Startseite Investigating willingness to communicate in synchronous group discussion tasks: one step closer towards authentic communication
Artikel Open Access

Investigating willingness to communicate in synchronous group discussion tasks: one step closer towards authentic communication

  • Shahin Nematizadeh

    Shahin Nematizadeh is a second language teacher trainer, and teaches applied linguistics and EAP courses in Ottawa, Canada. His research interests center around the psychology of language learning, L2 teacher education and student engagement, and L2 speech fluency. He also investigates individual difference variables through a complex dynamic systems perspective. His works have appeared in Canadian Modern Language Review, the Psychology for Language Learning, and Language Testing journals.

    und Yiqian (Katherine) Cao

    Yiqian Katherine Cao is a lecturer at University of Melbourne Trinity College. She holds a PhD from University of Auckland. Her current research interests are individual differences in language acquisition, interactionist approach to language learning, blended learning and teacher professional development. Her works include papers published in TESOL Quarterly, System, Computer Assisted Language Learning, RELC Journal, Sage Open, Cogent Education and Australian Review of Applied Linguistics.

    EMAIL logo
Veröffentlicht/Copyright: 21. April 2023

Abstract

This study employed a mixed-method approach to investigate L2 WTC as a dynamic variable in synchronous group discussion tasks. Data were collected from a group of Farsi-speaking ESL students in six online discussion tasks. The participants rated their WTC level every 5 min during the discussions and then provided explanations for changes in WTC in follow-up written stimulated recall procedures. The results indicate that participants’ WTC levels fluctuate significantly during the online discussion tasks, lending support to viewing online WTC as a fluid dynamic variable. The results also show that WTC changes are triggered by complex interactions between a range of internal and external forces. Pedagogical implications and directions for future research are discussed.

1 Introduction

Willingness to communicate (WTC) in a second language (L2) is a complex construct in the fields of Second Language Acquisition (SLA) and positive psychology (MacIntyre and Wang 2021). As a behavioural intention and the final step to the frequency of communication, L2 WTC is perceived to play a crucial role in L2 learning (MacIntyre et al. 1998). To date, the L2 WTC construct has been extensively examined as a trait or dynamic situational variable (Cao 2014; Yashima et al. 2016), but previous research has mainly focused on this dual characteristic of L2 WTC in face-to-face conversational or classroom settings, but less on digital communication situations (Lee and Lee 2020). This study is one of the first attempts at employing the Complex Dynamic System (CDS) theory and using an idiodynamic method to identify the WTC shifts in synchronous group interaction. The main purpose of the study is to better understand the dynamic and situated nature of L2 WTC in synchronous group discussion tasks by employing a mixed-methods approach to examining dynamic fluctuations of WTC among six Farsi-speaking ESL speakers.

1.1 Willingness to communicate

The notion of WTC was originally introduced with reference to first language (L1) communication to explain why some people are more willing to talk than others (McCroskey and Baer 1985), based on Burgoon’s (1976) work on unwillingness to communicate. As a “non-observable psychological construct” (Peng 2020, p. 143), WTC entails an intention to initiate communication which is often predictive of communicative behaviour (MacIntyre 1994). While L1 WTC was perceived as a personality-based, trait-like disposition (McCroskey and Richmond 1991), WTC in a second language (L2) was reinterpreted as a situational variable, open to change across time and situations and defined as an intention “to enter into discourse at a particular time with a specific person or persons, using a L2” (MacIntyre et al. 1998, p. 547).

MacIntyre et al. (1998) schematized L2 WTC in a pyramid model and conceptualized a range of social, contextual, psychological, and linguistic variables that converge to shape WTC in L2. In the past decade, this model has been extensively tested in diverse informal and instructional contexts with young learners and adult students (Cao and Wei 2019). Empirical research conducted in the last decade has identified causal relationships between L2 WTC and several learner variables, including communication and language-related factors of perceived communicative competence, perceived opportunity to communicate, perceived L2 proficiency, L2 fluency, and language experience; and psychological factors of language anxiety, perceived communicative competence, personality, motivation, attitude, learner belief, emotion, gender and age (Cao 2011, 2014; Fushino 2010; Khajavy and Ghonsooly 2017; Nematizadeh 2019; Zhou et al. 2020). Elahi Shirvan et al.’s (2019) meta-analytical study examined the overall average correlation between L2 WTC and the most frequently identified predictors, including perceived communicative competence, language anxiety, and motivation. They found that these variables moderately correlated with L2 WTC with perceived communicative competence having the largest effect.

1.2 Trait and situational WTC

Similar to other individual difference (ID) factors in SLA including anxiety, personality, motivation, and emotion, WTC in L2 also has dual characteristics: trait and situational WTC. Trait WTC prepares individuals for communication by creating a tendency to place themselves in situations where communication is expected; situational WTC, on the other hand, influences the decision to initiate communication in a particular situation (MacIntyre et al. 1999). While much of the quantitative research investigated L2 WTC as a trait variable, Kang’s (2005) qualitative study was the first attempt that offered insights into L2 WTC as a situational variable. She found that situational WTC was a multi-layered construct that could change from moment to moment in the conversational context, under the joint effect of the psychological conditions and situational variables.

L2 WTC as a situational construct has been investigated in experimental, quasi-experimental, and classroom settings (e.g., Cao 2011, 2014; MacIntyre and Legatto 2011; Pawlak and Mystkowska-Wiertelak 2015). In an earlier classroom-based study on L2 WTC, Cao and Philp (2006) compared English learners’ self-report of WTC and their WTC behaviour in class in New Zealand and found that interactional settings (i.e., whole class, small groups, and dyads) exerted a significant influence on WTC. Cao’s (2011, 2013, 2014 longitudinal study on L2 WTC in the same context used a multiple case study approach and employed an ecological framework. The key participants were followed over an academic year and their classroom WTC was examined as lessons unfolded each week. This study revealed dynamic changes in learners’ WTC on lesson-to-lesson, task-to-task, and moment-to-moment bases. Methodologically, this study contributed to validating a classroom observation scheme of WTC behaviour in class.

Peng (2014) examined Chinese EFL learners’ WTC as situated in university English classrooms and found a complex interrelationship among individual, contextual, and affective predictors of WTC. Her studies also highlighted the cultural influence on L2 WTC. Yashima et al. (2016) intervention study in a Japanese EFL class over one semester revealed that differences in the frequency of self-initiated turns in discussion sessions emerged through the interactions of enduring individual characteristics such as personality and proficiency and contextual influences including interlocutor’s reactions and group-level talk-silence patterns. Zhang et al. (2018) conducted a systematic review of 35 research studies on the situational antecedents contributing to variation in WTC. This study highlighted the role of the learning situation and concluded that the situational variables could be organized into a multi-layered framework to explain the dynamic processes underpinning WTC. Al-Murtadha (2021) compared self-reported WTC in L1 and L2. He found no significant correlations between self-reported and observed L1 WTC or between self-reported and observed L2 WTC. However, significant and positive correlations were found between observed L1 and L2 WTC.

1.3 Complex dynamic systems (CDS) approach and WTC research

The application of the theory of complex dynamic systems (CDS) in L2 research, and ID variables in particular, has recently gained ground producing new insights into uncharted dimensions in SLA. With its principal focus on the phenomenon of change, CDS has secured a move away from the use of reductionist quantitative approaches (Gaddis 2002) and the perception of causality (Larsen-Freeman and Cameron 2008). Specific to ID research, Dörnyei (2009) urged a reconsideration of two fundamental issues concerning individual-level variation: the conceptualization of a dynamic system of multiple interrelated ID components; and the feasibility of distinguishing different types of ID factors. He further argued that the interconnected, constantly changing, and environmentally sensitive IDs system serves as an example of a complex, dynamic system. He proposed that individual variation in the system is not so much a function of the strength of any individual determinants such as motivation or aptitude but rather of all the relevant factors working together.

Dörnyei et al. (2015), in their edited volume on motivational dynamics, argued that the theory of complex dynamic systems looks at the dynamics that emerge from the interaction between the interdependent components of a variable. Due to this interaction, CDS tend to be dynamic and nonlinear, meaning that they, sometimes, change continuously or discontinuously (Larsen-Freeman and Cameron 2008). Such variability is not captured through correlational means.

The CDS theory has been employed in several studies to investigate dynamism and moment-to-moment fluctuation in WTC in experimental and classroom contexts. In the Canadian context, MacIntyre and Legatto (2011) employed an idiodynamic method to investigate dynamism in L2 WTC in a laboratory setting. They monitored changes in participants’ WTC across and during eight communicative tasks and measured WTC and anxiety as variables. They found an interaction among the affective, cognitive, and linguistic systems and this interrelationship triggered changes in WTC. Failure in retrieving vocabulary and unfamiliarity with the discussion topics influenced the participants’ WTC. MacIntyre (2020) reported on his research group’s work on L2 WTC dynamism on a timescale of a few minutes and the “complex interactions and rapid changes in participants’ psychological conditions” (p. 111) when approaching and avoiding communication.

