Home Individual differences in event experiences and psychosocial factors as drivers for perceived linguistic change following occupational major life events
Article Open Access

Individual differences in event experiences and psychosocial factors as drivers for perceived linguistic change following occupational major life events

  • Mason A. Wirtz EMAIL logo , Simon Pickl and Simone E. Pfenninger
Published/Copyright: February 6, 2025
Become an author with De Gruyter Brill

Abstract

The aim of this study is to investigate both quantitatively and qualitatively the impact of career-related major life events (MLEs) on patterns of reported linguistic change across the lifespan, with an emphasis on how individual differences relate to differential patterns of MLE-related change. The occupational significant life events scrutinized here include entry into the workforce, job/career change, unemployment, and retirement. We analyzed survey data from 154 German-speaking adults in Austria who experienced (at least) one of these career-related MLEs. Results from Bayesian modeling showed that individual differences in event experiences (e.g., how stressful an MLE is perceived, how damaging an MLE is for one’s social status) alongside social factors such as varietal proficiency affect the degree of perceived MLE-related change in the sociolinguistic repertoire. Qualitatively, the thematic analysis revealed that facets of the linguistic marketplace seemed responsible for occupational MLE-related linguistic change, but also socio-affective drivers such as dialect pride and career-resultant shifts in one’s social networks and contact with other dialects.

1 Introduction

The idea that significant life events along an individual’s career trajectory may be crucial drivers for linguistic change across the adult lifespan has been a reoccurring hypothesis throughout the years (e.g., Sankoff and Laberge 1978, Eckert 1997, Wagner 2012a). Surprisingly, however, only a few studies in linguistics (e.g., Riverin-Coutlée and Harrington 2022, Wirtz and Pickl forthcoming) have actually set out to explicitly measure the extent to which career-related life events impact the sociolinguistic repertoire, despite an accumulating number of claims that “targeted studies of critical turning points in the life course can contribute to the understanding of individual life span change” (Wagner 2012b, 197, see also Eckert 1997, Buchstaller 2015). In this study, we thus focus on the linguistic relevance of career-related major life events (MLEs), which, according to Luhmann et al. (2012, 594), are defined as “time-discrete transitions that mark the beginning or the end of a specific status” (see also Bühler et al. 2023), such as an individual’s first job, career changes, unemployment, and retirement. Maneuvering such life events is presumed to place specific demands on speakers regarding demeanor and language use (e.g., Buchstaller 2015) and therefore to have measurable consequences for an individual’s sociolinguistic repertoire. For example, upon entry into the workforce, the standardization pressures of the linguistic marketplace (i.e., a domain in which [non]standard language forms are regarded as useful and desirable capital for particular economic roles) governing economically active adulthood are assumed to enforce a more pronounced use of standard language (i.e., linguistic conservatism) (e.g., Labov 1966, Wolfram 1969, Trudgill 1974, Sankoff and Laberge 1978, Eckert 1997, Wagner 2012a). Retirement, by contrast, holds an important theoretical status in variationist sociolinguistics as a phase in the life course during which the prescriptive pressures of the linguistic marketplace subside (Buchstaller et al. 2017, Chambers and Trudgill 1998, Downes 1998, Cheshire 2005), often resulting in a renewed use of vernacular variants and varieties (Buchstaller 2006, Vergeiner et al. 2021). That said, not all individuals will change in the same way, to the same extent, or even for the same reasons across the lifespan (see, e.g., Bülow and Vergeiner 2021, Pfenninger et al. 2023). This naturally raises a number of issues regarding “when … change happens, why it happens, [and] to whom it happens” (MacKenzie 2017, 1).

In social and developmental psychology, it has been argued that even individuals who experience the same significant life event do not necessarily perceive it in the same way (Rakhshani et al. 2022, Schwaba et al. 2023). Consequently, a single MLE may induce different patterns of change for different individuals rather than resulting in regular cyclical intraspeaker variability (Buchstaller 2015). With respect to retirement, for example, Mechler and Buchstaller (2019) reported two fundamentally different trajectories. Some individuals evinced an increase in vernacularity in their variable realization of (ing) following their exit from the workforce, whereas others preserved the standardizing effect of the linguistic market and so exhibited an ongoing retrenchment toward the standard, depending on their previous socioeconomic and professional backgrounds. This is in line with Coupland’s (2009) argument that the effects of retirement, like so many life events, on lifespan development are shaped by individuals’ previous lived experiences. Given this, and also seeing that adults tend to present a high degree of inter- and intraindividual (sociolinguistic) variation owing to differences in career paths and lifestyle factors (e.g., Pichler et al. 2018, Mechler and Buchstaller 2019, Pfenninger and Kliesch 2023), it can be expected that individual differences will play a critical role in terms of how change-inducing an MLE is for each individual (Wirtz et al. forthcoming). This necessitates accounting for a range of individual speaker differences that may moderate the relationship between the experience of an occupational MLE and the degree of change (and also the directionality thereof) it brings about in speakers’ linguistic repertoire. By what aspects of significant life events is change effected, and for whom and under what circumstances?

The aim of this study is to investigate the impact of career-related significant life events on patterns of perceived linguistic change, with an emphasis on how individual differences relate to differential patterns of perceived MLE-related linguistic change. We focus on retrospective perceptions of change in (a) the productive use of language varieties (cross-contextual use of standard German and dialect) and (b) affective-attitudinal factors (personal dialect accommodation, dialect identity, attitudes toward standard German [see Steiner et al. 2023a, b]). Specifically, we set out to identify, by means of Bayesian regression modeling, which quantitatively captured individual differences in event experiences and (psycho-)social factors affect perceived MLE-related linguistic change. In order to expand on these findings from another angle, we employ thematic analysis (TA). The goal herewith was to scrutinize the contextual reasons for perceived MLE-related linguistic change, that is, participants’ beliefs about why career-related MLEs have influenced their linguistic repertoires, and how these beliefs vary among individuals.

On the whole, our focus on individual differences from a mixed-methods perspective allows us to propel our knowledge forward concerning which individuals report linguistic change following career-related MLEs, in what direction this change occurs, and for what reasons it occurs – questions that are still at the heart of theoretical debate in variationist sociolinguistics (e.g., MacKenzie 2017, Sankoff 2018). Methodologically also, this study sets out a new direction for variationist designs that approach lifespan linguistic change not as a result of chronological age per se, but rather as a phenomenon impacted by experiential factors complexly intertwined with the process of aging (see also related arguments in, e.g., the field of second language acquisition, Singleton and Pfenninger 2022, Pfenninger et al. 2023). Such an approach attempts to do justice to one of Eckert’s (1997) early yet unsatisfactorily answered questions regarding how changes in the linguistic repertoire are embedded in life stages and life events. We begin in the next section with a brief overview of research to date on the current sociolinguistic setting (i.e., Austria) and on the linguistic relevance of occupational life-course transitions. We then turn to a discussion of our survey methods and findings before we conclude with a discussion of the broader implications of our results for sociolinguistic research.

2 Background

2.1 Setting the sociolinguistic stage: Austria as an empirical backdrop

Austria is a German-speaking country housing two main dialect areas, namely the Bavarian and Alemannic dialect regions (Wiesinger 1983). The Alemannic-speaking parts of Austria are characterized as diglossic and are geographically in the minority (i.e., the state of Vorarlberg and a few Tyrolean villages), whereas Bavarian dialects are spoken throughout the rest of Austria. Structurally, both Bavarian and Alemannic dialect varieties diverge from standard German varieties at the level of phonology, (morpho)syntax, and lexis.

Socio-stylistically, the use of Bavarian and Alemannic dialects has been found to be a function of macro-sociological (age, gender, interlocutor, contextual in/formality, etc.) and micro-situational (subject matter knowledge, topic, etc.) factors (e.g., Ender and Kaiser 2009), as well as identity-related issues (e.g., projection of meaning via socially indexed varieties). In addition, Austrians tend to have particularly positive attitudes toward dialect varieties – for example, in the Bavarian context, dialects are associated with social attractiveness, whereas standard German speakers are often perceived as more arrogant (though also more intelligent, educated, and professional) (Soukup 2009). Results from recent panel studies conducted in the Austro-Bavarian context suggest lifespan change in the form of increased dialect use in later life in comparison to during speakers’ economically active years (Bülow and Vergeiner 2021, Vergeiner et al. 2021), but remarkable stability as concerns their attitudes, specifically their dialect identity (Bülow et al. forthcoming). Whether and how occupational MLEs, as well as associated individual differences in event experiences and psychosocial factors, are related to changes in speakers’ linguistic repertoires, however, has not seen much empirical attention.

