Abstract
Although the predictive effect of emotion on language achievement has been substantially established, little is known about whether language achievement could, in turn, shape a constellation of emotions in second language/L2 learning, especially in the field of languages other than English. Given this, grounded on the control-value theory, this tentative study aims to fill the gap by investigating the predictive effect of language achievement on emotions (enjoyment, boredom, and anxiety) and digging into the mediating relationships between them in the underlying L2 Chinese learning mechanism through structural equation modeling. Three hundred and seven (N = 307) young students from a cram school in New Zealand participated in this study. The results indicated that students’ learning achievement, directly and indirectly, predicted three frequently experienced emotions in the multiple mediation model. In addition, positive and negative emotions interacted with each other in the L2 Chinese learning context. The finding of this study validated and extended the application of control-value theory in L2 Chinese learning.
1 Introduction
The revolution of positive psychology in Second Language Acquisition (SLA) has led to an expansion of an array of irrational factors due to their significant role in catalyzing language flourishing (Derakhshan 2022; Derakhshan et al. 2022; MacIntyre and Mercer 2014; Oxford 2016; Wang et al. 2021). In light of this, there has been a hype surrounding multiple emotions in recent years in language learning and teaching promoted by the emotional wave (Derakhshan et al. 2021a, 2021b; Tsang and Dewaele 2023; Zhao and Wang 2023a). More specifically, light has been shed from a single exclusive negative anxiety to a wider spectrum of both positive and negative emotions in the field, for instance, the integrating of two emotions (e.g., enjoyment and anxiety/boredom) and a holistic constellation of emotions (Li 2021a; Dewaele and Li 2020, 2022; Zhao et al. 2023) due to the hindering or contributing effects on academic learning.
In this prosperous developing phase of emotion (Li 2021b), the “three-body” emotions, enjoyment, boredom, and anxiety, have been considered as the most pervasive and popular emotions that learners might experience in the educational setting among the emerging diverse emotions (Li and Wei 2023; Tsang and Dewaele 2023). Against this backdrop, a bulk of empirical studies established the relationship between emotions and language achievement grounded on the control-value theory (CVT) across various contexts, principally in the English language domain (Wang and Li 2022; Zare et al. 2023; Zhao and Wang 2023b). Some studies have established the positive radiation effect of enjoyment while the adverse effect of anxiety and boredom on learners’ English learning achievement (Alrabai and Alamer 2022; Li and Wei 2023). Other studies, for example, Dewaele and Li (2022) found that English language achievement predicted anxiety and enjoyment in Saudi Arabia and Chinese contexts, respectively.
The present study differs from previous studies in a number of perspectives. To start with, despite the cornerstone CVT emphasizing the bi-directional reciprocal network between emotions and academic achievement (Pekrun 2006), scant attention has been paid to the reverse loop, the path from language achievement on diverse emotions in L2 learning from a positive psychological perspective. Additionally, this study embraces a constellation of frequently experienced positive and negative emotions in the less-featured languages other than English (LOTE), specifically in the Chinese learning field, to extend our understanding of achievement emotions from a holistic view. Furthermore, instead of focusing on the over-dominated compulsory education context, this study explores this pattern in an L2 learning mechanism in the underexplored cram school context. Therefore, this study will enhance our understanding concerning the contribution of L2 achievement on those emotions and the expansion of theories in the positive psychology discipline.
Given this, this study was designed to examine the influence of Chinese language achievement on L2 enjoyment, boredom, and anxiety in a sample of 307 elementary and middle school students in the New Zealand context with structural equation modeling (SEM). It is expected that the current study serves as a starting point to provide some empirical evidence for the complex relationship between language achievement and emotions, further broaden the knowledge scope about the applicability of the CVT in the Chinese learning context, and finally, promote the progress of positive psychology in SLA.
2 Literature review
2.1 The theoretical foundation of emotion
The advance of positive psychology contributed to the birth of the theoretical framework of emotions, further stimulating an increasing body of empirical studies concerning the role of emotions across different language learning contexts. Surrounding emotions, the groundbreaking broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson 2004) and the control-value theory (Pekrun 2006) provided insights into the pivotal role of emotions in students’ psychological state and academic performance.
The broaden-and-build theory not only differentiated emotions from two valences (positive and negative) but also promoted the merit of positive emotion from an individual’s cognitive, psychological, social, and professional aspects. An exponential body of research has empirically confirmed the benefit of enjoyment and the consequence of boredom and anxiety in language learning (Botes et al. 2022a; Derakhshan 2022; Derakhshan et al. 2022; Li and Han 2022; Wang and Li 2022).
Grounded on but different from the former theory, the CVT emphasizes the complex network among antecedents, achievement emotions, and learning achievement in the academic setting (Pekrun and Perry 2014). Firstly, the CVT diversifies the conceptualization of emotions based on the three-dimensional taxonomy approach, i.e., defining emotions from valence (pleasant/unpleasant), activation (the degree of physiological arousal), and object focus (activity-related/outcome-related) (Pekrun 2006). Following this approach, anxiety could be identified as a negative, activating, and outcome emotion, enjoyment as a positive, activating, and activity emotion, and boredom as a negative, deactivating, and outcome emotion. Secondly, CVT confirms the merit of achievement emotions on activities/outcomes, whereby it affects ‘the cognitive, motivational, and regulatory’ perspectives (Pekrun 2006, p. 326). Of note, achievement emotions are domain-specific which might perform to varying degrees across languages and contexts. Thirdly, the CVT emphasizes a bi-directional network between achievement emotions and language achievement. In other words, emotions and academic performance are interacted and mutually affect each other in a circular loop. Some recent work empirically echoed the complex network between emotions and learning achievement (Li 2021a; Li and Han 2022; Li and Wei 2023). This theory theoretically rationalizes the conceptual framework of the current study.
Prior work, however, converges into one fact that the adoption of the CVT in SLA is largely confined to English language learning, limiting its applicability across languages and contexts. In addition, it remains under-investigated whether the merit of language achievement on a collective of emotions could be endorsed. Thus, this study aims to extend the CVT by scrutinizing the influence of L2 Chinese achievement on enjoyment, boredom, and anxiety in a less-featured Chinese learning context in New Zealand.