The situated L2 WTC was also investigated in the Polish context and the findings confirmed that learners’ WTC was in a state of flux and influenced by a host of individual, linguistic, and contextual variables (Pawlak and Mystkowska-Wiertelak 2015; Pawlak et al. 2015). For example, Pawlak and Mystkowska-Wiertelak (2015) observed dynamism in L2 learners’ WTC during communicative tasks. Through the use of self-rating, questionnaires, and interviews, they found that contextual factors (e.g., topic, interlocutor, planning time), linguistic resources, and individual differences led to shifts in WTC. This study hinted at an interplay among linguistic, affective, and cognitive factors. This study confirmed MacIntyre and Legatto’s (2011) finding that participants’ WTC levels dropped when they struggled at lexical and conceptual levels. Failure in vocabulary retrieval caused an increase in language anxiety and loss of focus, which in turn negatively affected WTC.

Pawlak et al.’s (2015) subsequent classroom-based study investigated dynamism in students’ WTC during different classroom events. Apart from students’ self-rating and questionnaire data, teachers’ comments on students’ WTC were also used. They observed between-group changes to WTC due to interactions between contextual and individual factors and some of these interactions were unpredictable. The results also confirmed the influence of contextual factors (e.g., task, topic, and interlocutor) on WTC changes.

Employing an idiodynamic method, Nematizadeh and Wood (2019) examined the affective and cognitive dynamics of L2 students’ WTC. They explored how the affective and cognitive variables interact with temporal measures of speech. They found that the affect, as an underlying layer of WTC, and cognitive processes underlying speech fluency interacted in an unpredictable, multidimensional, and interconnected manner. The factors affecting WTC included vocabulary retrieval, preparedness, topic familiarity/transition, self-monitoring and inaccurate language. These factors also simultaneously affected speech fluency. The findings accounted for some features of complex dynamic systems including complexity, dynamism, and nonlinearity.

Similarly, Sato (2019, 2020 employed a CDST approach to investigate the relationships between WTC and its underlying factors, particularly the language proficiency of the speakers or their interlocutors. He used post-task stimulated recall activities, requiring participants to mark changes in their WTC on a transcription or a scale. The findings from Sato’s studies point to the soft-assembly nature of WTC when differing factors affected students’ WTC at lower-intermediate and advanced proficiency levels. These findings are in line with previous research (Cao 2011, 2014; Kang 2005) that interlocutor’s proficiency can promote or prohibit WTC (Ducker 2022).

The most recent attempt at investigating L2 WTC using CDST was Ducker’s (2022) research that examined WTC antecedents, ongoing WTC ratings, and spontaneous communication in an intact classroom. A unique finding from this study was the crucial role of listening comprehension as a prerequisite for participation and listening as a primary focus of learner behaviour. Ducker (2022) identified multiple engaged listening stances, including listening as a distinct turn-taking behaviour, listening carefully while observing a policy of non-interruption, listening while strictly observing allocated turns, and listening as a stance or approach to the activity (in this case, to learn about partners and topics).

Our review suggests that aspects of dynamism and interrelatedness are already recognized to some extent in the existing literature on L2 WTC in face-to-face interaction. However, there has been little attempt at investigating its dynamism in online synchronous communication.

1.4 WTC construct in online tasks

A handful of studies have explored WTC in online communication and teaching. Freiermuth and Jarrel (2006) compared Japanese EFL students’ WTC in face-to-face and online environments. This experimental study revealed that online chatting provided a more comfortable environment for the students and enhanced their WTC. In a subsequent study, Freiermuth and Huang (2012) examined Japanese EFL students’ motivation in chatting with Taiwanese EFL students using online synchronic software. They found that pronunciation difficulties and linguistic and paralinguistic deficiencies could lead to an increase in anxiety, lowering their WTC in face-to-face group interaction. In contrast, an online environment facilitated safe opportunities to communicate. Al-Amrani (2013) also compared Omani EFL learners’ WTC in synchronous, asynchronous communication, and face-to-face interaction, and reported less WTC in face-to-face communication compared to online communication due to fear of making mistakes, feeling of embarrassment, and loss of face in face-to-face contexts. Al-Amrani’s (2013) study confirmed findings from Freiermuth’s research that online communication is a more favourable mode than face-to-face interaction.

Research has also explored the positive impact of educational technologies on L2 learners’ WTC. For example, the role of massively multiplayer online games (MMOGs) in L2 learning was reviewed and synthesized by Jabbari and Eslami (2019), who found that L2 learners demonstrated a lower level of anxiety in the safe environment provided by MMOGs. The games presented many opportunities and socialization activities to enhance L2 learners’ communicative competence. Kruk’s study (2020) observed two advanced English learners’ experience in an online virtual reality game and explored their fluctuating trajectory of WTC, motivation, boredom, and anxiety. Results revealed that trajectory changes were influenced by both positive factors including interesting topics and cooperative interlocutors and negative factors such as unpleasant L2 users, monotony, boredom, and prior negative experience. Ebadi and Ebadijalal (2020) explored the use of the Google Expeditions VR tool and the results indicated that the students’ general knowledge, motivation, confidence, and cultural awareness were promoted with the help of technology and hence resulted in an improvement in their WTC and oral performance.

Lee’s (2019) study examined factors affecting Korean EFL learners’ WTC in the extramural digital environment and found that socio-political, contextual, and individual variables interacted with each other to influence participants’ L2 WTC in the extramural digital context. Lee and Lee’s (2020) study examined the extent to which the affective factors including motivation, self-confidence, risk-taking, L2 speaking anxiety and grit, and virtual intercultural experiences were linked to L2 WTC in in-class, out-of-class, and digital settings. Results suggested that demographic and affective variables as well as involvement in virtual intercultural activities played distinct roles in influencing EFL students’ WTC in three different L2 communication contexts. In particular, younger individuals who had a higher level of L2 confidence and engaged more frequently in virtual intercultural experiences had higher L2 WTC in digital settings.

Ardiansyah et al. (2020) conducted an idiodynamic multiple case study to examine the dynamism of students’ WTC in an online discussion task. Three strands of factors were found to underlie online WTC, namely, psychological, conversational, and linguistic. This study also highlighted the difference between online and offline communication, identifying the internet as a mediator to promote or hinder the WTC level. It was suggested, however, with an adequate level of self-confidence, technological issues can promote WTC in the form of asking for clarifications.

1.5 Research aims and questions

Previous empirical research on L2 WTC has revealed its trait and situational levels in face-to-face conversational and classroom contexts. Situational WTC has been found to emerge from complex dynamic interactions among social, contextual, cognitive, linguistic, and individual factors in face-to-face settings (Cao 2014; Nematizadeh 2019), but this dynamic nature of WTC has not been explored in online group discussion tasks using a mixed methods design. A further gap in previous WTC research is the lack of exploration of the relationship between observed WTC and speaking time. To address these research gaps, the current study aims to gauge the WTC of intermediate Farsi-speaking ESL speakers in online synchronous discussion tasks. The present study aims to address the following research questions:

  1. Are there any correlations between self-reported trait WTC, trait WTC, dynamic WTC, observed WTC, online WTC, and speaking time?

  2. To what extent does the participants’ WTC fluctuate during synchronous group discussion tasks?

  3. What factors do the participants attribute their WTC fluctuations to?

2 Method

2.1 Setting and participants

Upon receiving ethics clearance, the study recruited Farsi-speaking ESL speakers (2 males and 5 females) using two inclusion criteria – proficiency level (recent IELTS score of 6.5 and 7 – or C1 on CEFR scale) and length of residence (less than 2 years) in Canada. Previous research has found that the length of residence affects language confidence (Lopez and Bui 2014) which is a predictor of L2 WTC (MacIntyre et al. 2001). Language proficiency has also been suggested to affect self-confidence and WTC (Clément 1986; Yashima 2002). Therefore, the inclusion criteria were adopted to offset the effect of these variables.

Recruitment messages were posted in the Telegram channels of Iranian communities across Canada, and interested individuals contacted the first author for further details. Seven participants, who met the above criteria, received and signed online consent forms. Table 1 provides the profile of the participants. The participants did not know each other prior to the study.