2.2 Occupational life-course transitions and individual differences as predictors of linguistic change

Sociolinguistic theory assumes that communally stable variants (or entire language varieties) can exhibit age-grading, that is, individual variation through time, but no resultant community-wide language change, and that these patterns may recur “at a particular age in successive generations” (Chambers 2008, 180). In this vein, Wagner (2012b, 180) attested that “nonstandard variants are associated with younger and (to a lesser extent) older speakers, but not middle-aged speakers, yielding a curvilinear frequency distribution.” This behavior has often been accounted for in terms of linguistic marketplace models (Sankoff and Laberge 1978), which propose that in being subjected to the pressures of the standard language-expectant workplace during economically active adulthood, speakers may register a retreat from vernacularity and increase their rates of standard language usage (Eckert 1997, Wagner 2012a). Upon disassociation from these social pressures, for instance, at or when approaching retirement, speakers may evince a revival of vernacular variants they had suppressed during their time in the workforce (Edwards 1992, Downes 1998, Buchstaller 2006, Buchstaller et al. 2017, Vergeiner et al. 2021, Pfenninger et al. 2023). For example, Mechler and Buchstaller’s (2019) study revealed two fundamentally different trajectories after retirement: (a) The aforementioned ‘tail’ for speakers past retirement: The three working class individuals illustrated the upward trajectory generally assumed in sociolinguistic theorizing (i.e., an increase in vernacularity), as opposed to (b) the continued standardizing effect of the sociolinguistic marketplace, as observed for the two socioeconomic risers who exhibited ongoing retrenchment toward the standard language. As Riverin-Coutlée and Harrington (2022, 42) remarked, however, the “idea of the career as a potentially influential external factor has not been empirically much explored,” despite the hypotheses that it may be a highly influential factor in lifespan sociolinguistic development (e.g., Sankoff and Laberge 1978, Eckert 1997, Buchstaller 2006, Wagner 2012a, Wagner 2012b, Vergeiner et al. 2021).

Most notably in the speech sciences has longitudinal evidence for career-related linguistic change surfaced. For example, Shapp et al.’s (2014) analysis of Ruth Bader Ginsburg’s speech indicated that her occupational transition from a lawyer to Justice at the Supreme Court temporally coincided with a reduction in her use of non-standard variants of New York City English. Homing in more specifically on career as a predictive variable, Riverin-Coutlée and Harrington (2022) found that advances in public French speaker Michaëlle Jean’s career path (e.g., her career becoming more international) correlated with changes in select phonetic features. That said, while these analyses indicate that MLEs may be change-inducing, the individual case studies are limited to ‘notable individuals’ (Sankoff 2018, 307) – for example, politicians, broadcasters, and actors among other public figures for whom digitally recorded speech data is in abundance. The case studies also represent ‘extreme case[s]’ (Riverin-Coutlée and Harrington 2022, 49), and the effects found are not likely to generalize to the average adult population (i.e., who are not highly public figures). Riverin-Coutlée and Harrington (2022) thus call for future work to conceive of designs that can model the influence of career on linguistic change across a larger number of participants. In addition, previous sociolinguistic research (Chambers 2008, Wagner 2012b, Mechler and Buchstaller 2019) suggests that the degree and directionality of lifespan change is likely to correlate with speakers’ educational and professional trajectories, satisfaction with those trajectories (including subjective wellbeing), and with the standardization pressures that result from their individual life choices. This underscores the need for designs that ideally consider not only the effects of macro-sociological individual differences (gender, education, etc.) on ensuing patterns of linguistic change, but also individual differences at the level of the event (e.g., how events are differentially experienced).

Life event researchers in psychology have emphasized that even the same MLE can be (and usually is) experienced differently across individuals (Rakhshani et al. 2022, Schwaba et al. 2023) – for example, some may perceive the same MLE as more positive, others as more negative. Given this, it is necessary to account for individual differences in event experiences (e.g., Schwaba et al. 2023), also referred to as event-related characteristics (Luhmann et al. 2021), the rationale being that it may not necessarily always be the event per se that induces change, but rather how the event is experienced by the individual. Similar hypotheses are also evident in sociolinguistics. For instance, as Riverin-Coutlée and Harrington (2022) postulated, significant changes in a speaker’s social status due to an MLE – what is regarded as a consequence-focused event-related characteristic in psychology (Luhmann et al. 2021) – may induce change in an individual’s linguistic repertoire (see also Bowie 2010). In other words, an approach focusing on event-related characteristics effectively allows us to separate the effects of the event in and of itself from how it is experienced and from the consequences one incurs because of it. In general, this should facilitate more nuanced insights into which aspects of significant life events drive linguistic change as opposed to the comparatively narrow inquiry about which events are associated with change.

In addition to classic sociolinguistic influences such as gender and educational attainment, individual differences in socioeconomic status and, by extension, occupation may also influence the degree of linguistic change an individual experiences following an occupational MLE. As Mechler and Buchstaller (2019), for instance, noted, individuals with significant marketplace pressures to employ standard language may carry these patterns across the threshold of retirement. Individual speaker differences in occupational complexity, for example, manual and communicative complexity of an individual’s primary occupation, may therefore emerge as important predictors for the degree and directionality of linguistic change as a function of a career-related life-course transition.

Finally, it seems feasible that psychological factors may impact on patterns of change (Andresen 2015, Steiner et al. 2023b) or otherwise moderate the relationship between event-related characteristics and linguistic outcomes. Specifically, Bowie (2010) noted the relevance of ‘coping’ mechanisms in processes of linguistic change, for instance how an individual linguistically copes in reaction to social pressures and events. In this vein, exploring psychological resilience, that is the ability to ‘bounce back’ and cope with stressful situations (Smith et al. 2008, Chmitorz et al. 2018), in interaction with event-related characteristics appears fruitful. Concretely, there seems to be a theoretical motivation for the interaction between psychological resilience and certain event-related characteristics: Since resilience resources have been hypothesized as relating to “successful engagement with difficult events and experiences” (Ryff and Singer 2003, 21), it seems plausible to explore whether, after experiencing an occupational MLE perceived as particularly negative and/or stressful, differences in reported linguistic change among individuals with varying levels of resilience may reflect some form of linguistic coping mechanisms, as Bowie (2010) questioned.

3 This study

The data in this study are part of a larger study examining how Austrian adults, aged 18 to 76, perceive individual linguistic changes in relation to MLEs, as well as individual differences in event experiences and psychosocial factors. We are going to focus here on the individuals who reported on occupational MLEs. Importantly, the results presented here should be considered in conjunction with the analyses of the full sample (N = 701) of participants presented in Wirtz and Pickl (forthcoming), in which the goal was strictly to elucidate the interindividual effects of significant life events on patterns of language variation and change. Such a group-level analysis necessarily left open the question as to whether the identified interindividual effects may be additionally mediated by individual difference variables of sociodemographic or psychosocial nature, which gave rise to the following exploratory research questions:

  1. Which event-related and psychosocial factors predict retrospective perceptions of change after career-related MLEs among German-speaking Austrians?

  2. Which additional factors do participants identify as substantial drivers for their patterns of perceived linguistic change in relation to occupational life-course transitions?

Experimental materials and data are available on OSF (https://osf.io/46fkh/).

3.1 Research design

We integrate both quantitative and qualitative approaches by using an equal-status concurrent mixed-methods design (Johnson and Onwuegbuzie 2004), in which we place equal weight on insights generated via quantitative and qualitative approaches. The rationale for the mixed-methods approach was primarily that of expansion, that is “seeking to expand the breadth and range of research by using different methods for different inquiry components” (Johnson and Onwuegbuzie 2004, 22). Here, the quantitative analysis allows us to identify systematic sources of variation for differences in perceived linguistic change in relation to career-related significant life events. The qualitative approach gives participants an active voice to aid in advancing our current understanding of perceived MLE-related change, facilitating individualized insights into personal circumstances, attitudes, and idiosyncrasies that would otherwise be overlooked in a purely numerical analysis.

3.2 Participants

In this study, we analyze a subsample of participants from Wirtz and Pickl (forthcoming), specifically those who reported on occupational MLEs (first job, change of job, unemployment, and retirement). We removed from the full sample (a) individuals who did not report on an occupational MLE, and in addition to these (b) five individuals who did not provide information on occupational complexity, and also (c) one gender-diverse participant. The final sample pool thus comprised 154 Austrian participants between the ages of 18 and 76 (first job: n = 56; change of job: n = 55; unemployment: n = 3; retirement: n = 40), all of whom spoke German as a first language.

Figure 1 presents the sociodemographic characterization of the subsample, and Figure 2 presents the sociodemographic data disaggregated by MLE category. Given the crowdsourcing method (i.e., online experimental procedure) and thus the convenience sample, it was not possible to stratify for gender, age, or educational attainment, as is otherwise typical of sociolinguistic investigations. The distribution of participants across Austrian provinces is also uneven; this, however, does not pose a major problem given that most of Austria houses Bavarian dialect varieties and also because we focus on standard German and dialect rather than intermediate varieties, the existence of which is one of the main distinguishing characteristics between Austro-Bavarian and Austro-Alemannic dialect areas.

Figure 1 
                  Sociodemographic information about the subsample.
Figure 1

Sociodemographic information about the subsample.

Figure 2 
                  Sociodemographic information disaggregated by MLE.
Figure 2

Sociodemographic information disaggregated by MLE.

3.3 Tasks and procedure

The test battery comprised a single survey coded in LimeSurvey lasting approximately 10–20 min. We analyze here the results of six tasks, which we detail below.

