Chapter 2. On the strength of morphological paradigms
-
Walter Bisang
Abstract
This paper will present a historical explanation of radical pro-drop based on the strength of morphological paradigms. It will start out from the observation that East and mainland Southeast Asian languages (EMSEA) with their reduced or absent morphology are radical pro-drop, while West African Niger-Congo languages with similarly reduced or absent morphology have obligatory subject and object arguments. The reason for this is that the ancestor languages of Niger-Congo had a morphological paradigm expressing the features of [person] and [number] on the verb, while the ancestor languages of EMSEA had not. It will be argued that the existence of morphological paradigms keeps the frequency of [person] and [number] features above the critical percentage of 20–30% for linguistic change (s-curve model, Wang & Cheng 1970) and thus blocks the change from non-pro-drop to radical pro-drop in the West African languages concerned by passing on the frequency of the former morphological features to syntax and obligatory pronouns. In EMSEA languages, there is no morphological paradigm that pushes the frequency of these features up to the percentage that would be necessary for a change to non-pro-drop.
Abstract
This paper will present a historical explanation of radical pro-drop based on the strength of morphological paradigms. It will start out from the observation that East and mainland Southeast Asian languages (EMSEA) with their reduced or absent morphology are radical pro-drop, while West African Niger-Congo languages with similarly reduced or absent morphology have obligatory subject and object arguments. The reason for this is that the ancestor languages of Niger-Congo had a morphological paradigm expressing the features of [person] and [number] on the verb, while the ancestor languages of EMSEA had not. It will be argued that the existence of morphological paradigms keeps the frequency of [person] and [number] features above the critical percentage of 20–30% for linguistic change (s-curve model, Wang & Cheng 1970) and thus blocks the change from non-pro-drop to radical pro-drop in the West African languages concerned by passing on the frequency of the former morphological features to syntax and obligatory pronouns. In EMSEA languages, there is no morphological paradigm that pushes the frequency of these features up to the percentage that would be necessary for a change to non-pro-drop.
Chapters in this book
- Prelim pages i
- Table of contents v
- List of tables ix
- List of figures xv
- List of contributors xvii
- Acknowledgements xix
- Chapter 1. When paradigms change 1
-
Part I. Paradigm change
- Chapter 2. On the strength of morphological paradigms 23
- Chapter 3. Derivational paradigms in diachrony and comparison 61
- Chapter 4. On arguing from diachrony for paradigms 89
- Chapter 5. Reconstructing the Niger-Congo Verb Extension Paradigm 103
-
Part II. The continuation of paradigms
- Chapter 6. Perceived formal and functional equivalence 129
- Chapter 7. Comparative consequences of the tongue root harmony analysis for proto-Tungusic, proto-Mongolic, and proto-Korean 141
- Chapter 8. Old Japanese bigrade paradigms and Korean passives and causatives 177
- Chapter 9. The Japanese inflectional paradigm in a Transeurasian perspective 197
-
Part III. The innovation of paradigms
- Chapter 10. A Yakut copy of a Tungusic viewpoint aspect paradigm 235
- Chapter 11. Amdo Altaic directives and comparatives based on the verb ‘to see’ 243
- Chapter 12. Innovations and archaisms in Siberian Turkic spatial case paradigms 257
- Chapter 13. Paradigm copying in Tungusic 287
- Chapter 14. Ural-Altaic 311
- Language index 337
- Subject index 343
Chapters in this book
- Prelim pages i
- Table of contents v
- List of tables ix
- List of figures xv
- List of contributors xvii
- Acknowledgements xix
- Chapter 1. When paradigms change 1
-
Part I. Paradigm change
- Chapter 2. On the strength of morphological paradigms 23
- Chapter 3. Derivational paradigms in diachrony and comparison 61
- Chapter 4. On arguing from diachrony for paradigms 89
- Chapter 5. Reconstructing the Niger-Congo Verb Extension Paradigm 103
-
Part II. The continuation of paradigms
- Chapter 6. Perceived formal and functional equivalence 129
- Chapter 7. Comparative consequences of the tongue root harmony analysis for proto-Tungusic, proto-Mongolic, and proto-Korean 141
- Chapter 8. Old Japanese bigrade paradigms and Korean passives and causatives 177
- Chapter 9. The Japanese inflectional paradigm in a Transeurasian perspective 197
-
Part III. The innovation of paradigms
- Chapter 10. A Yakut copy of a Tungusic viewpoint aspect paradigm 235
- Chapter 11. Amdo Altaic directives and comparatives based on the verb ‘to see’ 243
- Chapter 12. Innovations and archaisms in Siberian Turkic spatial case paradigms 257
- Chapter 13. Paradigm copying in Tungusic 287
- Chapter 14. Ural-Altaic 311
- Language index 337
- Subject index 343