Table 1:

Participants’ profile

Pseudonym Gender Residence in Canada IELTS score
1 Aman Male Less than 2 years 6
2 Dorsa Female Less than 1 year 7
3 Parvin Female Less than 1 year 7
4 Nafise Female Less than 1 year 7
5 Peyman Male Less than 2 years 6.5
6 Zaki Female Less than 1 year 7
7 Masrez Female Less than 1 year 6.5

2.2 Discussion tasks facilitators

An invitation to take part in the study with information on the study goals and procedures was sent to potential discussion tasks facilitators who were experienced ESL teachers at a Canadian post-secondary institution and already familiar with synchronous teaching and the Zoom application. Those interested (1 male and 5 females) later received specific instructions (Appendix 1) on facilitating the discussion tasks online to ensure consistency.

2.3 Instruments

2.3.1 Zoom

Zoom is a video-conferencing software program that was used to facilitate online discussion tasks. The program features a gallery view in which all the participants can be monitored.

2.3.2 Trait WTC instrument

Trait WTC was measured using the scale developed by McCroskey and Richmond (1991). The scale provides a definition of WTC and 20 situations in which individuals might choose to communicate or not. Participants are required to rate their trait WTC in different communicative situations on a 0 to 100 scale. The alpha reliability estimate of this instrument has ranged from 0.86 to 0.90 (McCroskey 1992).

2.3.3 WTC rating scales and stimulated recall forms

The WTC rating grid and stimulated recall forms (Appendix 2) were created to allow the participants to rate their WTC every 5 min using a −5 to +5 scale, jot down keywords during the discussion task, and further explain their ratings. This method has been previously used in WTC research (Pawlak and Mystkowska-Wiertelak 2015).

2.3.4 Observation scheme

The WTC observation scheme items were adapted from Cao (2014) and Al-Murtadha (2019) and used by the researchers to identify the indicators of each participant’s observed WTC in the recordings (Appendix 3). The items included features such as observable intentions of engaging in discussion (e.g., raising hands), volunteering to talk or ask questions or for clarifications, trying out new structures or vocabulary, and presenting one’s opinion.

2.3.5 Online WTC scale

The scale adapted items from Cao’s (2014) classroom WTC observation scheme. However, the items were rewritten to specifically compare online WTC with classroom WTC (Appendix 4). This 10-item Likert scale was validated with 50 respondents in an online EAP program prior to the study and the alpha reliability was high (α = 0.92).

2.4 Data collection procedure

The participants were introduced to the data collection procedure in advance using a video tutorial in Farsi. This tutorial offered tips for using and downloading Zoom, completing online WTC Google Forms, using the WTC rating scale, and jotting-down notes during the stimulated recall stage. Participants were also asked to complete an online version of the trait WTC instrument on Google Forms.

Then six thirty-minute Zoom sessions were scheduled based on the participants’ availability in August and September 2020, each moderated by a different facilitator. Six previously piloted topics (Nematizadeh 2019), which included food, technology, traveling, online versus on-campus education, crime, and advertisement, were used in the study. The first author attended and recorded all the sessions, but did not participate in the discussions. The discussion tasks started with a quick introduction to the topic and a beeping sound to prompt the participants to input their initial WTC level on a scale between −5 and +5 using the WTC rating scale. The sound continued at intervals of 5 min. The rating scale also provided space for the participants to jot down notes that later assisted them with recalling the factors that triggered WTC shifts. Immediately after, in a stimulated recall procedure, the researcher played the recording of the session and shared the screen to help the participants with recalling their thoughts while they were performing the task and elaborating on the factors triggering changes. Stimulated recall is an introspective procedure whereby participants are prompted to “recall thoughts they had while performing a task” (Gass and Mackey 2000, p. 17). It should be noted that the stimulated recall sessions did not involve any group discussions and were done silently as the participants reflected on and recalled their state of mind while completing the tasks. Following this, participants completed the online WTC scale via Google Forms, where the participants responded to items that compared their online WTC during each given task with their WTC if they were to complete these tasks in traditional face-to-face sessions. This concluded the data collection in every session.

2.5 Data analysis

2.5.1 Translation

Some of the participants produced their stimulated recall responses in English, which facilitated the analysis. The Farsi responses were translated into English by the first researcher and a research assistant. For consistency, the responses elicited in session one were translated by the first author to English and back-translated into Farsi by a research assistant who was familiar with WTC research. The original Farsi commentaries and the back-translated version were compared by the two and slight nuances were resolved. The nuances mostly involved agreeing on appropriate English terms, for which the first author and the assistant drew on previous WTC literature to remain consistent. The rest of the sessions were translated into English by the first author, and reviewed by the research assistant.

2.5.2 Coding the stimulated recall data

The data collected in the stimulated recall sessions were then coded in vivo – or what Saldaña calls “verbatim coding” (2013, p. 91). This ensured that the researchers used the terms that the participants used. The first session’s stimulated-recall data were in vivo coded independently by the first researcher and the assistant to ensure consistency of the terms. They then compared the terms and created a codelist that was used later by the first researcher to code the remaining data. In the case of new codes, the first author and the assistant discussed the appropriacy of the labels. The second cycle of coding involved “pattern coding’’ (Saldaña 2013, p. 210), which involved developing major themes from the data. This involved categorizing the codes into smaller umbrella categories that were heavily informed by previous WTC research and underwent several revisions by the first author and the research assistant.

The trait WTC was measured by computing an average of the participants responses to three items of McCroskey and Richmond’s (1991) scale that specifically gauged the participants’ WTC in group discussions. The dynamic WTC was measured by computing an average of the participants’ per-five-minute WTC ratings for each session. Measuring the observed WTC behaviour involved identifying the WTC indicators using the observation scheme. The first author and a research assistant viewed and coded the first group discussion task together and shared insights for consistency and training purposes. They then viewed and coded the remaining five sessions independently, with an inter-rater reliability of 90 %. The total number of the WTC indicators each participant displayed during each task and the total task indicators yielded a percentage that displayed the WTC behaviour of each participant. Online WTC was computed using responses to the online WTC scale, which the participants completed via Google Forms after each task. The speaking times were measured manually for each session and each participant. The recordings were viewed by the research assistant and the participants’ total length of turns was measured using a digital timer and computed in minutes and seconds for each session.

3 Results

3.1 Correlating trait WTC, dynamic WTC, observed WTC, online WTC, and speaking times

The first research question aimed to examine if there were any correlations between the participants’ trait WTC, dynamic WTC, observed WTC, online WTC, and the speaking times in the online discussion tasks. To this end, multiple Pearson correlation tests were performed. As Table 2 shows, trait WTC during discussion tasks and online WTC were found to be moderately positively correlated, r(37) = 0.35, p = 0.031. This means that those reporting higher levels of trait WTC during discussion tasks also tended to display higher WTC in online discussion tasks. The participants’ online WTC also correlated closely with their actual dynamic WTC rating during the tasks, r(37) = 0.484, p = 0.002. This was a meaningful finding as the participants’ general perceptions of their online WTC experience were found to be consistent with their actual rating of WTC during the online discussion tasks in this study. However, no correlations were found between trait WTC versus dynamic WTC, observed WTC, or speaking times, a finding that was expected because the trait WTC was an independent measure that involved the participants’ general WTC prior to the discussion tasks, as opposed to dynamic WTC, observed WTC, and speaking times, which were measured in the context of the tasks.

Table 2:

Correlation tests

Online WTC Trait WTC (group discussions) Averaged dynamic WTC ratings Observed WTC percentages Speaking time
Online WTC Pearson correlation 1 0.355a 0.484b −0.404a −0.153
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.031 0.002 0.013 0.366
N 37 37 37 37 37
Trait WTC (for group discussions) Pearson correlation 0.355a 1 0.166 −0.112 −0.152
Sig. (2-Tailed) 0.031 0.327 0.510 0.369
N 37 42 37 37 37
Averaged dynamic WTC ratings Pearson correlation 0.484b 0.166 1 0.114 0.333a
Sig. (2-Tailed) 0.002 0.327 0.500 0.044
N 37 37 37 37 37
Observed WTC percentages Pearson correlation −0.404a −0.112 0.114 1 0.514b
Sig. (2-Tailed) 0.013 0.510 0.500 0.001
N 37 37 37 37 37
Speaking time Pearson correlation −0.153 −0.152 0.333a 0.514b 1
Sig. (2-Tailed) 0.366 0.369 0.044 0.001
N 37 37 37 37 37
  1. aCorrelation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed), bcorrelation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

A moderate negative correlation was found between online WTC and observed WTC, r(37) = −0.40, p = 0.01, and no correlation was found between dynamic WTC and observed WTC. These results indicate that the participants’ general perceptions of their online WTC measured through the same indicators used in the observation scheme did not manifest in their observed WTC behaviors.

Speaking time was positively correlated with dynamic WTC at a moderate level, r(37) = 0.33, p = 0.04, and with observed WTC at a strong level, r(37) = 0.51, p = 0.001. This means that those speaking more during the tasks both rated their WTC higher and exhibited more of the WTC indicators.