Before beginning the survey, participants were informed that (a) participation was entirely voluntary, (b) the survey was anonymous and thus no information provided could be traced back to any individuals, (c) they could withdraw their consent and delete their answers at any time during the procedure, and (d) the data collected will be used purely for scientific purposes. Participants who agreed to these terms then began the survey.

3.3.1 Experiencing an MLE

From a list of 16 event categories, participants were asked to identify a single MLE that occurred within the past max. 20 years which, in their opinion, had a significant impact on their use of or attitudes toward language (varieties). Note that the choice to constrain the temporal window to within the past max. 20 years was to capture the effects of a comparatively recent MLE that contributed to shaping an individual’s current sociolinguistic repertoire. On average, participants reported on MLEs that occurred approximately 9 years (SD = 6.6) prior to completion of the survey (first job: M = 11.3, SD = 7.5; change of job: M = 9.9, SD = 6.0; retirement: M = 5.6, SD = 4.5; unemployment: M = 12.3, SD = 4.9). Correlation analyses did not indicate that the length of time between participants’ current age and the age at which they experienced the respective MLE correlated with any of the measures of perceived change outlined later in this section and is thus not considered further in this article. Finally, participants also had the opportunity to provide qualitative narratives concerning how this MLE impacted perceived patterns of linguistic change (though answering this question was not a requirement).

3.3.2 Event-related characteristics

To capture participants’ perceptions of the identified MLE, we adopted Luhmann et al.’s (2021) Event Characteristics Questionnaire, which is a dimensional taxonomy of perceived characteristics of MLEs (i.e., individual differences in event experiences). Participants were asked about characteristics of the event that they believed had affected changes in their linguistic repertoire, specifically about the following event-related characteristics: (a) challenge (the degree of stressfulness and anxiety an individual associates with an event), (b) emotional significance (the degree of emotional impact the event had on an individual), and (c) social status changes (the extent of negative changes in an individual’s social status). Responses were provided on 100-point slider scales from ‘does not apply at all’ to ‘applies completely’. These event-related characteristics were chosen because the variable use of standard German and dialect in the Austrian context has been related to differences in emotionality (e.g., Huesmann 1998, Kroisenbrunner 2015) and social status (e.g., Steinegger 1998, Ender and Kaiser 2009), and thus, the extent to which an event is perceived as inducing changes in these characteristics may correlate with shifts in the sociolinguistic repertoire as well.

In line with Luhmann et al.’s (2021) extensive item analysis procedures, Cronbach’s alpha indicated acceptable internal consistency (computed on the basis of the full sample, i.e., N = 701; challenge: α = 0.85; emotional significance: α = 0.81; social status changes: α = 0.89). Figure 3 presents the descriptive statistics for the event-related characteristics disaggregated by MLE category (note that the individual differences for unemployment are not shown, as this MLE is not investigated in the quantitative analysis due to the small sample size).

Figure 3 
                     Descriptive statistics of the event-related characteristics disaggregated by MLE.
Figure 3

Descriptive statistics of the event-related characteristics disaggregated by MLE.

3.3.3 Varietal use and affective-attitudinal factors

To capture the extent to which participants’ retrospectively perceived changes in their sociolinguistic repertoires as a result of the identified event, we adapted the approach to measuring perceived change from the Life Event Study (Schwaba et al. 2023). Specifically, participants were provided with a statement (e.g., ‘In an average week, I often use dialect with my family.’) and asked to judge whether it applies more or less as a result of the MLE. Responses were recorded on a 200-point slider scale with 5-point intervals (from −100 to 100) from ‘applies less due to the event’ to ‘applies more due to the event’, with 0 representing no change as a result of the MLE.

The aforementioned statements concerning perceived changes in cross-contextual varietal use were adopted from Steiner et al.’s (2023a, b) Dialect Standard Profile and aim to measure changes in participants’ context-dependent use of standard German and dialect (with family, friends, with oneself, while shopping, etc.) as a result of the event. Note that at the beginning of the questionnaire, participants were asked to identify their current status (e.g., in the workforce, student, retired) and later also their status before and after the MLE (e.g., whether participants were in the workforce both before and after the MLE). All participants were asked about changes in their varietal use with family, friends, with oneself, and while shopping. Items from the aforementioned questionnaires addressing varietal use with coworkers, university colleagues, and/or schoolmates were only presented if the respective context was plausible as a result of the MLE (e.g., participants who indicated first job as their MLE were not asked about changes in their varietal use with coworkers, as no comparisons in this domain could be made to before the event). We additionally measured changes in affective-attitudinal factors, specifically in personal dialect accommodation, pride in one’s own local dialect, and attitudes toward standard German, adopting the items from Steiner et al. (2023a).

The items in the respective scales were aggregated (i.e., averaged) to create the following five outcome variables:

  1. Perceived change in cross-contextual standard German usage (between 4 and 10 items, depending on a participant’s individual circumstances).

  2. Perceived change in cross-contextual dialect usage (between 4 and 10 items, depending on a participant’s individual circumstances).

  3. Perceived change in personal dialect accommodation (3 items).

  4. Perceived change in dialect identity (i.e., attitudes toward one’s dialect, such as pride in one’s own dialect) (4 items).

  5. Perceived change in attitudes toward standard German (3 items).

3.3.4 Psychosocial variables

To capture participants’ proficiency in standard German and dialect (i.e., varietal proficiency), responses to four items on informants’ productive (speaking) and receptive (listening) language proficiency on 100-point slider scales were collected, based on the following item adaptations: ‘How well do you [speak/understand] [standard German/dialect]?’ This variable was included in the subsequent models given that Austrians’ varietal proficiency has been shown to vary (Ender and Kaiser 2009); thus, the degree of MLE-related change in their cross-contextual use of a certain variety may also be contingent on their proficiency in the respective variety.

Participants also completed the German version of the Brief Resilience Scale (Chmitorz et al. 2018), which is a short six-item measure that assesses their ability to ‘bounce back’ from stress and overcome stressful, particularly negative situations. In line with Chmitorz et al.’s (2018) validation study, the items also show acceptable internal consistency (computed on the basis of the full sample, i.e., N = 701; α = 0.84). Responses were provided on 100-point slider scales from ‘strongly disagree’ to ‘strongly agree’.

Finally, participants completed a questionnaire addressing their occupation-related communicative and manual complexity. The items for these scales were based on Zacher and Frese’s (2011) scale items gauging general job complexity, which refers to “the extent to which the tasks on a job are complex and difficult to perform … work that involves complex tasks requires the use of numerous high-level skills and is more mentally demanding and challenging” (Morgeson and Humphrey 2006, 1323). In line with this definition, our scales reflect the degree to which an individual’s job requires an individual to regularly communicate with others (e.g., ‘In your (if retired, former) job: How often do you need to talk to other people to complete your work tasks?’) and/or complete manual handling tasks (e.g., ‘In your (if retired, former) job: How often do you have to carry out manual work (possibly with different materials)?’). The two scales show acceptable internal consistency (computed on the basis of the individuals taken from the full sample who reported being or having been active in the workforce N = 600; occupation-related communicative complexity, three items: α = 0.65, though this is indeed on the lower end of accepted values; occupation-related manual complexity, three items: α = 0.86).

Figure 4 presents the descriptive statistics for the psychosocial variables disaggregated by MLE category.

Figure 4 
                     Descriptive statistics of the psychosocial variables disaggregated by MLE.
Figure 4

Descriptive statistics of the psychosocial variables disaggregated by MLE.

3.4 Data analysis

3.4.1 Quantitative analysis

In order to address the first research question, we ran a series of Bayesian models using the brms package (Bürkner 2017) in R (R Core Team 2020). Specifically, we computed a model for each of the five measures of self-reported change for each MLE (excluding unemployment, given the low sample size) as a function of our set of predictor variables of interest (i.e., a total of fifteen models). Importantly, by virtue of the slider scale, our response variables were bounded by −100 and 100. To handle these data, we truncated the model, i.e., we computationally defined the lower and upper bounds as −100 and 100, respectively, to prohibit the model from making predictions outside of these bounds. The models included as independent variables the event-related characteristics (three variables), psychological resilience (one variable), varietal proficiency (two variables), occupational complexity (two variables), age at MLE (one variable), gender (contrast coded: −0.5 = men, 0.5 = women), and educational attainment (contrast coded: −0.5 = no higher education degree, 0.5 = higher education degree). In line with our discussion on the potential relationship between psychological resilience and certain event-related characteristics – specifically those capturing potential negative or stressful consequences of the event – we included interactions between resilience and individual differences in event experiences relating to the degree of stressfulness of an event and negative changes in social status. Here, resilience is interpreted as a moderator variable, as it may affect the strength and direction of the relationship between event-related characteristics and reported linguistic change. Again, the rationale is to explore whether, after experiencing an occupational MLE perceived as particularly negative and/or stressful, differences in reported linguistic change among individuals with varying levels of resilience reflect linguistic coping mechanisms.

