCarthy, John. 2008b. “The Gradual Path to Cluster Simplification.” Phonology 25: 271–319. 10.1017/S0952675708001486.Suche in Google Scholar
McCarthy, John. 2009. Harmony in Harmonic Serialism. Ms., University of Massachusetts, Amherst. https://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/document?repid=rep1&type=pdf&doi=6c710e94212b38488e58c4722c5e4127c5b40283.Suche in Google Scholar
McCarthy, John. 2010a. “Autosegmental Spreading in Optimality Theory.” In Tones and Features, edited by John Goldsmith, Elizabeth Hume, and Leo Wetzels, 195–222. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter. http://works.bepress.com/john_j_mccarthy/100.Suche in Google Scholar
McCarthy, John. 2010b. “An Introduction to Harmonic Serialism.” Language and Linguistics Compass 4: 1001–18. 10.1111/j.1749-818X.2010.00240.x.Suche in Google Scholar
McCarthy, John. 2016. “The Theory and Practice of Harmonic Serialism.” In Harmonic Grammar and Harmonic Serialism, edited by John McCarthy and Joe Pater, 47−87. London, UK: Equinox Publishing Ltd. https://www.amazon.com/Harmonic-Grammar-Serialism-Advances-Optimality/dp/1845531493.Suche in Google Scholar
McCarthy, John and Allan Prince. 1993a. Prosodic Morphology I: Constraint Interaction and Satisfaction. MS. University of Massachusetts and Rutgers University. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/39730282_Prosodic_Morphology_I_Constraint_Interaction_and_Satisfaction.Suche in Google Scholar
McCarthy, John and Allan Prince. 1993b. “Generalized Alignment.” In Yearbook of Morphology, edited by Geert Booij and Jaap van Marle, 79–153. Dordrecht: Kluwer. 10.1007/978-94-017-3712-8_4.Suche in Google Scholar
Omari, Osama and Gerard Van Herk. 2016. “A Sociophonetic Study of Interdental Variation in Spoken Arabic.” Jordan Journal of Modern Languages and Literature 8 (2): 117–37. 10.47012/jjmll.14.2.1.Suche in Google Scholar
Prince, Allan and Paul Smolensky. 1993/2004. Optimality Theory. MS. Rutgers University and University of Colorado. https://roa.rutgers.edu/files/537-0802/537-0802-PRINCE-0-0.PDF.Suche in Google Scholar
Sakarna, Ahmad. 2005. “The Linguistic Status of the Modern Jordanian Dialects.” Arabica: Journal of Arabic and Islamic Studies 52 (4): 522–43. 10.1163/157005805774320231.Suche in Google Scholar
Watson, Janet. (2002). The Phonology and Morphology of Arabic. Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://books.google.jo/books?id=lLQSDAAAQBAJ&printsec=frontcover#v=onepage&q&f=false.Suche in Google Scholar
Watson, Janet. (2011a). “Word stress in Arabic.” In The Blackwell Companion to Phonology, edited by Marc van Oostendorp, Colin Ewen, Elizabeth Hume, and Keren Rice. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. https://www.academia.edu/90392137/Word_Stress_in_Arabic.Suche in Google Scholar
Watson, Janet. (2011b). “Arabic Dialects (General Article).” In The Semitic Languages: An international handbook. Handbooks of Linguistics and Communication Science, edited by Stefan Weninger, Geoffrey Khan, Michael Streck, and Janet Watson, 851–96. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/76443/8/HSKSE2_A050.pdf.Suche in Google Scholar
Zuraiq, Wael and Maisoon Abu-Joudeh. 2013. “Consonantal Assimilation in Four Dialects of Jordanian Arabic.” Studies in Literature and Language 6 (2): 73–80. 10.3968/j.sll.1923156320130602.3506.Suche in Google Scholar
© 2025 the author(s), published by De Gruyter
This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Artikel in diesem Heft
- Research Articles
- No three productions alike: Lexical variability, situated dynamics, and path dependence in task-based corpora
- Individual differences in event experiences and psychosocial factors as drivers for perceived linguistic change following occupational major life events
- Is GIVE reliable for genealogical relatedness? A case study of extricable etyma of GIVE in Huī Chinese
- Borrowing or code-switching? Single-word English prepositions in Hong Kong Cantonese
- Stress and epenthesis in a Jordanian Arabic dialect: Opacity and Harmonic Serialism
- Can reading habits affect metaphor evaluation? Exploring key relations
- Acoustic properties of fricatives /s/ and /∫/ produced by speakers with apraxia of speech: Preliminary findings from Arabic
- Translation strategies for Arabic stylistic shifts of personal pronouns in Indonesian translation of the Quran
- Colour terms and bilingualism: An experimental study of Russian and Tatar
- Argumentation in recommender dialogue agents (ARDA): An unexpected journey from Pragmatics to conversational agents
- Toward a comprehensive framework for tonal analysis: Yangru tone in Southern Min