3.2 WTC variations

The second research question aimed to examine the extent to which the participants’ WTC fluctuates during synchronous discussion tasks. To this end, a between-subject repeated measures ANOVA was performed to examine whether the per-five-minute WTC changes were statistically significant. The results showed significant changes in WTC over five-minute intervals [F(6, 216) = 2.247, p = 0.04, partial-eta squared = 0.59]. Since we were interested in whether WTC changes were noticeable and not in which minute of rating WTC changed more significantly, no post hoc tests were conducted.

Table 3 shows the participants’ mean dynamic WTC and the standard deviation per 5 min per task. Figure 1 also illustrates the per-five-minute changes of group WTC per session. As can be seen, while the group displayed high initial WTC in all the tasks, there are noticeable variations, which range from a low of 0.83 in T5 to a high of 3.16 in T3. The SDs, or the extent of dispersion, also show that the group WTC levels varied considerably upon the start of the tasks, with a SD of 1.60 recorded in T3 and a SD of 3.10 in T6. What is commonly observed across most of the tasks (except in T3) is the gradually rising WTC in the first half of the discussion tasks, with T2 and T5 reaching their peak WTC in the first 5 min, while in T1, T4, and T6, group WTC continued to rise to minutes 10 and 15, respectively. After the initial rise, the group WTC underwent a declining trend at some point with T2 and T3 in minute five, T5 and T6 in minute 10, and T1, T4, and T6 in minute 15. Except in T4, group WTC went on an upward trend toward the end of the tasks.

Table 3:

Participants’ mean dynamic WTC and SDs per session

Sessions Values Aman Dorsa Masrez Nafise Parvin Payman Zaki
T1 Mean dynamic WTC 4 3 4.71 0.57 NA 2.85 1.71
SD 1 2.64 2.05 2.63 0.48 NA 1.06
T2 Mean dynamic WTC 2 3.42 1 −1.71 1.57 1 4.28
SD 2.64 1.61 0.75 1.97 1.73 2.22 0.81
T3 Mean dynamic WTC 3.14 NA 3.57 0.57 2.28 3.57 2.6
SD 2.34 NA 1.51 2.5 1.27 0.95 0.53
T4 Mean dynamic WTC 1.71 3.14 5 −0.57 NA NA 3.28
SD 2.87 1.34 1.7 2.29 0 NA NA
T5 Mean dynamic WTC 3.42 2.57 2.85 −0.57 0.71 0.42 NA
SD 1.81 1.51 NA 2.29 1.57 1.25 0.97
T6 Mean dynamic WTC 3.28 3.85 3.28 −1.85 3.28 2.42 2.42
SD 2.56 1.21 2.14 1.46 1.49 1.11 0.78
Figure 1: 
Group mean WTC per five minutes per task/session.
Figure 1:

Group mean WTC per five minutes per task/session.

The second part of this research question aimed to investigate the factors that the participants attributed their WTC changes to during online discussion tasks. In two cycles of coding, a total of 414 codes were assigned, and nine categories of factors, to which the participants attributed their WTC changes, emerged.

The codes revolve around nine categories, including factors concerning: (1) topics or discussion questions; (2) the individual factors; (3) the possession of supporting ideas; (4) interlocutors; (5) the facilitators; (6) social or environmental considerations; (7) task dynamics; (8) perceived performance; (9) organizational factors or technical issues. Due to space limitations, a few representative samples will be presented.

3.3 Topics or discussion questions

This category involved the participants’ perceptions of the topics or questions. The participants expressed concern about not understanding the topics or the facilitators’ questions, which seemed to increase their anxiety as they tried to contribute relevant inputs. In support of this, Zaki mentioned: “a new question was posed and I was trying to fully understand it so as to avoid giving irrelevant answers”. Understanding the topic or the questions proved to be very important to all the other participants, too. For instance, Dorsa and Nafiseh lost WTC as they expected more clarifications from the facilitator, with Dorsa noting: “the question was ambiguous and difficult to answer”, or Nafiseh ranting: “the facilitator was silent and would not explain the topic”. There were instances, however, where the participants’ WTC gradually rose as they developed an understanding of the topics. Parvin, for instance, noted: “as the topic was starting to click, I felt more comfortable to engage in the discussion.

Other factors involved the participants’ overall topical experience or preparedness, which improved the participants’ WTC. For instance, Parvin mentioned that she had “relevant experiences to the topic”, or Aman stated that he “had lots to say about the topic as it was relevant to his field of study”. WTC also fluctuated as the facilitators posed new questions or when the participants perceived the discussions as tedious or questions as repetitive. New questions sometimes triggered a shift of focus in the discussion, which required some processing and preparation time on the part of the participants. As the discussions were ongoing and the participants struggled to retrieve ideas to contribute, new questions or topic shifts sometimes led to a reduced WTC, or in one case, Zaki gradually lost interest as the discussion moved on to some other subject. On the other hand, on several occasions, new questions promoted WTC mainly in cases where the participants had already contributed and had nothing to say. Therefore, posing new questions provided new opportunities for task engagement.

3.4 Individual/personal factors

This category involved the participants’ personal interests, views, experiences, or old memories, or the participants’ emotional or physical states. The ‘interest’ refers to whether something in the discussions and ideas appealed to the participants temporarily, as opposed to the general interest in a topic discussed in the previous section. WTC tended to rise when the participants related the discussion to their personal interests, experiences, or memories, while it dropped due to a lack of interest. For instance, Aman found the discussions interesting on a number of occasions: “personal interpretations have always been interesting to me”, while Zaki reported a low WTC, noting: “the discussion concerned the food decorations which I had no interest in”. This category also involved factors that appeared to provide opportunities for the participants to relate the discussions to their personal life (e.g., experiences, views, etc.) thus facilitating the generation of ideas and engagement.

In some situations, the participants felt distracted, stressed, depressed, or physically fatigued, all of which lowered their WTC. As an illustration, Aman, Dorsa, Nafiseh, and Masrez all showed low WTC and stated that they “had a long hectic day and were tired” during the sessions which were all scheduled for 6:00 pm. Nafisah further added that she “was unable to focus on the discussion and felt distracted all the time”. These factors seemed to sidetrack the participants and minimize their engagement in the discussion tasks.

3.5 Content or supporting ideas

The third category pertained to the possession of supporting ideas, which evoked a feeling of security to engage in the tasks. WTC typically rose as the participants generated or retrieved ideas relevant to the ongoing discussion. For instance, Dorsa mentioned: “after listening to Parviz, I had many more new ideas to share and my WTC rose”, or Parvin noted: Parvin: “I felt I had funny ideas regarding the topic and this made me want to talk, or I felt like I could joke about the topic”. On the other hand, a lack of supporting ideas sometimes resulted in low WTC. For instance, Dorsa noted: “while I was listening to others, I had nothing to say and this lowered my WTC”, or Peyman said: “I did not have strong ideas to make my argument”.

3.6 Interlocutors

The participants’ WTC fluctuated depending on factors associated with the interlocutors – or other participants. These included the interlocutors’ opinions and participation, interactions or reactions, and their level of familiarity or proficiency as perceived by the participants.

Many of the participants indicated that sometimes they enjoyed listening to others expressing their opinions and therefore had little WTC to engage. Aman, for example, noted: “I admired one of the participant’s ideas and was encouraged to listen rather than talk”. Dorsa also mentioned: “I found the opinions of other participants more interesting and thus had little WTC to engage”. Sometimes, WTC fluctuations depended on the quality of interlocutors’ ideas or whether or not the participants agreed or disagreed with the interlocutors’ opinions. For example, Masrez lost WTC as she “found another participant’s response too long for a 30-min session”. In two instances, Masrez and Zaki lost WTC as they were surprised by other participants’ responses. On the other hand, there were two instances where disagreeing with an interlocutor’s opinion promoted WTC. There were also a few cases where the other participants’ participation improved the one’s WTC. For instance, Parvin said: “it was a heated debate and I wanted to engage and express my thoughts”.

The interactions with or reactions of the interlocutors also affected WTC. Peyman’s WTC dropped due to an interlocutor’s reaction to what he said. He explained: “if others agreed with me, I would generally have lower WTC to engage in discussion”, which was because he preferred to listen carefully so he could concentrate and generate more ideas to contribute. Masrez and Dorsa indicated that challenging co-participants created a motivation that improved their WTC.

Familiarity or unfamiliarity with interlocutors also impacted their WTC. Nafise felt “anxious about talking to new people. On the other hand, Masrez reported higher WTC as she got acquainted with the participant over two sessions. In addition, Zaki and Peyman indicated that perceiving the interlocutors’ lower or equal proficiency level improved their WTC.