2.2 Enjoyment, boredom, and anxiety
The movement of emotions has progressed along with positive psychology in SLA in three stages (Li 2021b). In the embryonic stage, researchers mainly focused on anxiety due to its debilitative role in language learning (Horwitz et al. 1986), in addition to the slow development of theoretical frameworks and instruments in the field. In the later developing stage, some groundbreaking studies shifted attention from pure anxiety to enjoyment or the combination of anxiety and enjoyment in English language learning (MacIntyre and Mercer 2014). For example, Dewaele and MacIntyre (2014, 2016 associated enjoyment with anxiety and regarded them as ‘two faces of Janus’ (2014, p. 237) and the ‘right and left feet’ (2016, p. 215) of language learning. In addition, Dewaele and his co-author developed a 21-item foreign language enjoyment scale and adapted a short version of an 8-item foreign language anxiety scale. Jiang and Dewaele (2019) and Li et al. (2019) further validated the short version of the foreign language anxiety scale and developed an 11-item foreign language enjoyment scale, promoting an exponential body of ensuing emotional studies. In recent years, boredom has gradually emerged in the field (Derakhshan 2022; Derakhshan et al. 2022; Li 2021b; Zhao and Wang 2023a). The integration of enjoyment and boredom/anxiety (Alrabai and Alamer 2022; Dewaele and MacIntyre 2022; Dewaele and Li 2022; Kruk et al. 2022; Zhao and Wang 2023b), and even the combination of enjoyment, boredom, and anxiety (e.g., Alrabai 2022; Dewaele et al. 2022a, 2022b; Shao et al. 2023; Wang and Li 2022) have emerged and continuously attracted the attention of scholars.
The relationship between enjoyment and boredom/anxiety has also been well studied in L2 learning. For instance, anxiety and boredom were negatively associated with enjoyment, and two negative emotions positively correlated with each other in English language learning among 484 Saudi Arabia university learners (Alrabai and Alamer 2022). This relationship has also been validated among 504 (middle school) and 783 (middle and high school) Chinese ethnic minority students (Zhao and Wang 2023a, 2023b). Similarly, this pattern has also been found in a bulk of studies (e.g., Tsang and Dewaele 2023; Wang and Li 2022). Moreover, it is significant to note that this configuration has been stretched and validated in the online education context for Chinese and European learners (Li and Han 2022; Resnik and Dewaele 2023).
Informed by the prior work, it could be inferred that no prior studies have substantiated the contribution of language achievement on a collective emotion in the L2 Chinese learning mechanism, necessitating further investigation.
2.3 The relationship between enjoyment, boredom, anxiety, and language achievement
Inspired by the promotion of CVT, a blast of research concerning the relationship between emotions and language achievement has emerged in recent years. There is a consensus that enjoyment was positively correlated, while anxiety and boredom were negatively correlated with language learning achievement across different settings (Li and Wei 2023; Tsang and Dewaele 2023; Zhao and Wang 2023b).
For instance, Derakhshan et al. (2022) suggested that classroom atmosphere and boredom directly predicated students’ engagement among 287 Iran universities English learners, and the negative effect of boredom has been validated in the online Iranian EFL context (Derakhshan et al. 2021a, 2021b). Botes et al. (2022b) found that anxiety and enjoyment statistically predicted self-perceived foreign language achievement in a polynomial regression for 1,039 online learners. Their further meta-analysis based on 56 studies disclosed the positive predictive effect of enjoyment on WTC and actual and self-perceived language achievement. A longitudinal study by Li and Wei (2023) confirmed the correlation of anxiety, boredom, and enjoyment with English learning achievement and stressed the endurability of enjoyment among 954 secondary students in rural China. Congruently, Zhao and Wang (2023a) affirmed the predictive effect of enjoyment (positive) and boredom (negative) on English language achievement among Chinese ethnic minority learners. The same results have also appeared in Li and Han (2022) as well as Wang and Li (2022), empirically echoing the CVT.
It is intriguing that the significant association between emotion and language achievement may fluctuate if a collective emotion simultaneously enters a sophisticated model. For example, Li and Han (2022) found that enjoyment significantly and positively correlated with both actual and self-perceived online language achievement among 348 Chinese university EFL learners. However, only anxiety predicted actual language achievement, while enjoyment and boredom predicted self-perceived language achievement in the regression model. In the same online learning context, Wang and Li (2022) stated that participants’ actual performance was predicted by enjoyment and anxiety, yet boredom did not function as a significant predictor among 819 Chinese university learners in the stepwise regression analysis. Similarly, Dewaele et al. (2022) identified only anxiety, neither enjoyment nor boredom, significantly predicted English achievement based on 332 international samples in the exploratory structural equation modeling. Thus, the current study will explore their complex relationship by employing an advanced rigid model.
Despite the significant path from emotion to language achievement drawing sufficient attention from researchers, scarce attention has been paid to the reverse loop in terms of the predictive effect of language achievement on emotion in language learning. Dewaele and MacIntyre (2014) confirmed the instigation of self-perceived English achievement on enjoyment among an international sample that those perceived as more capable of English language learning are prone to experience more language enjoyment. Li et al. (2020) investigated the role of actual English achievement on general emotions within the Chinese context. The results indicated that English actual achievement was the main cause and two emotions. Alamer and Lee (2021) found that L2 achievement predicted anxiety but not the opposite through cross-lagged panel analysis among 160 English university learners in Saudi Arabia. Focusing on the domain-specific emotions, Dewaele and Li (2022) discovered that enjoyment was co-predicted by self-rating language competence and the domain reading, grammar, and speaking competence, while anxiety was predicted by speaking, grammar, self-perceived and actual competence among 1,415 middle school Chinese students.
Taken together, it is not hard to find that researchers mainly focus on the one-way pattern, i.e., from emotions to language achievement in English language learning. Very few studies examined the reverse loop from language achievement to emotions in the L2 mechanism. Specifically, no research has been conducted to examine the predictive effect of L2 achievement on collective enjoyment, boredom, and anxiety by considering the mediating paths in a model under the Chinese learning context. Such insufficiency might cause the incomplete understanding concerning the role of emotions in L2 learning. Furthermore, language educators might not provide students with adequate learning strategies and timely emotion regulations. Thus, to offer learners with a cheerful and fruitful L2 learning experience, this study will offer both empirical and theoretical insights by mitigating the gap in L2 Chinese language learning.
2.4 The present study
Rooted in the CVT, there should be a bi-directional reciprocal pattern between emotions and learning achievement in the educational setting. The fact is that only the predictive effect of emotion on language achievement has largely been investigated. The reverse loop, however, remains largely unknown in L2 learning. Given this, the present study proposed the hypothesized model to address the following two research questions (see Figure 1).

The hypothesized L2 Chinese learning mechanism.
RQ1:
: Does actual language achievement (ALA) directly predict anxiety, boredom, and enjoyment in the proposed model?