- Variation in the formant of ethno-regional varieties in Nigerian English vowels
- Cognitive effects of grammatical gender in L2 acquisition of Spanish: Replicability and reliability of object categorization
- Interaction of the differential object marker pam with other prominence hierarchies in syntax in German Sign Language (DGS)
- Modality in the Albanian language: A corpus-based analysis of administrative discourse
- Theory of ecology of pressures as a tool for classifying language shift in bilingual communities
- BSL signers combine different semiotic strategies to negate clauses
- Special Issue: Request for confirmation sequences across ten languages, edited by Martin Pfeiffer & Katharina König - Part II
- Request for confirmation sequences in Castilian Spanish
- A coding scheme for request for confirmation sequences across languages
- Special Issue: Classifier Handshape Choice in Sign Languages of the World, coordinated by Vadim Kimmelman, Carl Börstell, Pia Simper-Allen, & Giorgia Zorzi
- Classifier handshape choice in Russian Sign Language and Sign Language of the Netherlands
- Formal and functional factors in classifier choice: Evidence from American Sign Language and Danish Sign Language
- Choice of handshape and classifier type in placement verbs in American Sign Language
- Somatosensory iconicity: Insights from sighted signers and blind gesturers
- Diachronic changes the Nicaraguan sign language classifier system: Semantic and phonological factors
- Depicting handshapes for animate referents in Swedish Sign Language
- A ministry of (not-so-silly) walks: Investigating classifier handshapes for animate referents in DGS
- Choice of classifier handshape in Catalan Sign Language: A corpus study
Artikel in diesem Heft
- Research Articles
- No three productions alike: Lexical variability, situated dynamics, and path dependence in task-based corpora
- Individual differences in event experiences and psychosocial factors as drivers for perceived linguistic change following occupational major life events
- Is GIVE reliable for genealogical relatedness? A case study of extricable etyma of GIVE in Huī Chinese
- Borrowing or code-switching? Single-word English prepositions in Hong Kong Cantonese
- Stress and epenthesis in a Jordanian Arabic dialect: Opacity and Harmonic Serialism
- Can reading habits affect metaphor evaluation? Exploring key relations
- Acoustic properties of fricatives /s/ and /∫/ produced by speakers with apraxia of speech: Preliminary findings from Arabic
- Translation strategies for Arabic stylistic shifts of personal pronouns in Indonesian translation of the Quran
- Colour terms and bilingualism: An experimental study of Russian and Tatar
- Argumentation in recommender dialogue agents (ARDA): An unexpected journey from Pragmatics to conversational agents
- Toward a comprehensive framework for tonal analysis: Yangru tone in Southern Min
- Variation in the formant of ethno-regional varieties in Nigerian English vowels
- Cognitive effects of grammatical gender in L2 acquisition of Spanish: Replicability and reliability of object categorization
- Interaction of the differential object marker pam with other prominence hierarchies in syntax in German Sign Language (DGS)
- Modality in the Albanian language: A corpus-based analysis of administrative discourse
- Theory of ecology of pressures as a tool for classifying language shift in bilingual communities
- BSL signers combine different semiotic strategies to negate clauses
- Special Issue: Request for confirmation sequences across ten languages, edited by Martin Pfeiffer & Katharina König - Part II
- Request for confirmation sequences in Castilian Spanish
- A coding scheme for request for confirmation sequences across languages
- Special Issue: Classifier Handshape Choice in Sign Languages of the World, coordinated by Vadim Kimmelman, Carl Börstell, Pia Simper-Allen, & Giorgia Zorzi
- Classifier handshape choice in Russian Sign Language and Sign Language of the Netherlands
- Formal and functional factors in classifier choice: Evidence from American Sign Language and Danish Sign Language
- Choice of handshape and classifier type in placement verbs in American Sign Language
- Somatosensory iconicity: Insights from sighted signers and blind gesturers
- Diachronic changes the Nicaraguan sign language classifier system: Semantic and phonological factors
- Depicting handshapes for animate referents in Swedish Sign Language
- A ministry of (not-so-silly) walks: Investigating classifier handshapes for animate referents in DGS
- Choice of classifier handshape in Catalan Sign Language: A corpus study