3.7 The facilitators

This category concerned the facilitators’ support, feedback, or reactions during the discussion tasks or the way they managed, engaged, or contributed to the discussions. Aman, for instance, mentioned: “what I noticed facilitators were really important and affected the way I reacted during the tasks”, or Masrez’s gained WTC was because of a facilitator’s “nice and respectful” manner. On the other hand, the participants’ WTC declined due to the facilitators’ lack of support, interest, or feedback. Zaki noted: “the session lacked the energy and engaging force, in fact, not receiving feedback for the facilitator lowered my willingness to participate”.

The facilitator’s management of the tasks influenced their WTC. This involved whether or not the facilitators prompted or encouraged the participants to engage, engaged in the discussion themselves, provided clarifications when the participants were confused, or created equal opportunities for everyone to engage. For instance, Masrez noted that the “facilitator was directing the task very well by picking good and timely questions”. It turned out that the facilitators’ engagement also promoted the participants’ WTC. Parvin stated: “when the facilitator started to talk, I felt better because I expected him to say something or give examples that would eventually help me generate ideas and talk”.

Many of the participants were in favor of having the freedom to talk whenever they felt ready while Dorsa preferred to be given turns to talk. One of the facilitators chose to direct questions to the participants despite being instructed to not do so, and Dorsa noted: “I was used to this because it gives me the opportunity to talk and then listen to the others. This allowed me to focus on the quality of my responses rather than on when and how to get a chance to talk”. However, Zaki showed no interest in the way the facilitator managed the task.

3.8 Social or environmental considerations

This category mainly involved how the participants reserved the right for the other participants to engage and contribute to the discussion tasks. In some cases, the participants’ WTC dropped as they had already participated and thus avoided talking to allow the other interlocutors opportunities to talk. In other cases, some participants had low WTC because of having to wait for their turn. Dorsa, for example, lost WTC due to “interrupting Peyman and feeling embarrassed for this”. In the second session where the facilitator prompted the participants one by one, Parvin and Zaki exhibited low WTC because they felt they had to wait for the facilitator to directly engage them rather than volunteer to talk.

Some of the other instances involved the participants getting acquainted with the group and thus feeling more comfortable. Nafise, however, indicated that despite being forced to talk in one instance, she lost WTC because of the dominating silence and not feeling comfortable. Nafise also felt anxious and uncomfortable in session one because of “meeting new people and being in a new environment”.

3.9 Task dynamics

The dynamics of the discussion tasks also influenced the participants’ WTC. This involved the group dynamics and the participation opportunities that the participants perceived to arise from such dynamics. Some of the participants lost WTC upon finding the discussion irrelevant to the main topic, or if the session was coming to an end. For instance, Nafise mentioned: “I lost track of the discussion because of the irrelevant discussions, only one person talks, and this should have been managed by the instructor”. Presumably, Nafise had prepared ideas to share, but the discussion had taken a different direction and her ideas were no longer relevant. Silence was also found to improve WTC. Some chose to talk as silence dominated the sessions, with some perceiving it as an opportunity to engage while others, like Nafise, perceived it as an awkward moment that she had to put an end to.

Almost all the participants reported moments where they struggled for opportunities to talk, resulting in reduced WTC in all the cases. In Dorsa’s case, this also led to some anxiety: “I was slightly worried and losing WTC because others jumped in and wouldn’t let me talk”.

3.10 Perceived performance

This category mainly involved the participants’ perceived communication incompetence or lexical issues. The perceived communication incompetence pertained to situations where the participants found themselves unable to communicate their points, which lowered their WTC in all the cases. For instance, Aman noted: “I do not see the ability to make my point. It is terrible that the English language is so difficult for me after studying it for such a long time”.

3.11 Organizational factors or technical issues

This category involved moments of low WTC due to arriving late, experiencing technical issues typical of online sessions, or getting distracted due to background noise originating from other participants’ microphones.

The participants lost WTC due to being late, and this was mostly because they had missed the facilitator’s introduction to the topic and/or the showing of the images which would help with generating ideas. The low WTC was triggered by the anxiety of not being aware of what was going on and having to wait and learn more about the ongoing discussion. In support of this, Zaki mentioned: “due to arriving late, I had a high level of anxiety and needed some time to catch up. Due to stress, I felt I was unable to comprehend what the others were saying, and it was difficult for me to concentrate”.

Technical issues also had an impact on the participants’ WTC. For instance, Dorsa mentioned that “poor internet connections increased her anxiety giving her the impression that she might not speak well”, which lowered her WTC. Masrez also experienced multiple connection issues, which caused her to lose track of the discussion and lose WTC as a result. Nafise also mentioned that she had lost the speaker’s voice on multiple occasions which had sidetracked her and lowered her WTC as she had no idea what was being discussed.

In two more cases, Aman and Nafise mentioned that the background noise originating from another participant had disrupted them. While Aman did not lose much WTC and only mentioned it was annoying, Nafise lost WTC, noting: “I wish there weren’t distractions behind people like family members of the participants. I lost track of the discussion because of this”.

4 Discussion

This study set out to examine whether and how the online discussion tasks affected the WTC of L2 speakers of English. The first research question aimed to find any correlations between trait WTC in discussion tasks, dynamic WTC, observed WTC, online WTC, and speaking times. The findings suggested that the participants’ trait WTC during discussion tasks and their online WTC were relatively correlated. More specifically, the participant’s willingness to talk in a small group of strangers, acquaintances, and friends correlated with their WTC during online discussion tasks. While previous research identified online communication to be more favourable than face-to-face interaction (Al-Amrani 2013; Freiermuth and Huang 2012), the current study reveals that participants are equally willing or unwilling to communicate in face-to-face and online discussion tasks. This is an encouraging finding, especially for those concerned about students’ low WTC and engagement due to the 2020 abrupt transition to online courses and the current dilemma concerning the future of online and on-campus education. Based on our findings, while the levels of WTC varied from participant to participant and from task to task, the group’s average WTC during the online discussion tasks turned out to be closely the same as their classroom WTC.

The correlation between the online WTC and mean dynamic WTC during the tasks indicates that the participants’ perceptions of their online WTC levels after completing the discussion tasks were relatively consistent with their mean dynamic WTC, which encompasses the changes due to a variety of factors during each respective task. It is argued that quantitative procedures, which mostly use scales like the online WTC scale that we developed in this study, record observations that offer a “snapshot of the processes” underlying WTC and therefore “frozen in one photo” (MacIntyre 2007, p. 572). This will limit the scope of our observations of a given variable. The procedures we used integrated the so-called snapshot notion as well as the variability of WTC. The scale alone would not have provided such an observation. Therefore, we believe that we have captured a more thorough picture of WTC in an online environment by integrating these two methods. In addition, no other study, to our knowledge, has correlated participants’ dynamic ratings of WTC and their online WTC, and the consistency between these two constructs adds to the validity of both tools.

There was a moderate negative correlation between the observed WTC and the online WTC. As indicated earlier, online WTC was measured using a scale that incorporated items that compared online versus classroom WTC. This scale elicited the participants’ WTC in online classes immediately after the discussion tasks. The items were identical to those in the observation scheme used to gauge observed WTC. The participants may have displayed more online WTC indicators than they thought they would when comparing their online and classroom WTC. On the other hand, they may perceive themselves to have greater online WTC when, in fact, they do not display that in their actual online WTC behaviour. For instance, one of the indicators in both the online WTC scale and the observation scheme pertained to the likelihood of the participants volunteering or raising their hands to answer a question. In this case, the participants may have volunteered to talk more frequently than they thought they would in an online class, or on the contrary, they may have perceived themselves to be more willing to communicate, while they did not exhibit that in their actual WTC behaviour. This finding is in line with classroom WTC research in which self-reported WTC was found to be inconsistent with WTC behavior in class (Al-Murtadha 2021; Cao and Philp 2006). This finding also implies that other underlying promoting or inhibiting factors might have been at play to influence their WTC.

The positive correlations between the speaking time and dynamic WTC, and the speaking time and observed WTC were foreseeable. The participants were conscious of their degree of participation when they were making contributions and vice versa. They spoke more when they were more willing and spoke less when they had lower WTC. For instance, greater background knowledge led to higher WTC and speaking time. The positive correlation between speaking time and observed WTC is also reasonable; the more actively one participated, the more indicators of WTC he/she exhibited, and vice versa. This seems to partially mirror the findings of Dörnyei and Kormos (2000) who found strong and positive correlations between learners’ WTC and the amount of L2 they produced in task performance when they held a positive attitude towards the task; however, there was no correlation between WTC and the amount of L2 produced in the case of learners with more negative attitudes towards the task. Cao’s (2012) study only found that learners with higher WTC would be more inclined to produce more complex language than students with lower WTC; however, no clear correlations were found between WTC and the amount of L2 produced in oral tasks.