RQ2:
Does anxiety and boredom play as mediators in the proposed model?
3 Methodology
3.1 Participants and contexts
Embracing a convenience sampling approach, 307 pupils from 14 elementary and secondary schools in New Zealand, including 157 (51.1 %) females and 150 (48.9 %) males, participated in the current study. The participants were aged from 9 to 20 (M = 11.48, SD = 2.20). The present study invited participants from three different educational backgrounds to obtain a fuller picture. Specifically, 175 of them (57 %) were elementary school students, 84 (27.4 %) were junior high school students, and the remaining 48 (15.6 %) were senior high school cohorts. In terms of the language profile, most of them (n = 129, 42 %) classified their Chinese competence as intermediate, followed by beginner-intermediate (n = 103, 33.6 %), advanced (n = 43, 14 %), and beginners (n = 32, 10.4 %). Among them, 225 (73.29 %) were New Zealanders, followed by 76 Chinese (24.76 %) and 6 others (1.95 %). It was worth mentioning that over 95 % of participants were Chinese heritage students. Of all the participants, 209 (68.08 %) of them speak Chinese as L1, and the remaining 98 (31.92 %) speak English as L1.
New Zealand is one of the largest recipients of migrants in the Asia-Pacific region. Asians are the third largest ethnic group in New Zealand, and the Chinese account for the majority of the Asian group. It is necessary to point out that Chinese descendants develop as the fastest-growing non-indigenous ethnic group due to the immigrant policy. Two-thirds of the Chinese immigrants in New Zealand reside in Auckland, making Chinese Mandarin one of the most extensively used languages there (Wang and Chik 2022). In view of such a need, Chinese language cram schools are prevalent among Chinese heritage students. Parents with L2 academic demands on their children are likely to choose an appropriate cram school for their children to enhance their language achievement.
However, such a large group of Chinese heritage learners in New Zealand and the cram school learning context have yet to attract attention in the field, necessitating the present investigation to understand the contributing effect of academic achievement on enjoyment, anxiety, and boredom in the current study’s context.
3.2 Instruments
The present study employed a two-section composite questionnaire to obtain both the demographic information (age, gender, nationality, educational background, and self-perceived and actual language proficiency) and the rating items on emotions (anxiety, enjoyment, and boredom) from participants in the New Zealand context. To enhance the accessibility and reliability of the questionnaire, all items were provided bilingually both in Chinese and English. Before distributing the questionnaire, two professors in applied linguistics checked the content validity of the questionnaire (Creswell and Poth 2018). After revising some expressions and wordings, the questionnaire was officially given to the participants.
Considering the Chinese language proficiency and the age profile of participants, the current study has controlled the number of items, that is, extracted and adapted items from previous scales, which complied with the suggestions made by Kenny (1979). The item pool of the questionnaire was selected based on item response theory (IRT) (Embretson and Reise 2000). IRT offers powerful techniques for questionnaire design, including item selection and scale shortening (Fayers 2004, p. 715). To measure three emotional variables, all items were rated on a standard 7-point Likert scale from the lowest 1 (totally disagree) to the highest 7 (totally agree). Reliability analysis, exploratory factor analysis (EFA), and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) were performed in succession. The factor loading of each item and the discriminant and convergent validity were illustrated in Tables 1 and 2.
Factor loading of each item (N = 307).
Items | Factors | ||
---|---|---|---|
Enjoyment | Boredom | Anxiety | |
I enjoy learning Chinese. | 0.926 | ||
Learning Chinese is fun. | 0.881 | ||
I don’t get bored with learning Chinese. | 0.835 | ||
I’ve learned some interesting things in the Chinese learning process. | 0.754 | ||
It is difficult for me to concentrate in the Chinese class. | 0.854 | ||
I get restless and can’t wait for the Chinese class to end. | 0.846 | ||
The Chinese class is boring. | 0.826 | ||
I always feel sleepy in the Chinese classes. | 0.791 | ||
I am only physically in the classroom, while my mind is wandering outside the Chinese class. | 0.774 | ||
I get nervous when I am speaking Chinese in the class. | 0.861 | ||
I start to panic when being asked to speak Chinese without preparation in class. | 0.830 | ||
It embarrasses me to volunteer answers in the Chinese class. | 0.807 | ||
Even if I am well prepared for the Chinese listening and speaking class, I feel anxious about it. | 0.702 | ||
Cronbach’s alpha | 0.872 | 0.876 | 0.809 |
Convergent and discriminant validity of the constructs (N = 307).
Constructs | Convergent validity | Discriminant validity | Descriptive statistics | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
CR | AVE | Enjoyment | Anxiety | Boredom | Mean | SD | |
Enjoyment | 0.981 | 0.586 | 0.815 | 4.68 | 1.35 | ||
Anxiety | 0.955 | 0.606 | −0.440** | 0.778 | 3.06 | 1.48 | |
Boredom | 0.981 | 0.586 | −0.702** | 0.635** | 0.766 | 2.87 | 1.41 |
-
CR = composite reliability; AVE = average variance extracted; SD = standard deviation; the bold fonts values are square roots of AVE.
L2 enjoyment scale. To assess the perception of enjoyment in learning Chinese, the present study extracted and adapted four items from the Chinese version of the Foreign Language Enjoyment scale by Li et al. (2018). A sample item is “I enjoy learning Chinese”.
L2 boredom scale. Consisting of five items, the L2 boredom scale was adapted from the Foreign Language Learning Boredom Scale by Li et al. (2021). The sample item starts with “I always feel sleepy in the Chinese classes”.
L2 anxiety scale. To measure the anxiety level of the participants, the present study adapted four items of a short version of the Foreign Language Classroom Anxiety Scale (Dewaele and MacIntyre 2014, 2016). A case item is “Even if I am well prepared for the Chinese listening and speaking class, I feel anxious about it.”
Actual L2 Chinese language achievement (ALA). The current study used participants’ midterm test scores to represent their actual language achievement in the first section of the questionnaire survey. To ensure the accuracy of responses, each classroom teacher was invited to double-check the authenticity of examination results.
3.3 Data collection
The present study obtained the official ethical document from the Ethics Committee of the author’s university. Before officially distributing the questionnaire, the authors contacted the principal and each classroom instructor of the target school in New Zealand for collaboration and approval of the study. Upon receiving their permission, the authors informed the aim, procedure, time duration of answering the questionnaire, and their right to withdraw during the data collection. The anonymity and confidentiality of participants were guaranteed, and the data would never be revealed to a third party. Written consent forms from all participants were obtained. As some participants were under the age of 18, we also sent text messages to their parents for their consent before conducting the survey. Finally, a paper-and-pencil form questionnaire was distributed to participants at the end of their formal class from December 15th to 30th, 2022.