Importantly, Bayesian models generate an entire posterior distribution for each parameter value (e.g., for the effect size of each predictor variable). In these distributions, values closer to the mode and/or mean are more probable. In addition, the inference is based on the data available, regardless of the sample size (Gudmestad et al. 2013). In other words, with smaller sample sizes, Bayesian models are more conservative because they return estimates with greater uncertainty as opposed to frequentist models which, when fitted with many predictors and little data, might generate anticonservative p values (i.e., increased Type I error rates) (see, e.g., Wirtz and Pfenninger 2023 for a discussion of this). Especially given the dense model specifications and only moderate sample size here, Bayesian approaches are advantageous. Relatedly, because linear models can be susceptible to outliers, we computed models both with and without them (values of independent variables 2.5 standard deviations above or below the mean were classified as outliers). While we report on the models without outliers, we pay particular attention to effects that withstood both modeling procedures (the results of the analyses including the outliers are available on OSF).

Finally, we defined a region of practical equivalence (ROPE) to a range of ±0.10, which designates “the range of parameter values that are equivalent to the null value for practical purposes” (Kruschke 2018, 272). We consider there to be compelling evidence for an effect when the 95% highest density credible interval (HDI; essentially, the Bayesian analog to the frequentist confidence interval) does not fall within the ROPE.

3.4.2 Qualitative analysis

Our goal with the qualitative analysis was to expand on the quantitative insights by identifying additional factors that participants perceive as substantial drivers for their patterns of linguistic change in relation to occupational life-course transitions, especially as concerns drivers relating to social influences, feeling, emotions, and so forth that are difficult to capture numerically. To this end, the qualitative open-ended questionnaire items were analyzed using MAXQDA (version: 2022; https://www.maxqda.com). We pursued an inductive reflexive TA following the guidelines proposed by Braun and Clarke (2006). First, we familiarized ourselves with the data by reading each statement separately in order to generate initial codes (in the form of single words, formulaic expressions, short phrases, etc.). Following this, we analyzed the interactions between the codes to identify patterns and relationships between codes. Finally, we collated codes into overarching themes and relevant coded extracts were translated from German to English.

We believed that the reflexive approach to TA was well-suited to the theoretical and paradigmatic assumptions of our study, as it would enable us to collect and analyze qualitative data in a manner that honored and reflected the subjectivity of participants’ perspectives. To be more specific, TA allows us, in a flexible and iterative manner, to “identify patterns within and across data in relation to participants’ lived experience, views and perspectives, and behavior and practices” and thus to understand “what participants think, feel, and do” (Clarke and Braun 2017, 297). At the same time, TA provides us with a method to “highlight similarities and differences across the data set” (Braun and Clarke 2006, 97) which, in our case, is particularly useful in order to understand how similar themes may differently affect patterns of linguistic change. Finally, the ‘reflexive’ approach to TA emphasizes a recursive and iterative process of theme development (Braun and Clarke 2021), in that one moves back and forth between phases as necessary: Given the mixed-methods nature of the study, we also paid attention to the descriptive and inferential quantitative results during theme generation, additionally searching for potential insights from participants that may aid in explaining some of the quantitative results.

4 Results

Figure 5 visualizes the descriptive statistics and between-participant trends as concerns how career-related significant life events were perceived to influence different facets of the sociolinguistic repertoire. Positive values indicate a positive directionality of perceived change (e.g., higher rates of dialect use, stronger dialect identity, more positive attitudes toward standard language), and negative values a negative directionality.

Figure 5 
               Descriptive statistics of individual-level language change within and across MLEs.
Figure 5

Descriptive statistics of individual-level language change within and across MLEs.

As regards the individual MLEs, it is clear that retirement was reported to have the most notable impact on the productive side of the coin: Retired individuals report a retreat from standard language and an increase in their cross-contextual use of dialect varieties. Entry into the workforce represented by an individual’s first job appears to be related to a perceived retrenchment toward the standard language and, at the same time, to increased accommodation, and more positive attitudes toward standard German. Changes in an individual’s career trajectory, interestingly, were not interindividually related to perceived changes in the productive repertoire; rather, participants reported both an increase in accommodation tendencies and a stronger dialect identity. With respect to unemployment, it is difficult to identify any concrete trends because of the low sample size, but it is interesting to note that all three participants reported more positive attitudes toward standard German as a result of the event.

As the gray shading in Figure 5 visualizes, the within-MLE variation is considerable, which emphasizes that no single event was perceived to influence all individuals in the same way. In order to explore what might explain this variance, we now turn our attention to the role of both quantitatively and qualitatively captured individual differences, the goal being to determine whether these aid in clearing up what additional variables may play a role in perceived MLE-related linguistic change. We present the results addressing this issue in two parts corresponding to the two research questions (Section 3).

4.1 Event-related and psychosocial predictors of occupational MLE-related linguistic change

Figures 6 (first job), 7 (change of job), and 8 (retirement) present the visual model summaries from the Bayesian models assessing the influence of event-related characteristics and psychosocial and demographic variables on reported patterns of perceived MLE-related linguistic change in the form of quantile dot plots (without outliers, see OSF for the model summaries including the outliers). These visualize the posterior probability distribution of the predictor variable’s effect size. Each dot in the figure represents a 1% likelihood of a given value. Effects that overlap with the ROPE are shaded gray, indicating non-sufficient evidence for an effect. Green and red shadings show a significant effect with a positive and negative directionality, respectively.

Figure 6 
                  Visual model summaries for the effects of individual differences on perceived linguistic change in relation to entry into the workforce (n = 47 after outlier removal; men = 10, women = 37; higher education = 37; no higher education = 10). Note: The purple-colored bars show the 50%, 80%, and the 95% HDIs (from darker to lighter). The black point displays the posterior mean, and the thin and thick black bars visualize the 98% and 66% HDIs, respectively. The red-shaded area is the ROPE set at ±0.10.
Figure 6

Visual model summaries for the effects of individual differences on perceived linguistic change in relation to entry into the workforce (n = 47 after outlier removal; men = 10, women = 37; higher education = 37; no higher education = 10). Note: The purple-colored bars show the 50%, 80%, and the 95% HDIs (from darker to lighter). The black point displays the posterior mean, and the thin and thick black bars visualize the 98% and 66% HDIs, respectively. The red-shaded area is the ROPE set at ±0.10.

Figure 7 
                  Visual model summaries for the effects of individual differences on perceived linguistic change in relation to change of job (n = 45 after outlier removal; men = 17, women = 28; higher education = 26; no higher education = 19).
Figure 7

Visual model summaries for the effects of individual differences on perceived linguistic change in relation to change of job (n = 45 after outlier removal; men = 17, women = 28; higher education = 26; no higher education = 19).

Figure 8 
                  Visual model summaries for the effects of individual differences on perceived linguistic change in relation to retirement (n = 33 after outlier removal; men = 13, women = 20; higher education = 28; no higher education = 5).
Figure 8

Visual model summaries for the effects of individual differences on perceived linguistic change in relation to retirement (n = 33 after outlier removal; men = 13, women = 20; higher education = 28; no higher education = 5).

Upon entry into the workforce, differences in perceived changes in cross-contextual use of and attitudes toward standard German are moderated by proficiency in standard German, with more proficient individuals evincing a more pronounced self-reported retrenchment toward standard language (Figure 9a and c). We also found that individuals reporting that their occupations required them to perform at least some manual tasks were more resistant to changes in patterns of accommodation (Figure 9b), though this subsample evinced overall low rates of manual job complexity (see Figure 4).

Figure 9 
                  Conditional effects of significant predictors for perceived linguistic change following entry into the workforce. Note: We superimposed the raw data onto the conditional effects plots, with each point representing a participant.
Figure 9

Conditional effects of significant predictors for perceived linguistic change following entry into the workforce. Note: We superimposed the raw data onto the conditional effects plots, with each point representing a participant.

Event-related characteristics significantly predicted perceived MLE-related change exclusively among individuals who changed jobs. Participants who perceived their change in job to be particularly damaging to their social status reported a retreat from standard language (Figure 10a) and an uptake in cross-contextual dialect use. Conversely, individuals who perceived the change in their career trajectory to be especially stressful reported a retrenchment toward standard language (Figure 10b).

Figure 10 
                  Conditional effects of significant predictors for perceived linguistic change following a change of job.
Figure 10

Conditional effects of significant predictors for perceived linguistic change following a change of job.

In the unadjusted models (i.e., including the outliers), we found two interaction effects between psychological resilience and event-related characteristics (see the visual summaries for the unadjusted models on OSF), but these effects did not withstand outlier removal. We return to this point in the discussion.

Interestingly, none of the individual difference variables significantly predicted differential patterns of perceived linguistic change following retirement.

4.2 Beliefs about individual drivers for linguistic change

We now draw on the qualitative data collected in the open-ended questionnaire items (see the Codebook on OSF) in order to expand on the previous findings from another angle. These data were subject to a reflexive TA, which reflects and honors the subjectivity of participants’ lived experiences, views, and perspectives as concerns which aspects of an MLE were particularly salient in motivating linguistic change.