The third research question aimed to address the extent to which WTC fluctuated in online discussion tasks and the factors that contributed to these changes. The between-subject repeated measures ANOVA showed that online WTC fluctuated every 5 min, which is consistent with previous idiodynamic WTC studies with the same statistical procedure (Ducker 2022; MacIntyre and Legato 2011; Nematizadeh 2022a; Sato 2019, 2020). Therefore, a key finding of this study is the dynamic nature of online WTC, as is the case with previous classroom, laboratory-based studies and online context which revealed WTC to be a dynamic construct that can fluctuate on a lesson-to-lesson, task-to-task, and moment-to-moment bases (Cao 2013, 2014; Ducker 2022; Kruk 2020; Nematizadeh and Wood 2019; Pawlak and Mystkowska-Wiertelak 2015; Sato 2019, 2020).

Qualitative analysis of the stimulated recall comments produced nine categories of factors that affected WTC during the online discussion tasks. Online WTC, like in non-digital settings, is dynamic and may be influenced by internal (e.g., individual, linguistic, perceived, organizational factors) and external (e.g., environment, task dynamics, facilitators, coparticipants, topics) forces. More importantly, we observed that WTC changes were sometimes a function of interactions between more than one factor rather than a single factor. This is in line with previous research that found WTC in face-to-face conversational and classroom contexts to emerge from complex interrelationships among individual, affective, cognitive, social, and contextual predictors (Cao 2013, 2014; Ducker 2022; Kang 2005; Nematizadeh 2022b; Peng 2014; Sato 2019, 2020; Yashima et al. 2016; Zhang et al. 2018). For instance, a lack of supporting ideas, which often resulted in low WTC, was compensated for when participants shared personal views, experiences, and interests, or external forces such as situations when they generated ideas as a result of input from their co-participants or facilitators. Sometimes, a high WTC that arose from topical interest or knowledge dropped due to social considerations (e.g., allowing others some discussion time) or task dynamics (e.g., lack of perceived opportunities). In some cases, the participants felt lost during the tasks and expected the facilitators to provide more clarification regarding what was being discussed. This feeling of confusion mostly resulted in a lack of supporting ideas and a reduced WTC. Overall, our findings confirm a critical facet of CDST in online WTC (Cao 2013; Ducker 2022; MacIntyre and Legato 2011; Nematizadeh 2022b; Sato 2019, 2020), which is the interconnectedness between learners’ affective system and cognitive system (De Bot et al. 2007). However, the interrelationship among these underlying factors is so complex that future research is warranted to explore the possibility of organizing these variables into a multi-layered framework (Zhang et al. 2018).

5 Conclusions

The present study represents a mixed methods attempt to investigate the nature of online WTC and the factors contributing to WTC changes during online discussion tasks. To this end, a group of Farsi-speaking ESL participants attended six online discussion tasks. The participants rated their WTC every 5 min and then provided explanations of the WTC changes in a stimulated recall procedure. A repeated-measures ANOVA showed that WTC fluctuates significantly during the online discussion tasks, a key finding to consider online WTC as a dynamic construct. Qualitative analysis of the stimulated recall forms produced nine categories of internal and external forces that interacted and influenced each other, resulting in changes in online WTC at different points in time. An important feature of this study involved the recruitment of unfamiliar participants and facilitators which, we believe, simulated a more authentic communication context.

The study is not without limitations. First, the stimulated recall procedures were done in groups in session where the participants would view the thirty-minute recordings and provide additional commentary on the possible factors contributing to changes. While we decided to do the procedure in groups due to time restrictions, it would have been more effective had we organized one-on-one sessions to allow the participants and researcher to engage in more productive discussions (e.g., semi-structured interviews) and dig deeper into the emerging factors. It should be noted, however, that such sessions would not have been practical and would have imposed great memory demand on the participants as the interval between the discussion task and recall sessions would be longer. In addition, the sessions were all scheduled for the early evening (6:00 pm) due to the participants’ availability; however, in a few instances, the participants indicated that they felt distracted, stressed, depressed, or physically fatigued, all of which lowered their WTC. This might have been due to the timing of the sessions.

Despite the limitations, this study has a number of pedagogical implications to offer. Firstly, this study highlights the interactions between learner internal and external factors that determine fluctuations in WTC. We suggest that teachers should be mindful of this complex interrelationship and encourage students’ willing participation by promoting internal and external factors conducive to communication. Teachers should be aware that students’ WTC levels can drop due to a range of factors, such as fatigue, lack of perceived competence, difficult topics, class mismanagement, and technical issues. They can therefore use facilitating strategies to protect and promote students’ WTC in online classes. Teachers should ensure there is an interaction among students through group work and provide meaningful tasks for them to engage in. With proper facilitation from teachers, students can be equally willing to communicate in online classes as in physical classrooms.


Corresponding author: Yiqian (Katherine) Cao, Trinity College, The University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia, E-mail:

About the authors

Shahin Nematizadeh

Shahin Nematizadeh is a second language teacher trainer, and teaches applied linguistics and EAP courses in Ottawa, Canada. His research interests center around the psychology of language learning, L2 teacher education and student engagement, and L2 speech fluency. He also investigates individual difference variables through a complex dynamic systems perspective. His works have appeared in Canadian Modern Language Review, the Psychology for Language Learning, and Language Testing journals.

Yiqian (Katherine) Cao

Yiqian Katherine Cao is a lecturer at University of Melbourne Trinity College. She holds a PhD from University of Auckland. Her current research interests are individual differences in language acquisition, interactionist approach to language learning, blended learning and teacher professional development. Her works include papers published in TESOL Quarterly, System, Computer Assisted Language Learning, RELC Journal, Sage Open, Cogent Education and Australian Review of Applied Linguistics.

Appendix 1: Group discussion procedure provided to the facilitators

Here’s the instruction for the discussion “moderator/facilitator”.

Thanks for taking the role of a facilitator in this research study. The students will take part in a 30-min online discussion tasks on very typical topics in ESL classes (e.g., online education, public transport, etc.). You will be provided with a list of questions and some images that you can share (in the first minute of the session ONLY) to generate discussion.

Your role is to warm them up to the task and start up the discussion. Once the students start contributing to the discussion, please let the discussion flow naturally and let students contribute at their own pace, ensuring everyone is presented with opportunities to engage. We are trying to create an environment fairly similar to real-life communicative events and whether and how the participants engage or avoid communication in their second language is what we are researching. Once the students run out of ideas for a particular question and remain silent, please pose the next question for the discussion to continue. Please remember not to correct the students when they make errors or when they use their first language during the discussion.

You will be hearing beeping sounds every 5 min and it is for the participants to rate their WTC on grids that they will be provided. We are hoping this won’t be very disruptive. The session will be video-recorded for later interviews and I (Shahin) will be attending but not participating. Thanks very much for your participation in advance and please do not hesitate to email me if you have any questions.

The PI (Nematizadeh 2019) piloted 10 ESL discussion topics with 20 participants using a background knowledge questionnaire (Khabbazbashi 2017). This Likert-style questionnaire contains eight items that elicit information about the participants’ interest, familiarity, background knowledge, and lexical knowledge of the topics. The initial ten topics included shopping, hobbies and free time activities, foods, online versus on-campus education, crime and security, art, advertisement, transportation and travel problems, jobs and lifetime careers, and technology. Each response was assigned a number: strongly agree was assigned +1, agree +0.5, disagree −0.5, and strongly disagree −1. The respondents’ responses to all the items were totalled and averaged, then four topics with the closest ratings were selected. The selected topics included foods, online versus on-campus education, technology, and transportation/travel problems. The PI has access to the pilot stage data and has chosen two additional topics: crime and security and advertisement.

One sample of the material that will be provided to the moderators/facilitators is provided below. We will ask the moderators/facilitators to share this with the participants in the first minute.

Appendix 2: Dynamic WTC rating grid

Appendix 3: Observation Scheme for Measuring WTC. Adopted and modified from Cao (2014) and Al-Murtadha (2019)

Item Participant 1 Participant 2 Participant 3 Participant 4 Participant 5 Participant 6 Participant 7
1 The participant greets the moderator and other participants
2 The participant volunteers an answer (any visible intention e.g., raising a hand, willing facial expression, etc.)
3 The participant gives an answer to the moderator’s question (e.g., provides information, responds to other participants, agrees or disagrees with the moderator or other participants, etc.)
4 The participant asks questions from the moderator or other participants for clarification or curiosity
5 The participant guesses the meaning of an unknown word or questions
6 The participant clarifies a question for other participants or helps other participants to answer a question
7 The participant thanks the facilitator or other participants when they help him/her
8 The participant tries out a difficult form in the target language (vocabulary or grammar structure)
9 The participant presents his/her own opinions
10 The participant says sorry when he/she is wrong

Appendix 4: Online WTC scale

The following questions aim to compare your level of willingness to communicate in traditional face-to-face classroom setting and in online classes. Reflecting on your general impression about online classes and/or your online class experience recently, please respond to the following questions.