3.4 Data analysis
After collecting the questionnaire, five random missing data (n = 5, 1.6 %) of the actual language achievement were identified, and then the imputation technique with the series mean method was embraced to replace the missing data. Subsequently, to ensure the following stages of statistical analysis, the normality of distribution (skewness and kurtosis), the reliability (Cronbach’s alpha) and validity (the exploratory factor analysis/EFA and the confirmatory factor analysis/CFA) of enjoyment, boredom, and anxiety were rigorously examined. Three hundred and seven (N = 307) sample cases were used for the following analysis with SPSS v26 and Amos v26.
The complex relationship between multiple emotions and L2 Chinese learning achievement was investigated using structural equation modeling (SEM) with the bootstrapping technique. SEM can compensate for the shortcomings of traditional regression statistical methods, allowing analysis of the causal relationship among multiple factors in a model (Creswell and Poth 2018).
4 Results
4.1 Preliminary analysis
Before reporting the results of the research question, we conducted a series of preliminary analyses to establish the assumptions for subsequent analysis. The results of descriptive statistical analysis showed that the skewness and kurtosis of all the items fell within ±2, indicating the normal distribution of the data set (George and Mallery 2020). Further, the EFA presented that each construct has good internal reliability, with Cronbach’s alpha above 0.70 (Hair et al. 2019) and factor loading above 0.702 (see Table 1).
After that, the CFA was performed to fulfill the assumption for SEM analysis. The results of the CFA indicated that all the constructs had good model fit indices (χ 2/df < 3, comparative fit index (CFI) > 0.90; Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) > 0.90; root means square error of approximation (RMSEA) < 0.08; and standardized root means square residual (SRMR) < 0.08) (Kline 2011). In addition, the three emotional constructs of the current study, as shown in Table 2, had good convergent validity with composite reliability (CR) > 0.70 and the average variance extracted (AVE) > 0.50. The constructs also had discriminant validity as the square roots of the AVE were higher than the correlation coefficients between the corresponding factors (Hair et al. 2019).
Table 2 illustrates the level of a constellation of emotions in L2 Chinese learning. The results indicated that New Zealander L2 Chinese participants experienced a high level of enjoyment (M = 4.68, SD = 1.35), a moderate to a high level of anxiety (M = 3.06, SD = 1.48), and a low to moderate level of boredom (M = 2.87, SD = 1.41). In addition, the frequency of descriptive analysis reported that the proportion of participants who indicated a moderate to high level of enjoyment (86.97 %) was twice as students’ percept of boredom (41.04 %) and anxiety (43.97 %). The perception of anxiety was higher than boredom in the moderate to high range in learning Chinese.
4.2 The direct and indirect effects of ALA on emotions
Before examining the direct and indirect paths in the underlying L2 Chinese learning mechanism, the goodness of fit indices of the proposed SEM model was firstly evaluated. The results indicated that the model fitted the sample data excellently (χ 2/df = 2.314, CFI = 0.957; TLI = 0.946; RMSEA = 0.066; and SRMR = 0.050), ensuring the subsequent statistical analysis. Then, the SEM was analyzed, and Figure 2 illustrates the final results.

The finalized model of L2 Chinese learning mechanism.
Direct effects. This study assessed the predictive effect of actual learning achievement on each emotion and the direct path among different emotions in the SEM with Amos v26. As shown in Table 3, ALA significantly predicted anxiety (H1: β = −0.288, p < 0.001) and enjoyment (H3: β = 0.135, p < 0.01). In addition, anxiety was a significant positive predictor of boredom (H5: β = 0.693, p < 0.001), and boredom was a significant negative predictor of enjoyment (H6: β = −0.710, p < 0.001). To put it another way, participants with a higher level of Chinese language achievement are likely to have a lower perception of anxiety but a higher level of enjoyment. Anxiety was positively correlated with boredom, while boredom, in turn, was negatively related to enjoyment. The two remaining direct paths, H2 (β = −0.091, p > 0.05) and H4 (β = −0.065, p > 0.05), were not significantly identified. However, these findings were important because the insignificant direct effect of ALA on boredom suggested that anxiety fully mediated the association between ALA and boredom, and the insignificant direct effect of anxiety on enjoyment indicated that boredom functioned as a full mediator between anxiety and enjoyment (MacKinnon 2008).
Results of direct path analysis in SEM (N = 307).
Paths | Parametric estimates | 95 % CI Bias-corrected | HT | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | SE | Z | i | STD est. | Lower | Upper | ||
ALA → anxiety | −0.288 | 0.061 | −4.714 | 0.000 | −0.306 | −0.419 | −0.187 | H1*** |
ALA → boredom | −0.091 | 0.058 | −1.563 | 0.118 | −0.085 | −0.222 | 0.052 | H2ns |
ALA → enjoyment | 0.135 | 0.051 | 2.659 | 0.008 | 0.128 | 0.034 | 0.222 | H3** |
Anxiety → enjoyment | 0.065 | 0.080 | 0.809 | 0.418 | 0.058 | −0.090 | 0.212 | H4ns |
Anxiety → boredom | 0.693 | 0.092 | 7.498 | 0.000 | 0.609 | 0.487 | 0.720 | H5*** |
Boredom → enjoyment | −0.710 | 0.081 | −8.754 | 0.000 | −0.722 | −0.865 | −0.603 | H6*** |
-
B = unstandardized regression coefficient; SE = standard error; Z = Z-value; CI = confidence interval; p = p-value (2-tailed); HT = hypothesis testing; ***p-value < 0.001; **p-value < 0.01; ns = not statistically significant; ALA = actual language achievement.
Indirect effects. Regarding the mediating paths in the SEM, the present study found three significant indirect effects (Table 4). Anxiety mediated the relationship between ALA and boredom (β = −0.200, p < 0.001; 95 % CI = −0.304 to 0.112), and boredom mediated the path from anxiety to enjoyment (β = −0.492, p < 0.001; 95 % CI = −0.686 to −0.355). One of the most intriguing findings in the current study was that double mediators of L2 anxiety and boredom mediated the association between ALA and L2 enjoyment (β = 0.142, p < 0.001; 95 % CI = 0.080 to 0.240). Therefore, a distal mediating effect (ALA → Anxiety → Boredom → Enjoyment) was established in the current study. However, both the paths, ALA → Boredom → Enjoyment and ALA → Anxiety → Enjoyment, were not found to be statistically significant.