Across the occupational MLEs, facets of the linguistic market manifested in participant responses. For example, standardization pressures as drivers for patterns of retrenchment toward standard German (i.e., ‘High German’) were mentioned by informants in the workforce (our translations):

  1. When I started working, I was required to speak in High German (P369_W_NÖ_First_Job).

  2. Starting work meant that I often had to speak standard German instead of colloquial language (P608_W_SBG_First_Job).

  3. Due to my job, it became necessary to speak more High German (P543_M_SBG_Change_Job).

That said, not every workplace is equally standard expectant. A plethora of work-related aspects such as job requirements and social networks will affect speakers’ linguistic conduct. For instance, a multicultural team, or at least a team with less affinity for dialects, may give rise to considerable linguistic accommodation necessities to facilitate communication among coworkers:

  4. Multicultural team, therefore, avoidance of dialect expressions (P562_W_SBG_Change_Job).

  5. Supra-regional work requires accommodation (P1366_M_TRL_Change_Job).

  6. Nobody at work would understand my dialect (P1261_M_WIEN_First_Job).

By contrast, a less standard language-conformant workspace, and especially ones in which contact with dialect is the norm rather than the exception, may be conducive to an increase in vernacularity:

  7. Administrative work with contact to parties from all over Austria (lots of telephone contact with callers from Vorarlberg to Burgenland every day, […]), one colleague with a strong Eastern Austrian dialect, another from the Rhineland (P1383_M_WIEN_First_Job).

  8. Since 2016, I have been working with people from all walks of life and different regions of Salzburg. In conversations, I realized how many different dialects are still alive in our country. As a result, I also specifically use my dialect where it is appropriate and also like to play with it (P568_W_SBG_Change_Job)

  9. Changing jobs to a company where a strong regional dialect is spoken (P1025_M_TRL_Change_Job).

10. Change of job to a much more down-to-earth industry. Fewer colleagues with a high affinity for High German, more conversations with people from rural areas (P1349_W_TRL_Change_Job).

Advances in the career path and the associated shifts in social status, communicative necessities, social networks, etc., may also play a role in shaping the (socio)linguistic repertoire and in strengthening pressures to employ standard language:

11. As a supervisor, I had to pay more attention to my choice of words and their meaning (P636_W_SBG_Change_Job).

12. Since I’ve been lecturing all across Austria, I almost only use High German. This was probably reinforced by my online seminars, too (P449_W_WIEN_Change_Job).

13. Due to my position in a working environment with many highly educated people, I am forced to speak more High German (P706_W_WIEN_Change_Job).

Importantly, shifts in social status need not always occur in an upward direction in one’s career trajectory, and perceived ‘downward’ social mobility may coincide with a potentially less standard-expectant workplace and thus facilitate (a revival of) vernacularity:

14. Most of my [previous] colleagues were from the city and spoke High German, while my colleagues at work come from rural areas, speak dialect, and have a different educational background (P549_W_SBG_Change_Job).

Respondents reporting on the impact of the retirement threshold noted a clear drop in standardization pressures associated with economically active adulthood, linking this with a retreat from the standard language toward dialect varieties. In addition, retired individuals clearly viewed this relaxation of social and work-related pressures to employ ‘pretty language’ (i.e., standard language) in a positive light:

15. The use of High German in dealing with customers has gone away (P415_M_OÖ_Retirement).

16. As a teacher, I had to speak standard language. Now I use my Pongau dialect as often as possible (P736_M_SBG_Retirement).

17. Spoke a lot of standard language at work, but no longer a compelling need in retirement (P1315_W_OÖ_Retirement).

18. Due to my retirement, I am no longer “forced” to speak High German or Standard German with colleagues and employees, which means I automatically speak a lot more dialect (P129_W_SBG_Retirement).

19. I’m no longer required to use the standard language for professional reasons (P221_W_SBG_Retirement).

20. There’s no longer any need to speak standard German (P223_W_SBG_Retirement).

21. It is no longer necessary to speak beautiful ‘written German’ (P535_W_SBG_Retirement).

Interestingly, this perceived retreat from standard language in reaction to weakening standardization pressures is not only evident for retired individuals in later life, but may manifest in general upon disassociation from a standard-expectant workplace, as one of the participants reporting on the effects of unemployment stated:

22. Unemployment, […] loss of colleagues with whom you spoke High German (P1395_M_WIEN_Unemployed).

Finally, participants also expressed socio-affective aspects that may moderate the relationship between career-related life-course transitions and individual differences in linguistic change. One particularly recurrent theme was dialect-related pride experienced during or because of a career-related event, which may account for perceived MLE-related increases in vernacularity and a strengthened sense of dialect identity:

23. In the beginning, I tried to keep work colleagues from noticing that I came from the “countryside,” but after a few years the opposite was the case, I was and am happy and proud that I can speak dialect, understand dialect, that you can tell where I come from (P536_W_SBG_First_Job).

24. Many colleagues from Germany. Greater focus on my own dialect. Became more important to me. The local ‘color’ [i.e., dialect, MW/SP/SP] is very pleasant compared to the dry, hard High German in everyday working life (P520_W_SBG_First_Job).

25. [Because I’m a] nurse in a retirement home: Old/forgotten words in dialect are reintegrated into everyday speech. You learn to appreciate your own dialect more and want to pass it on/keep it alive (P262_W_SBG_First_Job).

26. As a native of the Innviertel region, I found High German […] to be unnatural, and I am happy to now speak more cultivated dialect than in my childhood and youth, to be understood and to be accepted linguistically (P229_W_OÖ_Retirement).

27. I’m using my “learned” dialect [i.e., the first-acquired dialect, MW/SP/SP] again and I’m glad that others do this too and appreciate it (P633_M_SBG_Retirement).

5 Discussion

As Wirtz and Pickl (forthcoming) illustrated in their analysis of the interindividual effects of MLEs on perceived linguistic change, there is a considerable amount of variation in how significant life events impact the sociolinguistic repertoire, and no single event influenced all individuals’ perceptions of linguistic change to the same degree. Given this, the aim of the present study was to integratively investigate from both a quantitative and qualitative angle which individual difference variables may be useful in explaining differential sociolinguistic reactions to career-related MLEs.

5.1 RQ1: Effects of quantitatively captured individual differences on reported linguistic change

Life event researchers in psychology recognize that even the same MLE can be and typically is experienced differently across individuals (Rakhshani et al. 2022, Schwaba et al. 2023), and recent sociolinguistic evidence points in a similar direction (see, e.g., Mechler and Buchstaller’s [2019] analysis of different linguistic trajectories when approaching or after retirement). In order to deal with this, we explored individual differences in event experiences (e.g., Luhmann et al. 2021, Schwaba et al. 2023), an approach which effectively allowed us to separate the effects of the event in and of itself from how it was experienced by the participants.

We found that negative changes in social status incurred especially after a change in job correlated with a perceived retreat away from the standard language. In other words, individuals who perceived a change in their career trajectory to induce a shift down the social ladder may draw increasingly on nonstandard variation, a finding that is in line with previous empirical evidence illustrating that (perceived) social mobility may “counteract declining plasticity” (Sankoff 2018, 299) and thus turn out to be a powerful driver for patterns of (perceived) linguistic change (e.g., Beaman 2021, Riverin-Coutlée and Harrington 2022).

At the same time, we identified nuanced interaction effects between event-related characteristics and resilience resources in the unadjusted models, but these effects did not withstand outlier removal (the directionality of the effects in both modeling procedures did, however, remain the same). In the unadjusted models, we found, for instance, that in the face of a high-stress job change, more resilient individuals reported more positive attitudes toward standard German. This may be a first indication of what Bowie (2010) considers ‘linguistic coping’. That is, particularly resilient individuals may adjust their sociolinguistic repertoire more noticeably in order to better navigate high-pressure professional contexts – ones which likely go hand in hand with high workplace pressures to employ standard language. This insight, however, remains speculative, and future research with a focus on the interaction between psychological resources (e.g., resilience) and significant life-course transitions may consider addressing this issue further to determine whether the effect becomes more or less pronounced with more data.

Interestingly, entry into economically active adulthood was not reliably moderated by any event-related characteristics. Rather, our results indicate that individual differences primarily in proficiency are more predictive of individual-level perceived linguistic change. For example, individuals more proficient in standard German reported a stronger retrenchment and more positive attitudes toward standard language. While the regression analysis inherently indicates a predictor-outcome relationship, this effect is likely more circular in nature. That is, speakers who have a higher prior proficiency for the standard language are able to undergo more pronounced retrenchment, and those who have undergone more retrenchment will likely evince a higher proficiency in standard German. A similar circular pattern can be argued for their attitudes toward standard language as well. In addition, we found that individuals who reported that their job required at least some manual tasks were more stable in their accommodation practices following the MLE entry into the workforce. That said, the overall manual complexity scores were low in this subsample, which raises the question as to whether it is truly manual job complexity associated with the more stable patterns of accommodation, or whether related job components not captured in our two occupation-related scales may be manifesting here.