(1) unwilling, (2) less willing, (3) equally willing/unwilling, (4) more willing, (5) extremely more willing

Compared to a traditional face-to-face classroom setting, in online classrooms/settings I am …

  1. ……… to engage in discussion with the other participants.

  2. ……… to volunteer to answer a question raised by the teacher/moderator.

  3. ……… to volunteer to answer a question raised by another student/participant.

  4. ……… to volunteer to participate in class activities (e.g., pair-works, reading a passage, etc.)

  5. ……… to ask the teacher and/or another participant a question (for clarification or curiosity).

  6. ……… to guess the meaning of an unknown word and use it while talking.

  7. ……… to try out a difficult or newly learned lexical (word, idiom, or grammatical form).

  8. ……… to present own opinions in class.

  9. ……… to agree or disagree with the teacher’s/moderator’s opinion.

  10. ……… to agree or disagree with another participant’s/student’s opinion.

References

Al-Amrani, Said. 2013. Willingness to communicate by Arab EFL learners: Conceptualisation and realisation. Brisbane, QLD: University of Queensland dissertation.Suche in Google Scholar

Al-Murtadha, Mutahar. 2021. The relationships among self-reported and observed first language and second language willingness to communicate and academic achievement. Language Culture and Curriculum 34(1). 80–94. https://doi.org/10.1080/07908318.2020.1727495.Suche in Google Scholar

Al-Murtadha, Mutahar. 2019. Enhancing EFL learners’ willingness to communicate with visualization and goal-setting activities. Tesol Quarterly 53(1). 133–157. https://doi.org/10.1002/tesq.474.Suche in Google Scholar

Ardiansyah, Showat, Agus Wijayanto & Abdul Asib. 2020. Dynamics of students’ willingness to communicate in English during an online discussion. International Journal of Education and Literacy Studies 2(5). 11–20. https://doi.org/10.32996/ijels.2020.2.5.2.Suche in Google Scholar

Burgoon, Judee. 1976. The unwillingness to communicate scale: Development and validation. Communication Monographs 43. 60–69. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637757609375916.Suche in Google Scholar

Cao, Yiqian & Jenefer Philp. 2006. Interactional context and willingness to communicate: A comparison of behaviour in whole class, group and dyadic interaction. System 34(4). 480–493. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2006.05.002.Suche in Google Scholar

Cao, Yiqian. 2011. Investigating situational willingness to communicate within second language classrooms from an ecological perspective. System 39(4). 468–479. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2011.10.016.Suche in Google Scholar

Cao, Yiqian. 2012. Willingness to communicate and communication quality in ESL classrooms. TESL Reporter 45(1). 17–36.Suche in Google Scholar

Cao, Yiqian. 2013. Exploring dynamism in willingness to communicate: A longitudinal case study. Australian Review of Applied Linguistics 36(2). 160–176. https://doi.org/10.1075/aral.36.2.03cao.Suche in Google Scholar

Cao, Yiqian. 2014. A socio-cognitive perspective on L2 classroom willingness to communicate. Tesol Quarterly 48(4). 789–814. https://doi.org/10.1002/tesq.155.Suche in Google Scholar

Cao, Yiqian & Wei Wei. 2019. An investigation of willingness to communicate from an English as an International Language (EIL) perspective: The case of Macao. System 87. 1–11.10.1016/j.system.2019.102149Suche in Google Scholar

Clément, Richard. 1986. Second language proficiency and acculturation: An investigation of the effects of language status and individual characteristics. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 5. 271–290. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927x8600500403.Suche in Google Scholar

De Bot, Kees, Wander Lowie & Verspoor Marjolijn. 2007. A dynamic systems theory approach to second language acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 10. 7–12. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1366728906002732.Suche in Google Scholar

Dörnyei, Zoltan. 2009. Individual differences: Interplay of learner characteristics and learning environment. Language Learning 59. 230–248. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2009.00542.x.Suche in Google Scholar

Dörnyei, Zoltan & Judit Kormos. 2000. The role of individual and social variables in oral task performance. Language Teaching Research 4(3). 275–300. https://doi.org/10.1191/136216800125096.Suche in Google Scholar

Dörnyei, Zoltan, Peter MacIntyre & Alastair Henry. 2015. Motivational dynamics in language learning. Applied Linguistics 36(5). 648–651.Suche in Google Scholar

Ducker, Nathan Thomas. 2022. Bridging the gap between willingness to communicate and learner talk. The Modern Language Journal 106(1). 216–244. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12764.Suche in Google Scholar

Ebadi, Saman & Marjan Ebadijalal. 2020. The effect of Google Expeditions virtual reality on EFL learners’ willingness to communicate and oral proficiency. Computer Assisted Language Learning 35. 1975–2000. https://doi.org/10.1080/09588221.2020.1854311.Suche in Google Scholar

Elahi Shirvan, Majid, Hassan Khajavy Gholam, Peter MacIntyre & Tahereh Taherian. 2019. A meta-analysis of L2 willingness to communicate and its three high-evidence correlates. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 48. 1241–1267. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-019-09656-9.Suche in Google Scholar

Freiermuth, Mark & Hsin-chou Huang. 2012. Bringing Japan and Taiwan closer electronically: A look at an intercultural online synchronic chat task and its effect on motivation. Language Teaching Research 16(1). 61–88. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168811423341.Suche in Google Scholar

Freiermuth, Mark & Douglas Jarrell. 2006. Willingness to communicate: Can online chat help? International Journal of Applied Linguistics 16(2). 189–212. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1473-4192.2006.00113.x.Suche in Google Scholar

Fushino, Kumiko. 2010. Causal relationships between communication confidence, beliefs about group work, and willingness to communicate in foreign language group work. Tesol Quarterly 44(4). 700–724. https://doi.org/10.5054/tq.2010.235993.Suche in Google Scholar

Gass, Susan & Alison Mackey. 2000. Stimulated recall methodology in second language research. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Suche in Google Scholar

Gaddis, John Lewis. 2002. The landscape of history. New York: Oxford University Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Jabbari, Nasser & Zohreh Eslami. 2019. Second language learning in the context of massively multiplayer online games: A scoping review. ReCALL 31(1). 92–113. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0958344018000058.Suche in Google Scholar

Kang, Su-Ja. 2005. Dynamic emergence of situational willingness to communicate in a second language. System 33. 277–292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2004.10.004.Suche in Google Scholar

Khabbazbashi, Nahal. 2017. Topic and background knowledge effects on performance in speaking assessment. Language Testing 34(1). 23–48.10.1177/0265532215595666Suche in Google Scholar

Khajavy, Gholam Hassan & Behzad Ghonsooly. 2017. Predictors of willingness to read in English: Testing a model based on possible selves and self-confidence. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 38(10). 871–885. https://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2017.1284853.Suche in Google Scholar

Kruk, Mariusz. 2020. Dynamicity of perceived willingness to communicate, motivation, boredom and anxiety in second life: The case of two advanced learners of English. Computer Assisted Language Learning 35. 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1080/09588221.2019.1677722.Suche in Google Scholar

Larsen-Freeman, Diane & Lynne Cameron. 2008. Complex systems and applied linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Lee, Ju-Seong. 2019. EFL students’ views of willingness to communicate in the extramural digital context. Computer Assisted Language Learning 32(7). 692–712. https://doi.org/10.1080/09588221.2018.1535509.Suche in Google Scholar

Lee, Ju-Seong & Kilryoung Lee. 2020. Affective factors, virtual intercultural experiences, and L2 willingness to communicate in in-class, out-of-class, and digital settings. Language Teaching Research 24(6). 813–833. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168819831408.Suche in Google Scholar

Lopez, Iris Y. & Ngoc H. Bui. 2014. Acculturation and linguistic factors on international students’ self-esteem and language confidence. Journal of International Students 4(4). 314–329. https://doi.org/10.32674/jis.v4i4.451.Suche in Google Scholar

MacIntyre, Peter. 1994. Variables underlying willingness to communicate: A causal analysis. Communication Research Reports 11(2). 135–142. https://doi.org/10.1080/08824099409359951.Suche in Google Scholar

MacIntyre, Peter. 2007. Willingness to communicate in the second language: Understanding the decision to speak as a volitional process. The Modern Language Journal 91. 564–576. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.2007.00623.x.Suche in Google Scholar

MacIntyre, Peter. 2020. Expanding the theoretical base for the dynamics of willingness to communicate. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching 10(1). 111–131. https://doi.org/10.14746/ssllt.2020.10.1.6.Suche in Google Scholar