Results of mediating paths in the SEM (N = 307).
Indirect effect | Parametric estimates | 95 % CI Bias-corrected | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
B | SE | Z | p | LLCI | ULCI | |
ALA → anxiety → boredom | −0.200 | 0.048 | 4.167 | 0.000 | −0.304 | −0.112 |
Anxiety → boredom → enjoyment | −0.492 | 0.082 | −6.000 | 0.000 | −0.686 | −0.355 |
ALA → anxiety → boredom → enjoyment | 0.142 | 0.040 | 3.550 | 0.000 | 0.080 | 0.240 |
ALA → boredom → enjoyment | 0.065 | 0.053 | 1.226 | 0.207 | −0.039 | 0.172 |
ALA → anxiety → enjoyment | −0.019 | 0.026 | −0.731 | 0.394 | −0.078 | 0.026 |
To determine whether the statistically significant findings are meaningful in practice, we computed the effect size/ES (f 2) of the endogenous variables with the multiple squared correlations/SMC (R 2) as suggested by Cohen (1988, 1992. According to Cohen (1992, p. 157), the small, medium, and large effect sizes (f 2) are 0.02, 0.15, and 0.35, respectively. The results indicated that ALA had a small effect size on anxiety (R 2 = 0.09, f 2 = 0.10). However, this variable, together with other mediators as shown in Figure 2, had large effect sizes on boredom (R 2 = 0.41, f 2 = 0.69) and enjoyment (R 2 = 0.53, f 2 = 1.13).
5 Discussion
Employing the SEM, the current study tentatively examined the predictive effect of L2 Chinese learners’ actual language achievement on a constellation of emotions in the New Zealand context. The pattern from L2 Chinese achievement to diverse emotions indicated that language achievement, directly and indirectly, predicted enjoyment, boredom, and anxiety. That is, in addition to the path from emotion to language achievement, emotions and academic performance mutually affect each other in a reverse circle, empirically echoing the reciprocal loop of CVT (Pekrun 2006).
As to the direct path from language achievement to emotions, two paths between ALA and emotions, ALA → Anxiety and ALA → Enjoyment, were significantly identified in the mechanism, corroborating with the discovery of Alamer and Lee (2021) and Dewaele and Li (2022) in Chinese and Saudi Arabian English learning context. In other words, L2 learners who achieved greater actual language accomplishments might reversely decrease their perception of anxiety and boost their learning enjoyment. This might be because, based on the CVT in the academic setting (Pekrun 2006), learning achievement could act as a pivotal component of the learning environment to regulate learners’ control ability over a test or achievement-related activities and the value of the mission itself, thus further facilitate the perception of positive emotions such as confident, happiness, and enjoyment, and offset negative emotion, for example, anxiety in the L2 learning process (Dewaele and Li 2022). However, the predictive effect of ALA on boredom was not identified in the mechanism, which might be attributed to the diverse conceptual framework of emotions. In other words, the unobtrusive deactivating feature of boredom in language learning is hard to show within a short period compared with anxiety or anger (Coşkun and Yüksel 2022; Pekrun 2006). In addition, the cram school of the present study does not belong to the formal education system in New Zealand. The parents or students voluntarily enroll in this language school to reinforce Chinese proficiency with a strong instrumental drive. Thus, the relationship between Chinese language achievement and boredom is not directly significant due to the potential influence of motivation.
The indirect path, however, ALA → Anxiety → Boredom was significant, suggesting that ALA negatively impacted boredom through the influence of anxiety. This indicated that those with better language achievement might perceive less anxiety and then reduce the perception of dullness in the class. However, it should be noted that neither boredom nor anxiety was identified as the sole significant mediator between ALA and enjoyment, as the original relevant effect was diverted by the intermediary path. It did not mean that the relationship between emotions and ALA was separated. Instead, the distal mediating nexus, for example, ALA → Anxiety → Boredom → Enjoyment, encompassing ALA and all emotions, was found to be statistically significant. The finding validated the contributing effect of language achievement on achievement emotions, as previous studies showed that self-perceived/actual English achievement was the instigator of enjoyment or anxiety among Chinese, international, and Saudi Arabian sample (Alamer and Lee 2021; Dewaele and MacIntyre 2014; Dewaele and Li 2022; Li et al. 2020; Li 2022). The current study endorsed the reverse reciprocal link from learning achievement to the proximal and distal emotions but also stretched the nature of the CVT in L2 Chinese learning (Pekrun and Perry 2014).
Speaking of the relationship between emotions, two direct paths from Anxiety → Boredom and Boredom → Enjoyment were found to be significant. That is, anxiety was a positive predictor of boredom, and boredom negatively predicted enjoyment in L2 Chinese learning. Previous studies support the finding of the current study. Consistently, some research documented the negative association between anxiety and boredom in English language learning (Tsang and Dewaele 2023; Li and Han 2022; Wang and Li 2022). The path Boredom → Enjoyment is also highly germane to the research conducted by Zhao et al. (2023), who has elaborated on the co-existence of boredom and enjoyment, the moderating effect of boredom between motivation and enjoyment, and the distal impact on self-perceived language achievement. Furthermore, the finding empirically validated the mutually exclusive yet co-existing relationships between positive and negative emotions (Botes et al. 2022a; Derakhshan et al. 2021a, 2021b; Zhao and Wang 2023a) and theoretically confirmed the counteracting effect between positive and negative emotions advocated by the broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson 2004) in the complex L2 Chinese learning mechanism.
Although anxiety was significantly and negatively correlated with enjoyment in the correlation analysis, it failed to function as a predictor of enjoyment in the SEM, which has also been the case in some prior studies (e.g., Dewaele et al. 2022a, 2022b; Li and Han 2022). The rigid statistical approach and the various contexts might account for it. However, the mediating path, Anxiety → Boredom → Enjoyment, was significantly observed, indicating that anxiety indirectly predicted enjoyment due to the mediating role of boredom, which further illustrated the complex emotional network in language learning (Dewaele and MacIntyre 2014, 2016; Kruk et al. 2022; Li 2021b). Theoretically, this study validated and stretched the reverse reciprocal link from learning achievement to the proximal and distal emotions (Pekrun and Perry 2014).
6 Conclusion, limitation, and implication
The current study is one of the very first attempts to investigate the predictive effect of learning achievement on a group of emotions based on the CVT in the L2 Chinese learning context. The results validated and extended the CVT by disclosing that language learning achievement directly and indirectly predicted enjoyment, boredom, and anxiety based on 307 New Zealand L2 Chinese learners. In addition, the complex relationship among different emotions has also been featured in the learning activity.
