Abstract
A crucial question for historical linguistics has been why some sound changes happen but not others. Recent work on the foundations of sound change has argued that subtle distributional facts about segments in a language, such as functional load, play a role in facilitating or impeding change. Thus not only are sound changes not all equally plausible, but their likelihood depends in part on phonotactics and aspects of functional load, such as the density of minimal pairs. Tests of predictability on the chance of phoneme merger suggest that phonemes with low functional load (as defined by minimal pair density) are more likely to merge, but this has been investigated only for a small number of languages with very large corpora and well attested mergers. Here we present work suggesting that the same methods can be applied to much smaller corpora, by presenting the results of a preliminary exploration of nine Australian languages, with a particular focus on Bardi, a Nyulnyulan language from Australia’s northwest. Our results support the hypothesis that the probability of merger is higher when phonemes distinguish few minimal pairs.
Acknowledgement
Many thanks to Andrew Wedel for his very helpful feedback on an earlier version of this paper. This work is funded by NSF, Funder Id: 10.13039/100000001, grant BCS-1423711.
References
Aklif, G. 1999. Ardiyooloon Bardi ngaanka: One Arm Point Bardi dictionary. Halls Creek, Western Australia: Kimberley Language Resource Centre.Suche in Google Scholar
Blevins, J. & A. Garrett. 1998. The origins of consonant-vowel metathesis. Language 74(3). 508–556.10.1353/lan.1998.0012Suche in Google Scholar
Bowern, C. 2004. Bardi verb morphology in historical perspective. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University dissertation.Suche in Google Scholar
Bowern, C. 2012. A grammar of Bardi. Berlin: Mouton.10.1515/9783110278187Suche in Google Scholar
Bowern, C. 2016. Chirila: Contemporary and historical resources for the indigenous languages of Australia. Language Documentation and Conservation 10(1). 1–45.Suche in Google Scholar
Bradley, J. n.d. Yanyuwa dictionary. ASEDA 0382 electronic file.Suche in Google Scholar
Coate, H. & A. Elkin. 1974. Ngarinyin-English dictionary. 2 vols. NSW, Australia: University of Sydney.Suche in Google Scholar
Dixon, R. 1980. The languages of Australia. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Suche in Google Scholar
Evans, N., F. Merlan, M. Tukumba & M. Arts. 2004. A first dictionary of Dalabon (Ngalkbon). Maningrida, NT: Maningrida Arts & Culture Centre, NT.Suche in Google Scholar
Garrett, A. & K. Johnson. 2013. Phonetic bias in sound change. In A. Yu (ed.), Origins of sound change: approaches to phonologization, 51–97. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199573745.003.0003Suche in Google Scholar
Gasser, E. & C. Bowern. 2014. Revisiting phonological generalizations in Australian languages. Proceedings of the 2013 Annual Meeting on Phonology. 1–10. https://journals.linguisticsociety.org/proceedings/index.php/amphonology/article/view/17 (1 July, 2018).10.3765/amp.v1i1.17Suche in Google Scholar
Hansen, K. & L. Hansen. 1992. Pintupi/Luritja dictionary. Alice Springs: IAD Press.Suche in Google Scholar
Meakins, F., P. McConvell, E. Charola, N. McNair, H. McNair & L. Campbell. 2013. Gurindji multimedia dictionary. http://ausil.org/Dictionary/Gurindji/ (4 February, 2017).Suche in Google Scholar
Ohala, J. 1992. What’s cognitive, what’s not, in sound change. In G. Kellermann & M. Morrissey (eds.), Diachrony within synchrony: language history and cognition, 309–355. Frankfurt: Peter Lang Verlag.Suche in Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. 2001. Exemplar dynamics: Word frequency, lenition and contrast. In J. L. Bybee & P. Hopper (eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure, 137–158. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/tsl.45.08pieSuche in Google Scholar
R Development Core Team. 2015. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Tech. rep. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. www.R-project.org.Suche in Google Scholar
Schwarz, S. 1996. Warlpiri-English dictionary. Electronic file. September 15, 1996.Suche in Google Scholar
Surendran, D. & P. Niyogi. 2003. Measuring the functional load of phonological contrasts. arXiv preprint cs/0311036.https://newtraell.cs.uchicago.edu/research/publications/techreports/TR-2003-12 (27 June, 2018).Suche in Google Scholar
Surendran, D. & P. Niyogi. 2006. Quantifying the functional load of phonemic oppositions, distrinctive features, and suprasegmentals. In O. Thomsen (ed.), Competing models of linguistic change: evolution and beyond, 43–58. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/cilt.279.05surSuche in Google Scholar
Valiquette, H. 1993. A basic Kukatja to English Dictionary. WA: Luurnpa Catholic School.Suche in Google Scholar
Wedel, A., S. Jackson & A. Kaplan. 2013a. Functional load and the lexicon: Evidence that syntactic category and frequency relationships in minimal lemma pairs predict the loss of phoneme contrasts in language change. Language and Speech 56(3). 395–417. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/0023830913489096 (4 February, 2017).10.1177/0023830913489096Suche in Google Scholar
Wedel, A., A. Kaplan & S. Jackson. 2013b. High functional load inhibits phonological contrast loss: A corpus study. en. Cognition 128(2). 179–186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.002. http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0010027713000541 (4 February, 2017).10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.002Suche in Google Scholar
©2018 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston
Artikel in diesem Heft
- Predictability and phonology: past, present and future
- Predictability and perception for native and non-native listeners
- Mergers in Bardi: contextual probability and predictors of sound change
- Predictability of stop consonant phonetics across talkers: Between-category and within-category dependencies among cues for place and voice
- Assessing predictability effects in connected read speech
- The interdependence of frequency, predictability, and informativity in the segmental domain
- Loci and locality of informational effects on phonetic implementation
- Three steps forward for predictability. Consideration of methodological robustness, indexical and prosodic factors, and replication in the laboratory
- Distributional learning is error-driven: the role of surprise in the acquisition of phonetic categories
- Truncation in message-oriented phonology: a case study using Korean vocative truncation
- Durational contrast in gemination and informativity
- Practice makes perfect: the consequences of lexical proficiency for articulation
- Patterns of probabilistic segment deletion/reduction in English and Japanese
- The role of predictability in shaping phonological patterns
Artikel in diesem Heft
- Predictability and phonology: past, present and future
- Predictability and perception for native and non-native listeners
- Mergers in Bardi: contextual probability and predictors of sound change
- Predictability of stop consonant phonetics across talkers: Between-category and within-category dependencies among cues for place and voice
- Assessing predictability effects in connected read speech
- The interdependence of frequency, predictability, and informativity in the segmental domain
- Loci and locality of informational effects on phonetic implementation
- Three steps forward for predictability. Consideration of methodological robustness, indexical and prosodic factors, and replication in the laboratory
- Distributional learning is error-driven: the role of surprise in the acquisition of phonetic categories
- Truncation in message-oriented phonology: a case study using Korean vocative truncation
- Durational contrast in gemination and informativity
- Practice makes perfect: the consequences of lexical proficiency for articulation
- Patterns of probabilistic segment deletion/reduction in English and Japanese
- The role of predictability in shaping phonological patterns