MacIntyre, Peter D., Susan C. Baker, Richard Clément & Sarah Conrod. 2001. Willingness to communicate, social support, and language-learning orientations of immersion students. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 23(3). 369–388. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0272263101003035.Suche in Google Scholar

MacIntyre, Peter & James Legatto. 2011. A dynamic system approach to willingness to communicate: Developing an idiodynamic method to capture rapidly changing affect. Applied Linguistics 32. 149–171. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amq037.Suche in Google Scholar

MacIntyre, Peter & Lanxi Wang. 2021. Willingness to communicate in the L2 about meaningful photos: Application of the pyramid model of WTC. Language Teaching Research 25(6). 878–898. https://doi.org/10.1177/13621688211004645.Suche in Google Scholar

MacIntyre, Peter, Patricia Babin & Richard Clément. 1999. Willingness to communicate: Antecedents and consequences. Communication Quarterly 47. 215–229. https://doi.org/10.1080/01463379909370135.Suche in Google Scholar

MacIntyre, Peter, Richard Clément, Zoltan Dörnyei & Kimberly Noels. 1998. Conceptualizing willingness to communicate in an L2: A situated model of confidence and affiliation. The Modern Language Journal 82. 545–562. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.1998.tb05543.x.Suche in Google Scholar

McCroskey, James. 1992. Reliability and validity of the willingness to communicate scale. Communication Quarterly 40(1). 16–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/01463379209369817.Suche in Google Scholar

McCroskey, James & Elaine Baer. 1985. Willingness to communicate: The construct and its measurement. Denver, CO: Paper presented at the Speech Communication Association Convention.Suche in Google Scholar

McCroskey, James & Virginia Richmond. 1991. Willingness to communicate: A cognitive view. In M. Booth-Butterfield (ed.), Communication, cognition, and anxiety, 19–37. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Suche in Google Scholar

Nematizadeh, Shahin. 2019. Willingness to communicate and second language speech fluency: A complex dynamic systems perspective. Ottawa, ON: Carleton University Doctoral Dissertation.Suche in Google Scholar

Nematizadeh, Shahin & David Wood. 2019. Willingness to communicate and L2 speech fluency: An investigation of affective and cognitive dynamics. The Canadian Modern Language Review 73(3). 197–215. https://doi.org/10.3138/cmlr.2017-0146.Suche in Google Scholar

Nematizadeh, Shahin. 2022a. Investigating the dynamics of change in second language willingness to communicate. OLBI Journal 12(1). 207–232. https://doi.org/10.18192/olbij.v12i1.5997.Suche in Google Scholar

Nematizadeh, Shahin. 2022b. A per-second investigation of the interconnectedness between linguistic and cognitive factors underlying L2 willingness to communicate. Journal for the Psychology of Language Learning 4(2). 1–14. https://doi.org/10.52598/jpll/4/2/8.Suche in Google Scholar

Pawlak, Miroslaw & Anna Mystkowska-Wiertelak. 2015. Investigating the dynamic nature of L2 willingness to communicate. System 50. 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2015.02.001.Suche in Google Scholar

Pawlak, Miroslaw, Anna Mystkowska-Wiertelak & Jakub Bielak. 2015. Investigating the nature of classroom willingness to communicate (WTC): A micro-perspective. Language Teaching Research 20(5). 654–671. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168815609615.Suche in Google Scholar

Peng, Jian-E. 2014. Willingness to communicate in the Chinese EFL university classroom: An ecological perspective. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.10.21832/9781783091560Suche in Google Scholar

Peng, Jian-E. 2020. Willing silence and silent willingness to communicate (WTC) in the Chinese EFL classroom: A dynamic systems perspective. In J. King & S. Harumi (eds.), East Asian perspectives on silence in English language education, 143–165. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.10.21832/9781788926775-013Suche in Google Scholar

Saldaña, Johnny. 2013. The coding manual for qualitative researchers. Los Angeles: Sage Publications.Suche in Google Scholar

Sato, Rintaro. 2019. Fluctuations in an EFL teacher’s willingness to communicate in an English medium lesson: An observational case study in Japan. Innovation in Language Learning and Teaching 13. 105–117. https://doi.org/10.1080/17501229.2017.1375506.Suche in Google Scholar

Sato, Rintaro. 2020. Examining fluctuations in the WTC of Japanese EFL speakers: Language proficiency, affective and conditional factors. Language Teaching Research 24. 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168820977825.Suche in Google Scholar

Yashima, Tomoko. 2002. Willingness to communicate in a second language: The Japanese EFL context. The Modern Language Journal 86(1). 54–67. https://doi.org/10.1111/1540-4781.00136.Suche in Google Scholar

Yashima, Tomoko, Peter MacIntyre & Maiko Ikeda. 2016. Situated willingness to communicate in an L2: Interplay of individual characteristics and context. Language Teaching Research 22(1). 115–137. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168816657851.Suche in Google Scholar

Zhang, Jiayi, Nadin Beckmann & Jens Beckmann. 2018. To talk or not to talk: A review of situational antecedents of willingness to communicate in the second language classroom. System 72. 226–239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2018.01.003.Suche in Google Scholar

Zhou, Li, Yiheng Xi & Katja Lochtman. 2020. The relationship between second language competence and willingness to communicate: The moderating effect of foreign language anxiety. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 44(10). 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2020.1801697.Suche in Google Scholar

Received: 2022-05-01
Accepted: 2023-04-04
Published Online: 2023-04-21
Published in Print: 2024-11-26

© 2023 the author(s), published by De Gruyter, Berlin/Boston

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Artikel in diesem Heft

  1. Frontmatter
  2. Research Articles
  3. “Am I really abroad?” The informal language contact and social networks of Chinese foundation students in the UK
  4. Comprehension of English articles by Korean learners of L2 English and L3 Spanish
  5. Motivation and growth in kanji proficiency: a longitudinal study using latent growth curve modeling
  6. The impact of textual enhancement on the acquisition of third person possessive pronouns by child EFL learners
  7. Domain-general grit and domain-specific grit: conceptual structures, measurement, and associations with the achievement of German as a foreign language
  8. The different effects of the ideal L2 self and intrinsic motivation on reading performance via engagement among young Chinese second-language learners
  9. Image-schema-based-instruction enhanced L2 construction learning with the optimal balance between attention to form and meaning
  10. Investigating willingness to communicate in synchronous group discussion tasks: one step closer towards authentic communication
  11. Investigating willingness to communicate vis-à-vis learner talk in a low-proficiency EAP classroom in the UK study-abroad context
  12. Functions, sociocultural explanations and conversational influence of discourse markers: focus on zenme shuo ne in L2 Chinese
  13. The impact of abdominal enhancement techniques on L1 Spanish, Japanese and Mandarin speakers’ English pronunciation
  14. Discourse competence across band scores: an analysis of speaking performance in the General English Proficiency Test
  15. Videoed storytelling in primary education EFL: exploring trainees’ digital shift
  16. Competing factors in SLA: how the CASP model of SLA explains the acquisition of English restrictive relative clauses by native speakers of Arabic and Korean
  17. Factors behind L2 English learners’ performance of oppositional speech acts: a look at pragmatic-related episodes (PREs) during thinking aloud
  18. Revisiting after-class boredom via exploratory structural equation modeling
  19. Exploring aural vocabulary knowledge for TOEIC as a language exit requirement in higher education in Taiwan
  20. Grammatical gender assignment in L3 versus L4 Swedish: a pseudo-longitudinal study
  21. Strategic reading comprehension in L2 and L3: assuming relative interdependence within Cummins’ linguistic interdependence hypothesis
  22. Incidental collocational learning from reading-while-listening and the impact of synchronized textual enhancement
  23. Learnability of L2 collocations and L1 influence on L2 collocational representations of Japanese learners of English
  24. The use of interlanguage pragmatic learning strategies (IPLS) by L2 learners: the impact of age, gender, language learning experience, and L2 proficiency levels
  25. “They forget and forget all the time.” The complexity of teaching adult deaf emergent readers print literacy
  26. Exploring the learning benefits of collaborative writing in L2 Chinese: a product-oriented perspective
  27. Investigating the effects of varying Accelerative Integrated Method instruction on spoken recall accuracy: a case study with junior primary learners of French
  28. Testing a model of EFL teachers’ work engagement: the roles of teachers’ professional identity, L2 grit, and foreign language teaching enjoyment
  29. The combined effects of a higher-level verb distribution and verb semantics on second language learners’ restriction of L2 construction generalization
  30. Insights from an empirical study on communicative functions and L1 use during conceptual mediation in L2 peer interaction
  31. On the acquisition of tense and agreement in L2 English by adult speakers of L1 Chinese
Heruntergeladen am 9.9.2025 von https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/iral-2022-0092/html
Button zum nach oben scrollen