This study has some limitations, however, that might generalize the findings. Firstly, the CVT is a bidirectional model that focuses on the reciprocal link between achievement emotions and language achievement. Due to space constraints, the present study mainly examined the less-featured path from language achievement to emotion in a holistic manner. Thus, future studies would offer more insights if the model can be validated with bi-directional processes. Secondly, the CVT underscores the reciprocal relationship between achievement emotions and academic achievement in the dynamic learning process. Yet, this study failed to investigate the endurability of emotions on academic achievement. Researchers are suggestive of adopting the longitudinal study across different contexts with a thorough grasp of the change of this pattern. Of note, the CVT advocated the domain specificity of achievement emotions, which means that the pattern between emotions and language achievement might vary across languages, subjects, and contexts, necessitating further studies to validate this pattern in the future.
Our research provides some pedagogical implications that are practical and theoretical to facilitate L2 Chinese learning in New Zealand. To start with, L2 educators should realize the strong instrumental motivation of L2 Chinese students in the cram school. Given that students’ core demand is to improve their academic performance and Chinese language skills, enhancing their actual language achievement should be prioritized to embellish the language learning process. Higher language proficiency can positively influence the control and value appraisals of a series of tasks, tests, and activities, thus further diminishing negative emotions and increasing the experience of positive emotions. In light of this, L2 educators are suggested to prioritize teaching quality and achievements as the pivotal role of a sense of learning achievement. Secondly, it is necessary for L2 educators to recognize the complex nature of the language learning process due to the intercorrelated relationship between positive and negative emotions. As revealed in the present study, positive and negative emotions are twins with reverse natures, co-existing in the language learning process. L2 educators are suggestive of promoting positive language learning experiences to dissimilate the effect of negative emotions in the L2 learning process (Fredrickson 2004; Li and Wei 2023). For example, they can make good use of multimedia resources and organize intriguing classroom tasks to enhance language enjoyment and reduce boredom (Zhao et al. 2023). Last but not least, this study theoretically validated and extended the CVT in the Chinese language learning context by examining the direct and indirect path between learning achievement and emotions, necessitating more attention from researchers in the future.
References
Alamer, Abdullah & Jihyun Lee. 2021. Language achievement predicts anxiety and not the other way around: A cross-lagged panel analysis approach. Language Teaching Research 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1177/13621688211033694.Suche in Google Scholar
Alrabai, Fakieh. 2022. Modeling the relationship between classroom emotions, motivation, and learner willingness to communicate in EFL: Applying a holistic approach of positive psychology in SLA research. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2022.2053138.Suche in Google Scholar
Alrabai, Fakieh & Abdullah Alamer. 2022. The role of learner character strengths and classroom emotions in L2 resilience. Frontiers in Psychology 13. 1–14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.956216.Suche in Google Scholar
Botes, Elouise, Jean-Marc Dewaele & Samuel Greiff. 2022a. By the old gods and the new: The effect of the congruence and incongruence of foreign language classroom anxiety and enjoyment on self-perceived proficiency. The Modern Language Journal 8. 1–37. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12807.Suche in Google Scholar
Botes, Elouise, Jean-Marc Dewaele & Samuel Greiff. 2022b. Taking stock: A meta-analysis of the effects of foreign language enjoyment. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching 12(2). 205–232. https://doi.org/10.14746/ssllt.2022.12.2.3.Suche in Google Scholar
Cohen, Jacob. 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edn. New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Suche in Google Scholar
Cohen, Jacob. 1992. A power primer. Psychological Bulletin 112(1). 155–159. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155.Suche in Google Scholar
Coşkun, A. & Y. Yüksel. 2022. Examining English as a foreign language students’ boredom in terms of different variables. Journal of English Language Pedagogy, Literature and Culture 7. 19–36. https://doi.org/10.35974/acuity.v7i2.2539.Suche in Google Scholar
Creswell, John W. & Cheryl N. Poth. 2018. Qualitative inquiry and research design: Choosing among five approaches, 4th edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Suche in Google Scholar
Derakhshan, Ali. 2022. Revisiting research on positive psychology in second and foreign language education: Trends and directions. Language Related Research 13(5). 1–43. https://doi.org/10.52547/LRR.13.5.1.Suche in Google Scholar
Derakhshan, Ali, Fathi Jalil, Miroslaw Pawlak & Kruk Mariusz. 2022. Classroom social climate, growth language mindset, and student engagement: The mediating role of boredom in learning English as a foreign language. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 6(3). 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2022.2099407.Suche in Google Scholar
Derakhshan, Ali, Mariusz Kruk, Mostafa Mehdizadeh & Miroslaw Pawlak. 2021a. Activity-induced boredom in online EFL classes. ELT Journal 76(1). 58–68. https://doi.org/10.1093/elt/ccab072.Suche in Google Scholar
Derakhshan, Ali, Mariusz Kruk, Mostafa Mehdizadeh & Miroslaw Pawlak. 2021b. Boredom in online classes in the Iranian EFL context: Sources and solutions. System 101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2021.102556.Suche in Google Scholar
Dewaele, Jean-Marc & Alfaf Albakistani & Iman Kamal Ahmed. 2022a. Levels of foreign language enjoyment, anxiety and boredom in emergency remote teaching and in in-person classes. Language Learning Journal 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1080/09571736.2022.2110607.Suche in Google Scholar
Dewaele, Jean-Marc, Elouise Botes & Samuel Greiff. 2022b. Sources and effects of foreign language enjoyment, anxiety, and boredom: A structural equation modeling approach. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263122000328.Suche in Google Scholar
Dewaele, Jean-Marc & Chengchen Li. 2020. Emotions in second language acquisition: A critical review and research agenda. Foreign Language World 196(1). 34–49.Suche in Google Scholar
Dewaele, Jean-Marc & Chengchen Li. 2022. Foreign language enjoyment and anxiety: Associations with general and domain-specific English achievement. Chinese Journal of Applied Linguistics 45(1). 32–48. https://doi.org/10.1515/CJAL-2022-0104.Suche in Google Scholar
Dewaele, Jean-Marc & Peter D. MacIntyre. 2014. The two faces of Janus? Anxiety and enjoyment in the foreign language classroom. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching 4(2). 237–274. https://doi.org/10.14746/ssllt.2014.4.2.5.Suche in Google Scholar