5.2 RQ2: Qualitative insights on between- and within-event variation in patterns of linguistic change

The TA also illustrated that a single significant life event need not impact in the same way or for the same reasons on the sociolinguistic repertoire. Let us consider the MLE entry into the workforce. The descriptive data evinced a mean increase in cross-contextual standard German use and a moderate reduction in reported rates of dialect. This general trend comes as little surprise, especially given the extensive sociolinguistic theorizing that entering economically active adulthood may coincide with linguistic retrenchment toward standard language and, at the same time, cause a retreat from vernacularity (e.g., Eckert 1997, Wagner 2012a), likely owing to the standardization pressures of the linguistic marketplace (Sankoff and Laberge 1978, see also Labov 1966, Wolfram 1969, Trudgill 1974, Bowie and Yaeger-Dror 2015). Indeed, aspects of the qualitative analysis confirm that marketplace influences are at play for many individuals, driving retrenchment toward standard language and necessitating interpersonal accommodation. However, there do seem to be several variables influential enough to mitigate the effects of typical normative forces at play during an individual’s career-active years and even nudge speakers out of the typical shift toward conservative norms. For example, job tasks placing little pressure on speakers to employ standard German and colleagues with an affinity for vernacularity contribute to a generally less standard language-conformant workplace. In addition, a conscious assertion of identity and personal pride in one’s own dialect, even in the face of high-pressure workplace norms for standard language, may give rise to higher (perceived) rates of dialectality. These findings bear parallels to Wagner’s (2012b) analyses scrutinizing the transition from adolescence into adulthood (specifically, into higher education): She found that individuals with new college networks and at prestigious universities significantly reduced their use of nonstandard variants, whereas those who retained strong neighborhood ties remained stable in their use of the nonstandard variants. Like our results, this underscores the critical role of social networks, feelings of identity and belonging, and in general of event-related individual differences (e.g., the degree of standardization expectations in an MLE-resultant context) in how perceived changes in linguistic behavior manifest in relation to a significant life-course transition.

Whereas we found different trajectories of perceived change for individuals reporting on the linguistic relevance of entry into the workforce and a change in job, retirement evinced a remarkably stable interindividual directionality of perceived change patterns, at least in the productive domain. Apart from a few select outliers, retirement was strongly associated with a self-reported increase in dialectality and retreat away from standard language, a finding which is congruent with existing panel studies on Austrian speakers (e.g., Bülow and Vergeiner 2021, Vergeiner et al. 2021). In line with previous sociolinguistic theorizing (Edwards 1992, Downes 1998, Cheshire 2005, Buchstaller 2006, Vergeiner et al. 2021), our results underscore that this perceived linguistic behavior relates to weakening societal and workplace standardization pressures, the disassociation from which was perceived very positively. The affectively positive view toward relaxed conservatism generally corresponds to the reported changes in both the productive and affective-attitudinal aspects of the sociolinguistic repertoire and harmonizes nicely with Mechler and Buchstaller’s (2019) analysis illustrating that some speakers may proudly avoid social norms relating to the use of standard language in later life.

5.3 Limitations and future directions

To begin, one major limitation of this study is our focus only on reported measures of linguistic change in relation to occupational MLEs in relatively well-educated adults. Indeed, previous work from psychology focusing on personality development emphasizes that retrospectively judged change correlates moderately to strongly with measured change (Schwaba et al. 2023), but whether the same holds for retrospective judgments of linguistic change is an outstanding question. Future work focusing on the effects of significant life events would thus do well to collect speech and attitudinal data both in real time and retrospectively in order to correlate longitudinal trajectories with retrospective self-reports of linguistic change – possibly with participants that do not meet the WEIRD criteria (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic).

An additional limitation of the present method was that the participants were required to identify one MLE which they believed had the most pronounced impact on their sociolinguistic repertoire. This obscures the fact that linguistic behavior (and, by extension, perceptions thereof) is shaped by a multitude of forces, including, for instance, sequences of MLEs, day-to-day experiences, and so forth. Given this, the perceived effects of a single MLE may be overwhelmed by the cumulative effects of other factors. In merging both quantitative and qualitative analyses, the latter offering richer insights into participants’ contextual background which would have been lost in a purely numerical analysis, we attempted to mitigate this bias, but future analyses should strive to better ascertain the interactive effects of MLEs on changes in the (socio-)linguistic repertoire.

In addition, this study is the first to our knowledge to have looked at the linguistic relevance of unemployment. Despite the small sample size for this event (n = 3), the qualitative insights in combination with the descriptive data suggest that, at the least for one individual, the disassociation from the linguistic marketplace owing to unemployment curtailed the cross-contextual use of standard language, but at the same time gave rise to more positive attitudes toward standard language. Future research may consider investigating the linguistic relevance of unemployment on patterns of linguistic change, and how (the short-term disconnect from) linguistic marketplace pressures modulate individual-level linguistic change.

Finally, we believe that the design of the current study and analysis is noteworthy in sociolinguistic lines of inquiry. As Wagner (2012b, 182) puts it, “large studies [cannot] explain the individual differences without more detailed ethnographic information, whereas the smaller studies are unable to untangle the effects of social and/or stylistic factors.” By meaningfully integrating different investigative components, we were able to explore both systematic individual difference predictors of MLE-related linguistic change without neglecting individualized insights into personal circumstances, attitudes, and idiosyncrasies. In other words, complementary methods of investigation can facilitate deeper engagement with the multilevel and multivariate analysis of complex issues involved in sociolinguistic development and allow us to “obtain data about both the individual and broader societal context and bring out the best of the qualitative and the quantitative paradigms while also compensating for their weaknesses” (Pfenninger and Singleton 2017, 5).

6 Conclusion

The aim of the present study was to investigate individual difference drivers on patterns of perceived linguistic change following career-related significant life events (first job, change of job, unemployment, and retirement), both from a quantitative and qualitative angle. Our results suggest that MLEs are experienced in different ways, and thus even the same life event need not give rise to the same patterns of linguistic change across all individuals. Inferential analyses underscored the prevalent role of, for example, social mobility and proficiency-related factors, and qualitative insights illustrated that a plethora of individual differences in marketplace societal pressures, social networks, and socio-affective variables contribute to how career-related life-course transitions differentially relate to perceived change in the sociolinguistic repertoire. We argue alongside Wagner (2012b, 197) that “targeted studies of critical turning points in the life course” provide a meaningful and very fruitful ground for the variationist enterprise to continue identifying the complex constellation of drivers of intra-speaker malleability across the lifespan.

Abbreviations

HDI

highest density credible interval

MLE

major life event

ROPE

region of practical equivalence

TA

thematic analysis

Acknowledgments

We would like to express our gratitude to all of the participants for taking the time to provide such rich data, and also to our contacts in the Austrian news outlets Salzburger Nachrichten and Der Standard for their cooperation in advertising the online experiment. We also thank the anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

  1. Funding information: Open access publication was supported by the Paris Lodron University of Salzburg Publication Fund.

  2. Author contributions: All authors accepted the responsibility for the content of the manuscript and consent to its submission, reviewed all the results, and approved the final version of the manuscript. M.W. and S.P. designed the experiment and collected the data. M.W. performed the statistical analyses, with input from S.P. and S.E.P. M.W. wrote the first draft of the manuscript with input from S.P. and S.E.P. S.P. and S.E.P. revised and provided additional notes on the manuscript. All authors commented on the manuscript.

  3. Conflict of interest: The authors state no conflict of interest.

  4. Data availability statement: The datasets analyzed during the current study are available on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/46fkh/).

References

Andresen, Liv. 2015. Persönlichkeitsspezifische Sprachvariation: eine empirische Untersuchung zum Zusammenhang von Extraversion und Nähesprachlichkeit [Personality-specific language variation: an empirical study on the relationship between extraversion and proximity linguistics]. Olms: Georg.Search in Google Scholar

Beaman, Karen V. 2021. “Identity and Mobility in Linguistic Change Across the Lifespan: The Case of Swabian German.” In Urban Matters: Current Approaches of International Sociolinguistic Research, edited by Arne Ziegler, Stefanie Edler, and Georg Oberdorfer, 27–60. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/silv.27.02beaSearch in Google Scholar

Bowie, David and Malcah Yaeger-Dror. 2015. “Language Change in Real Time.” In Handbook of Historical Phonology, edited by Patrick Honeybone and Joseph Salmons, 603–18. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199232819.013.004Search in Google Scholar

Bowie, David. 2010. “Aging and Sociolinguistic Variation.” In Aging and Sociolinguistic Variation, edited by Anna Duszak and Urszula Okulska, 29–52. Berlin and New York: De Gruyter.10.1515/9783110238112.2.29Search in Google Scholar

Braun, Virginia and Victoria Clarke. 2006. “Using Thematic Analysis in Psychology.” Qualitative Research in Psychology 3 (2): 77–101.10.1191/1478088706qp063oaSearch in Google Scholar

Braun, Virginia and Victoria Clarke. 2021. “One Size Fits All? What Counts as Quality Practice in (reflexive) Thematic Analysis?” Qualitative Research in Psychology 18 (3): 328–52. 10.1080/14780887.2020.1769238.Search in Google Scholar