Dewaele, Jean-Marc & Peter D. MacIntyre. 2016. Foreign language enjoyment and foreign language classroom anxiety: The right and left feet of the language learner. In Peter D. MacIntyre, Tammy Gregersen & Sarah Mercer (eds.). Positive psychology in SLA, 215–236. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.10.21832/9781783095360-010Suche in Google Scholar
Dewaele, Jean-Marc & Peter D. MacIntyre. 2024. “You can’t start a fire without a spark”. Enjoyment, anxiety, and the emergence of flow in foreign language classrooms. Applied Linguistics Review 15. 403–426. https://doi.org/10.1515/applirev-2021-0123.Suche in Google Scholar
Embretson, Susan E. & Steven P. Reise. 2000. Item response theory for psychologists. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Suche in Google Scholar
Fayers, Peter. 2004. Item response theory for psychologists. Quality of Life Research 13(3). 715–716. https://doi.org/10.1023/b:qure.0000021503.45367.f2.10.1023/B:QURE.0000021503.45367.f2Suche in Google Scholar
Fredrickson, Barbara L. 2004. The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Science 359. 1367–1377. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2004.1512.Suche in Google Scholar
George, Darren & Paul Mallery. 2020. IBM SPSS statistics 26 step by step: A simple guide and reference. New York: Routledge.10.4324/9780429056765Suche in Google Scholar
Hair, Joseph F.Jr., William C. Black, Barry J. Babin & Rolph E. Anderson. 2019. Multivariate data analysis, 8th edn. Hampshire: Cengage Learning EMEA.Suche in Google Scholar
Horwitz, Elaine K., Michael B. Horwitz & Joann Cope. 1986. Foreign language classroom anxiety. The Modern Language Journal 70(2). 125–132. https://doi.org/10.2307/327317.Suche in Google Scholar
Jiang, Yan & Jean-Marc Dewaele. 2019. How unique is the foreign language classroom enjoyment and anxiety of Chinese EFL learners? System 82. 13–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2019.02.017.Suche in Google Scholar
Kenny, David A. 1979. Correlation and causation. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.Suche in Google Scholar
Kline, Rex B. 2011. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling, 3rd edn. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Suche in Google Scholar
Kruk, Mariusz, Marisa Pawlak, Majid Elahi Shirvan, Tahereh Taherian & Elham Yazdanmehr. 2022. A longitudinal study of foreign language enjoyment and boredom: A latent growth curve modelling. Language Teaching Research 1–32. https://doi.org/10.1177/13621688221082303.Suche in Google Scholar
Li, Chengchen. 2021a. A control–value theory approach to boredom in English classes among university students in China. The Modern Language Journal 105(1). 317–334. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12693.Suche in Google Scholar
Li, Chengchen. 2021b. Looking back and looking forward: SLA research from a positive psychology perspective over the past decade (2012–2021). Foreign Language Education 42(4). 57–63.Suche in Google Scholar
Li, Chengchen. 2022. Foreign language learning boredom and enjoyment: The effects of learner variables and teacher variables. Language Teaching Research 10. 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1177/13621688221090324.Suche in Google Scholar
Li, Chengchen, Jean-Marc Dewaele & Guiying Jiang. 2019. The complex relationship between classroom emotions and EFL achievement in China. Applied Linguistics Review 11(3). 485–510. https://doi.org/10.1515/applirev-2018-0043.Suche in Google Scholar
Li, Chengchen, Jean-Marc Dewaele & Guiying Jiang. 2020. The complex relationship between classroom emotions and EFL achievement in China. Applied Linguistics Review 11(3). 485–510. https://doi.org/10.1515/applirev-2018-0043.Suche in Google Scholar
Li, Chengchen, Jean-Marc Dewaele & Yanhong Hu. 2021. Foreign language learning boredom: Conceptualization and measurement. Applied Linguistics Review 14(2). 223–249. https://doi.org/10.1515/applirev-2020-0124.Suche in Google Scholar
Li, Chengchen & Ye Han. 2022. The predictive effects of foreign language anxiety, enjoyment, and boredom on learning outcomes in online English classrooms. Modern Foreign Languages 45(2). 207–219.Suche in Google Scholar
Li, Chengchen, Guiying Jiang & Jean-Marc Dewaele. 2018. Understanding Chinese high school students’ foreign language enjoyment: Validation of the Chinese version of the foreign language enjoyment scale. System 76. 183–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2018.06.004.Suche in Google Scholar
Li, Chengchen & Li Wei. 2023. Anxiety, enjoyment, and boredom in language learning amongst junior secondary students in rural China: How do they contribute to L2 achievement? Studies in Second Language Acquisition 45(1). 93–108. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263122000031.Suche in Google Scholar
MacIntyre, Peter D. & Sarah Mercer. 2014. Introducing positive psychology to SLA. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching 4(2). 153–172. https://doi.org/10.14746/ssllt.2014.4.2.2.Suche in Google Scholar
MacKinnon, David P. 2008. Introduction to statistical mediation analysis. New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Suche in Google Scholar
Oxford, Rebecca L. 2016. Powerfully positive: Searching for a model of language learner well-being. In Danuta Gabryś-Barker & Dagmara Gałajda (eds.). Positive psychology perspectives on foreign language learning and teaching, 21–37. Switzerland: Springer.10.1007/978-3-319-32954-3_2Suche in Google Scholar
Pekrun, Reinhard. 2006. The control-value theory of achievement emotions: Assumptions, corollaries, and implications for educational research and practice. Educational Psychology Review 18(4). 315–341. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-006-9029-9.Suche in Google Scholar
Pekrun, Reinhard & Raymond P. Perry. 2014. Control-value theory of achievement emotions. In Reinhard Pekrun & Lisa Linnenbrink-Garcia (eds.). International handbook of emotions in education, 130–151. New York: Routledge.10.4324/9780203148211Suche in Google Scholar
Resnik, Pia & Jean-Marc Dewaele. 2023. Learner emotions, autonomy and trait emotional intelligence in ‘in-person’ versus emergency remote English foreign language teaching in Europe. Applied Linguistics Review 14(3). 473–501. https://doi.org/10.1515/applirev-2020-0096.10.1515/applirev-2020-0096Suche in Google Scholar
Shao, Kaiqi, Kristina Stockinger, Herbert W. Marsh & Reinhard Pekrun. 2023. Applying control-value theory for examining multiple emotions in L2 classrooms: Validating the achievement emotions questionnaire–second language learning. Language Teaching Research 1–48. https://doi.org/10.1177/13621688221144497.Suche in Google Scholar
Tsang, Art & Jean-Marc Dewaele. 2024. The relationships between young FL learners’ classroom emotions (anxiety, boredom, & enjoyment), engagement, and FL proficiency. Applied linguistics review 15. 2015–2034. https://doi.org/10.1515/applirev-2022-0077.Suche in Google Scholar
Wang, Danping & Alice Chik. 2022. Chinese-language education in Australia and New Zealand: Key issues and challenges. In Wing On Lee, Philip Brown, Lin Goodwin & Andy Green (eds.), International handbook on education development in Asia-Pacific, 1–17. Singapore: Springer.10.1007/978-981-16-2327-1_33-1Suche in Google Scholar
Wang, Xinpeng & Yi Li. 2022. The predictive effects of foreign language enjoyment, anxiety, and boredom on general and domain-specific English achievement in online English classrooms. Frontiers in Psychology 13. 1–12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1050226.Suche in Google Scholar
Wang, Yongliang, Derakhshan Ali & Lawrence Jun Zhang. 2021. Researching and practicing positive psychology in second/foreign language learning and teaching: The past, current status and future directions. Frontiers in Psychology 12. 1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.731721.Suche in Google Scholar
Zare, Javad, Khadijeh Aqajani Delavar & Derakhshan Ali. 2023. The impact of altruism on the emotions and English summary writing skills of second language learners: An intervention study in light of positive psychology. Language Teaching Research 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1177/13621688231151632.Suche in Google Scholar
Zhao, Xian & Danping Wang. 2023a. Grit, emotions, and their effects on ethnic minority students’ English language learning achievements: A structural equation modelling analysis. System 113. 1–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2023.102979.Suche in Google Scholar
Zhao, Xian & Danping Wang. 2023b. The role of enjoyment and boredom in shaping English language achievement among ethnic minority learners. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1080/01434632.2023.2194872.Suche in Google Scholar
Zhao, Xian, Guoxing Lan & Tianxu Chen. 2023. Motivational intensity and self-perceived Chinese language proficiency: A moderated mediation model of L2 enjoyment and boredom. Language Teaching Research 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1177/13621688231180465.Suche in Google Scholar
© 2023 the author(s), published by De Gruyter, Berlin/Boston
This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Artikel in diesem Heft
- Frontmatter
- Research Articles
- Interactional features in second language classroom discourse: variations across novice and experienced language teachers
- English-medium instruction and impact on academic performance: a randomized control study
- ESL classroom interactions in a translanguaging space
- Motivation profiles of Chinese rural foreign language learners: link with learning strategy and achievement
- Translingual practice as a representation of heritage languages and regional identities in multilingual society
- Pedagogical implications of translingual practices for content and language integrated learning
- Understanding micro-blogging users’ translanguaging in Chinese language play: a qualitative phenomenological approach
- Do teachers’ well-being and resilience predict their Foreign Language Teaching Enjoyment (FLTE)?
- Investigating in-class and after-class boredom among advanced learners of English: intensity, interrelationships and learner profiles
- Africatown in Guangzhou as geosemiotic assemblage: connecting multilingualism, store signs, and chronotopes
- “I’m not angry!”: language ideologies, misunderstanding, and marginalization among North Korean refugees in rural South Korea
- Developing a taxonomy of teacher emotion labor through metaphor: personal, interpersonal, and sociocultural angles
- When women’s empowerment meets health communication: a critical discourse analysis of the WeChat official account “Health China”
- “I never make a permanent decision based on a temporary emotion”: unveiling EFL teachers’ perspectives about emotions in assessment
- How ‘good-enough’ is second language comprehension? Morphological causative and suffixal passive constructions in Korean
- The predictive effect of language achievement on multiple emotions in languages other than English: validating a distal mediation model based on the control-value theory
- Narratives of the self in bilingual speakers: the neurophenomenal space
- Uncovering English as a foreign language teacher resilience: a structural equation modeling approach
- Documenting students’ conceptual understanding of second language vocabulary knowledge: a translanguaging analysis of classroom interactions in a primary English as a second language classroom for linguistically and culturally diverse students
- Investigating translanguaging strategies and online self-presentation through internet slang on Douyin (Chinese TikTok)
- Collaboratively pursuing student uptake of feedback through storytelling: a conversation analytic study of interaction in team doctoral supervision
- Languages ontologies in higher education: the world-making practices of language teachers
- English loanwords in Hong Kong Cantonese: false friend cognates and English vocabulary acquisition
- Artificial intelligence and posthumanist translation: ChatGPT versus the translator
Artikel in diesem Heft
- Frontmatter
- Research Articles
- Interactional features in second language classroom discourse: variations across novice and experienced language teachers
- English-medium instruction and impact on academic performance: a randomized control study
- ESL classroom interactions in a translanguaging space
- Motivation profiles of Chinese rural foreign language learners: link with learning strategy and achievement
- Translingual practice as a representation of heritage languages and regional identities in multilingual society
- Pedagogical implications of translingual practices for content and language integrated learning
- Understanding micro-blogging users’ translanguaging in Chinese language play: a qualitative phenomenological approach
- Do teachers’ well-being and resilience predict their Foreign Language Teaching Enjoyment (FLTE)?
- Investigating in-class and after-class boredom among advanced learners of English: intensity, interrelationships and learner profiles
- Africatown in Guangzhou as geosemiotic assemblage: connecting multilingualism, store signs, and chronotopes
- “I’m not angry!”: language ideologies, misunderstanding, and marginalization among North Korean refugees in rural South Korea
- Developing a taxonomy of teacher emotion labor through metaphor: personal, interpersonal, and sociocultural angles
- When women’s empowerment meets health communication: a critical discourse analysis of the WeChat official account “Health China”
- “I never make a permanent decision based on a temporary emotion”: unveiling EFL teachers’ perspectives about emotions in assessment
- How ‘good-enough’ is second language comprehension? Morphological causative and suffixal passive constructions in Korean
- The predictive effect of language achievement on multiple emotions in languages other than English: validating a distal mediation model based on the control-value theory
- Narratives of the self in bilingual speakers: the neurophenomenal space
- Uncovering English as a foreign language teacher resilience: a structural equation modeling approach
- Documenting students’ conceptual understanding of second language vocabulary knowledge: a translanguaging analysis of classroom interactions in a primary English as a second language classroom for linguistically and culturally diverse students
- Investigating translanguaging strategies and online self-presentation through internet slang on Douyin (Chinese TikTok)
- Collaboratively pursuing student uptake of feedback through storytelling: a conversation analytic study of interaction in team doctoral supervision
- Languages ontologies in higher education: the world-making practices of language teachers
- English loanwords in Hong Kong Cantonese: false friend cognates and English vocabulary acquisition
- Artificial intelligence and posthumanist translation: ChatGPT versus the translator