Buchstaller, Isabelle, Anne Krause, Anja Auer, and Stefanie Otte. 2017. “Levelling Across the Life-Span?: Tracing the Face Vowel in Panel Data from the North East of England.” Journal of Sociolinguistics 21 (1): 3–33. 10.1111/josl.12227.Search in Google Scholar

Buchstaller, Isabelle. 2006. “Diagnostics of Age-Graded Linguistic Behaviour: The Case of the Quotative System.” Journal of Sociolinguistics 10 (1): 3–30. 10.1111/j.1360-6441.2006.00315.x.Search in Google Scholar

Buchstaller, Isabelle. 2015. “Exploring Linguistic Malleability Across the Life Span: Age-Specific Patterns in Quotative Use.” Language in Society 44 (4): 457–96. 10.1017/S0047404515000391.Search in Google Scholar

Bühler, Janina Larissa, Ulrich Orth, Wiebke Bleidorn, Elisa Weber, André Kretzschmar, Louisa Scheling, and Christopher J. Hopwood. 2023. “Life Events and Personality Change: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis.” European Journal of Personality 38 (3): 544–68. 10.1177/08902070231190219.Search in Google Scholar

Bülow, Lars and Philip C. Vergeiner. 2021. “Intra-Individual Variation Across the Lifespan: Results from an Austrian Panel Study.” Linguistics Vanguard 7 (s2): 1–11. 10.1515/lingvan-2020-0026.Search in Google Scholar

Bülow, Lars, Philip C. Vergeiner, and Dominik Wallner. Forthcoming. “Change of Language Attitudes in Real Time. Results from the Ulrichsberg Project in Austria.” In Panel Research: Methodological Challenges, Practices and Ways Forward, edited by Karen Beaman and Isabell Buchstaller. London: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Bürkner, Paul-Christian. 2017. “brms: An R Package for Bayesian Multilevel Models Using Stan.” Journal of Statistical Software 80 (1): 1–28.10.18637/jss.v080.i01Search in Google Scholar

Chambers, Jack K. 2008. Sociolinguistic Theory: Linguistic Variation and its Social Significance. 3rd ed. New Jersey: Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar

Chambers, Jack K. and Peter Trudgill. 1998. Dialectology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511805103Search in Google Scholar

Cheshire, Jenny. 2005. “Age- and Generation-Specific Use of Language.” In An International Handbook of the Science of Language and Society, edited by Ulrich Ammon, Norbert Dittmar, Klaus J. Mattheier, and Peter Trudgill, vol. 2, 1552–63. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.Search in Google Scholar

Chmitorz, Andrea, Mario Wenzel, Rolf-Dieter Stieglitz, Angela Kunzler, Christiana Bagusat, Isabella Helmreich, Anna Gerlicher, Miriam Kampa, Thomas Kubiak, Raffael Kalisch, Klaus Lieb, and Oliver Tüscher. 2018. “Population-based Validation of a German Version of the Brief Resilience Scale.” PLOS ONE 13 (2): e0192761. 10.1371/journal.pone.0192761.Search in Google Scholar

Clarke, Victoria and Virginia Braun. 2017. “Thematic Analysis.” The Journal of Positive Psychology 12 (3): 297–8. 10.1080/17439760.2016.1262613.Search in Google Scholar

Coupland, Justine. 2009. “Discourse, Identity and Change in Mid-to-Late Life: Interdisciplinary Perspectives on Language and Ageing.” Ageing & Society 29 (6): 849–61. 10.1017/S0144686X09008800.Search in Google Scholar

Downes, William. 1998. Language and Society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Eckert, Penelope. 1997. “Age as a Sociolinguistic Variable.” In The Handbook of Sociolinguistics, edited byFlorian Coulmas, 151–67. Oxford: Blackwell.10.1002/9781405166256.ch9Search in Google Scholar

Edwards, Walter F. 1992. “Sociolinguistic Behavior in a Detroit Inner-city Black Neighborhood.” Language in Society 21 (1): 93–115. 10.1017/S0047404500015050.Search in Google Scholar

Ender, Andrea and Irmtraud Kaiser. 2009. “Zum Stellenwert von Dialekt und Standard im österreichischen und Schweizer Alltag – Ergebnisse einer Umfrage. [The importance of dialect and standard in everyday life in Austria and Switzerland - results of a survey]” Zeitschrift für germanistische Linguistik 37 (2): 266–95. 10.1515/ZGL.2009.018.Search in Google Scholar

Gudmestad, Aarnes, Leanna House, and Kimberly L. Geeslin. 2013. “What a Bayesian Analysis Can Do for SLA: New Tools for the Sociolinguistic Study of Subject Expression in L2 Spanish.” Language Learning 63 (3): 371–99. 10.1111/lang.12006.Search in Google Scholar

Huesmann, Anette. 1998. Zwischen Dialekt und Standard. Empirische Untersuchung zur Soziolinguistik des Varietätenspektrums im Deutschen [Between dialect and standard. Empirical study on the sociolinguistics of the variety spectrum in German]. Tübingen: Niemeyer.10.1515/9783110918984Search in Google Scholar

Johnson, R. Burke and Anthony J. Onwuegbuzie. 2004. “Mixed Methods Research: A Research Paradigm Whose Time Has Come.” Educational Researcher 33 (7): 14–26. 10.3102/0013189X033007014.Search in Google Scholar

Kroisenbrunner, Sabrina Verena. 2015. “Dialekt in Salzburger Kindergärten [Dialect in Salzburg kindergartens].” ÖDaF-Mitteilungen 31 (1): 115–27. 10.14220/odaf.2015.31.1.115.Search in Google Scholar

Kruschke, John K. 2018. “Rejecting or Accepting Parameter Values in Bayesian Estimation.” Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science 1 (2): 270–80.10.1177/2515245918771304Search in Google Scholar

Labov, William. 1966. The Social Stratification of English in New York City. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Luhmann, Maike, Ina Fassbender, Mark Alcock, and Peter Haehner. 2021. “A Dimensional Taxonomy of Perceived Characteristics of Major Life Events.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 121 (3): 633–68. 10.1037/pspp0000291.Search in Google Scholar

Luhmann, Maike, Wilhelm Hofmann, Michael Eid, and Richard E. Lucas. 2012. “Subjective Well-Being and Adaptation to Life Events: A Meta-Analysis.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 102 (3): 592–615. 10.1037/a0025948.Search in Google Scholar

MacKenzie, Laurel. 2017. “Frequency Effects Over the Lifespan: A Case Study of Attenborough’s r’s.” Linguistics Vanguard 3 (1): 20170005. 10.1515/lingvan-2017-0005.Search in Google Scholar

Mechler, Johanna and Isabelle Buchstaller. 2019. “[In]Stability in the Use of a Stable Variable.” Linguistics Vanguard 5 (s2): 1–13. 10.1515/lingvan-2018-0024.Search in Google Scholar

Morgeson, Frederick P. and Stephen E. Humphrey. 2006. “The Work Design Questionnaire (WDQ): Developing and Validating a Comprehensive Measure for Assessing Job Design and the Nature of Work.” Journal of Applied Psychology 91 (6): 1321–39. 10.1037/0021-9010.91.6.1321.Search in Google Scholar

Pfenninger, Simone E. and David Singleton. 2017. Beyond Age Effects in Instructional L2 Learning. Revisiting the Age Factor. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.10.21832/9781783097630Search in Google Scholar

Pfenninger, Simone E. and Maria Kliesch. 2023. “Variability as a Functional Marker of Second Language Development in Older Adult Learners.” Studies in Second Language Acquisition 45 (4): 1004–30. 10.1017/S0272263123000013.Search in Google Scholar

Pfenninger, Simone E., Julia Festman, and David Singleton. 2023. Second Language Acquisition and Lifelong Learning. New York and London: Routledge.10.4324/9781003168935Search in Google Scholar

Pichler, Heike, Suzanne Evans Wagner, and Ashley Hesson. 2018. “Old-age Language Variation and Change: Confronting Variationist Ageism.” Language and Linguistics Compass 12 (6): e12281. 10.1111/lnc3.12281.Search in Google Scholar

R Core Team. 2020. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/.Search in Google Scholar

Rakhshani, Andrew, Richard E. Lucas, M. Brent Donnellan, Ina Fassbender, and Maike Luhmann. 2022. “Personality Traits and Perceptions of Major Life Events.” European Journal of Personality 36 (4): 683–703. 10.1177/08902070211045825.Search in Google Scholar

Riverin-Coutlée, Josiane and Jonathan Harrington. 2022. “Phonetic Change Over the Career: A Case Study.” Linguistics Vanguard 8 (1): 41–52. 10.1515/lingvan-2021-0122.Search in Google Scholar

Ryff, Carol D. and Burton Singer. 2003. “Flourishing: Positive Psychology and the Life Well-lived.” In Flourishing: Positive Psychology and the Life Well Lived, edited by Corey L. M. Keyes, Jonathan Haidt, and Martin E. P. Seligman, 15–36. Washington: APA.10.1037/10594-001Search in Google Scholar

Sankoff, David and Suzanne Laberge. 1978. “The Linguistic Market and the Statistical Explanation of Variability.” In Linguistic Variation: Models and Methods, edited by David Sankoff, 239–50. New York: Academic Press.Search in Google Scholar

Sankoff, Gillian. 2018. “Language Change Across the Lifespan.” Annual Review of Linguistics 4 (1): 297–316.10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011817-045438Search in Google Scholar

Schwaba, Ted, Jaap Denissen, Maike Luhmann, Chris Hopwood, and Wiebke Bleidorn. 2023. “Subjective Experiences of Life Events Match Individual Differences in Personality Development.” PsyArXiv. 10.31234/osf.io/veztu.Search in Google Scholar

Shapp, Allison, Nathan LaFave, and John Victor Singler. 2014. “Ginsburg v. Ginsburg: A Longitudinal Study of Regional Features in a Supreme Court Justice’s speech.” University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 20 (2): 149–58.Search in Google Scholar

Singleton, David and Simone E. Pfenninger. 2022. “Age.” In The Routledge Handbook of Second Language Acquisition and Individual Differences, edited by Shaofeng Li, Phil Hiver, and Mostafa Papi, 251–66. New York: Routledge.10.4324/9781003270546-21Search in Google Scholar

Smith, Bruce W., Jeanne Dalen, Kathryn Wiggins, Erin Tooley, Paulette Christopher, and Jennifer Bernard. 2008. “The Brief Resilience Scale: Assessing the Ability to Bounce Back.” International Journal of Behavioral Medicine 15 (3): 194–200. 10.1080/10705500802222972.Search in Google Scholar

Soukup, Barbara. 2009. Dialect Use as Interaction Strategy. A Sociolinguistic Study of Contextualization, Speech Perception, and Language Attitudes in Austria. Wien: Braumüller.Search in Google Scholar

Steinegger, Guido. 1998. Sprachgebrauch und Sprachbeurteilung in Österreich und Südtirol, Ergebnisse einer Umfrage [Language use and language assessment in Austria and South Tyrol, results of a survey]. Frankfurt a. M. et al.: Peter Lang.Search in Google Scholar

Steiner, Carina, Péter Jeszenszky, and Adrian Leemann. 2023a. “Variation and Change in Swiss German Agreement Morphology: Spatial, Social, and Attitudinal Effects.” Journal of Linguistic Geography 11 (1): 8–24. 10.1017/jlg.2022.12.Search in Google Scholar

Steiner, Carina, Péter Jeszenszky, Viviane Stebler, and Adrian Leemann. 2023b. “Extraverted Innovators and Conscientious Laggards? Investigating Effects of Personality Traits on Language Change.” Language Variation and Change 35 (1): 1–28. 10.1017/S0954394523000091.Search in Google Scholar

Trudgill, Peter. 1974. The Social Differentiation of English in Norwich. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Vergeiner, Philip C., Dominik Wallner, Lars Bülow, and Hannes Scheutz. 2021. “Redialektalisierung und Alter. Ergebnisse einer real-time-Studie zum age-grading in Ulrichsberg [Redialectalization and age. Results of a real-time study on age-grading in Ulrichsberg].” In Regiolekt – Der neue Dialekt? Akten des 6. Kongresses der Internationalen Gesellschaft für Dialektologie des Deutschen (IGDD) [Regiolect - The new dialect? Proceedings of the 6th Congress of the International Association for German Dialectology], edited by Helen Christen, Brigitte Ganswindt, Joachim Herrgen, and Jürgen Erich Schmidt, 101–22. Stuttgart: Franz Steiner Verlag.Search in Google Scholar

Wagner, Suzanne Evans. 2012a. “Age Grading in Sociolinguistic Theory.” Language and Linguistics Compass 6 (6): 371–82. 10.1002/lnc3.343.Search in Google Scholar

Wagner, Suzanne Evans. 2012b. “Real-time Evidence for Age Grad(ing) in Late Adolescence.” Language Variation and Change 24 (2): 179–202. 10.1017/S0954394512000099.Search in Google Scholar

Wiesinger, Peter. 1983. “Die Einteilung der deutschen Dialekte [The classification of German dialects].” In Dialektologie. Ein Handbuch zur deutschen und allgemeinen Dialektforschung [Dialectology. A handbook on German and general dialect research], edited by Werner Besch, Ulrich Knoop, Wolfgang Putschke, and Herbert Ernst Wiegand, vol. 2, 807–900. Berlin et al.: de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110203332.807Search in Google Scholar

Wirtz, Mason A. and Simon Pickl. Forthcoming. “Major Life Events as Drivers for Perceived Linguistic Change Across Adulthood.” Language Variation and Change.Search in Google Scholar

Wirtz, Mason A. and Simone E. Pfenninger. 2023. “Variability and Individual Differences in L2 Sociolinguistic Evaluations: The GROUP, the INDIVIDUAL and the HOMOGENEOUS ENSEMBLE.” Studies in Second Language Acquisition 45 (5): 1186–209. 10.1017/S0272263123000177.Search in Google Scholar

Wirtz, Mason A., Simone E. Pfenninger, Annika Rossmanith, and Lukas Zbinden. Forthcoming. “Continuity vs Cut-off Points in SLA Across the Lifespan: From Significant Life Events to Biologically Regulated Thresholds.” In Context Matters, edited by Clare Wright, Thorsten Piske, Anja Steinlein, and Pauliina Peltonen. Multilingual Matters.Search in Google Scholar

Wolfram, Walt. 1969. A Sociolinguistic Description of Detroit Negro Speech. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Zacher, Hannes and Michael Frese. 2011. “Maintaining a Focus on Opportunities at Work: The Interplay between Age, Job Complexity, and the Use of Selection, Optimization, and Compensation Strategies.” Journal of Organizational Behavior 32 (2): 291–318. 10.1002/job.683.Search in Google Scholar

Received: 2024-04-09
Revised: 2024-10-19
Accepted: 2024-11-12
Published Online: 2025-02-06

© 2025 the author(s), published by De Gruyter

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Articles in the same Issue

  1. Research Articles
  2. No three productions alike: Lexical variability, situated dynamics, and path dependence in task-based corpora
  3. Individual differences in event experiences and psychosocial factors as drivers for perceived linguistic change following occupational major life events
  4. Is GIVE reliable for genealogical relatedness? A case study of extricable etyma of GIVE in Huī Chinese
  5. Borrowing or code-switching? Single-word English prepositions in Hong Kong Cantonese
  6. Stress and epenthesis in a Jordanian Arabic dialect: Opacity and Harmonic Serialism
  7. Can reading habits affect metaphor evaluation? Exploring key relations
  8. Acoustic properties of fricatives /s/ and /∫/ produced by speakers with apraxia of speech: Preliminary findings from Arabic
  9. Translation strategies for Arabic stylistic shifts of personal pronouns in Indonesian translation of the Quran
  10. Colour terms and bilingualism: An experimental study of Russian and Tatar
  11. Argumentation in recommender dialogue agents (ARDA): An unexpected journey from Pragmatics to conversational agents
  12. Toward a comprehensive framework for tonal analysis: Yangru tone in Southern Min
  13. Variation in the formant of ethno-regional varieties in Nigerian English vowels
  14. Cognitive effects of grammatical gender in L2 acquisition of Spanish: Replicability and reliability of object categorization
  15. Interaction of the differential object marker pam with other prominence hierarchies in syntax in German Sign Language (DGS)
  16. Modality in the Albanian language: A corpus-based analysis of administrative discourse
  17. Theory of ecology of pressures as a tool for classifying language shift in bilingual communities
  18. BSL signers combine different semiotic strategies to negate clauses
  19. Embodiment in colloquial Arabic proverbs: A cognitive linguistic perspective
  20. Special Issue: Request for confirmation sequences across ten languages, edited by Martin Pfeiffer & Katharina König - Part II
  21. Request for confirmation sequences in Castilian Spanish
  22. A coding scheme for request for confirmation sequences across languages
  23. Special Issue: Classifier Handshape Choice in Sign Languages of the World, coordinated by Vadim Kimmelman, Carl Börstell, Pia Simper-Allen, & Giorgia Zorzi
  24. Classifier handshape choice in Russian Sign Language and Sign Language of the Netherlands
  25. Formal and functional factors in classifier choice: Evidence from American Sign Language and Danish Sign Language
  26. Choice of handshape and classifier type in placement verbs in American Sign Language
  27. Somatosensory iconicity: Insights from sighted signers and blind gesturers
  28. Diachronic changes the Nicaraguan sign language classifier system: Semantic and phonological factors
  29. Depicting handshapes for animate referents in Swedish Sign Language
  30. A ministry of (not-so-silly) walks: Investigating classifier handshapes for animate referents in DGS
  31. Choice of classifier handshape in Catalan Sign Language: A corpus study
Downloaded on 13.10.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/opli-2024-0037/html
Scroll to top button