Abstract
The order of acquisition of consonants by children is impacted by both language-specific and species-wide factors. The latter factors also help to motivate the crosslinguistic commonness of some consonants. Here we explore the extent of overlap between crosslinguistic commonness and order of acquisition. We test how well the typological commonness of consonants is predictive of their order of acquisition in English, and vice versa, helping to shed light on the extent to which general cognitive and physical factors are explanatory vis-à-vis the acquisition of specific consonant types. We utilize several different sources to demonstrate that typological frequency and order of acquisition are indeed highly mutually predictive. We rely on acquisition data from English. We demonstrate that the crosslinguistic usage of a consonant is a better predictor of its order of acquisition than the language-specific factors tested.
-
Author contributions: CE: Wrote paper and conducted statistical analyses. CE and SS: Collected data.
References
Bernhardt, Barbara H. & Joseph P. Stemberger. 1998. Handbook of phonological development from the perspective of constraint-based nonlinear phonology. San Diego: Academic Press.10.1163/9789004653405_008Suche in Google Scholar
Blasi, Damián E., Steven Moran, Scott R. Moisik, Paul Widmer, Dan Dediu & Balthasar Bickel. 2019. Human sound systems are shaped by post-Neolithic changes in bite configuration. Science 363(6432). 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aav3218.Suche in Google Scholar
de Boysson-Bardies, Bénédicte & Marilyn May Vihman. 1991. Adaptation to language: Evidence from babbling and first words in four languages. Language 67(2). 297–319. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.1991.0045.Suche in Google Scholar
Cohen-Priva, Uriel. 2017. Informativity and the actuation of lenition. Language 93. 569–597. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2017.0037.Suche in Google Scholar
Cychosz, Margaret. 2017. Functional load and frequency predict consonant emergence across five languages. UC Berkeley Phonetics and Phonology Lab Annual Report, 312–320.10.5070/P7131040758Suche in Google Scholar
Dediu, Dan, Rick Janssen & Scott R. Moisik. 2017. Language is not isolated from its wider environment: Vocal tract influences on the evolution of speech and language. Language & Communication 54. 9–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langcom.2016.10.002.Suche in Google Scholar
Edwards, Jan, Mary E. Beckman & Benjamin Munson. 2015. Frequency effects in phonological acquisition. Journal of Child Language 42. 306–311. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000914000634.Suche in Google Scholar
Everett, Caleb. 2018a. The similar rates of occurrence of consonants across the world’s languages: A quantitative analysis of phonetically transcribed word lists. Language Sciences 69. 125–135. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2018.07.003.Suche in Google Scholar
Everett, Caleb. 2018b. The global dispreference for posterior voiced obstruents: A quantitative analysis of word-list data. Language 94. e311–e323. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2018.0069.Suche in Google Scholar
Everett, Caleb & Sihan Chen. 2021. Speech adapts to differences in dentition within and across populations. Scientific Reports 11. 1066. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-80190-8.Suche in Google Scholar
Gordon, Matthew. 2016. Phonological typology. United Kingdom: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199669004.001.0001Suche in Google Scholar
Hayden, Rebecca. 1950. The relative frequency of phonemes in General-American English. Word 6(3). 217–223. https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1950.11659381.Suche in Google Scholar
Jakobson, Roman. 1941. Kindersprache, Aphasie und Allgemeine Lautgesetze [Child language, aphasia and phonological universals]. Translated by Allan R. Keiler in 1968. The Hague: Mouton Publishers.Suche in Google Scholar
Kager, Rene, Suzanne van der Feest, Paula Fikkert, Annemarie Kerkhoff & Tania Zamuner. 2007. Representations of [voice]: Evidence from acquisition. In Jeroen, van de Weijer & Erik Jan van der Torre (eds.), Voicing in Dutch: (De)voicing- phonology, phonetics and psycholinguistics, 41–80. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/cilt.286.03kagSuche in Google Scholar
King, Adam & Andrew Wedel. 2020. Greater early disambiguating information for less-probable words: The lexicon is shaped by incremental processing. Open Mind: Discoveries in Cognitive Science 4. 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00030.Suche in Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. 2000. A new view of language acquisition. In Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, vol. 97, 11857–11850.10.1073/pnas.97.22.11850Suche in Google Scholar
Ladefoged, Peter & Ian Maddieson. 1996. The sounds of the world’s languages. Oxford: Blackwell.Suche in Google Scholar
Lindblom, Björn. 1990. Explaining phonetic variation: A sketch of the H&H theory. In William J. Hardcastle and Alain Marchal (eds.), Speech Production and Speech Modelling, 403–439. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.10.1007/978-94-009-2037-8_16Suche in Google Scholar
Lindblom, Björn & Johan Sundberg. 1971. Acoustical consequences of lip, tongue, jaw, and larynx movement. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 50(4). 1166–1179. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1912750.Suche in Google Scholar
Locke, John. 1983. Phonological acquisition and change. New York: Academic Press.Suche in Google Scholar
Ludusan, Bogdan, Reiko Mazuka & Emmanuel Dupoux. 2021. Does infant-directed speech help phonetic learning? A machine learning investigation. Cognitive Science 45. 1–31. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12946.Suche in Google Scholar
Maddieson, Ian. 1984. Patterns of sounds. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511753459Suche in Google Scholar
Maddieson, Ian. 2013. Voicing in plosives and fricatives. In Matthew S. Dryer & Martin Haspelmath (eds.), The world atlas of language structures online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. http://wals.info/chapter/4.Suche in Google Scholar
Mines, M. Ardussi, Barbara Hanson & June Shopu. 1978. Frequency of occurrence of phonemes in Conversational English. Language and Speech 21(3). 205–220. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383097802100302.Suche in Google Scholar
McLeod, Sharynne & Kathryn Crowe. 2018. Children’s consonant acquisition in 27 languages: A crosslinguistic review. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 27(4). 1546–1571. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_ajslp-17-0100.Suche in Google Scholar
Moran, Steven & Daniel McCloy (eds.). 2019. PHOIBLE 2.0. Jena: Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History. Available at: http://phoible.org.Suche in Google Scholar
Moran, Steven & Sabine Stoll. 2017. Worldwide frequency of phonemes predicts their age of acquisition. Poster presented at the 42nd Boston University Conference on Language Development (BUCLD).Suche in Google Scholar
Mugitani, Ryoko & Sadao Hiroya. 2012. Development of vocal tract and acoustic features in children. Acoustical Science and Technology 33. 215–220. https://doi.org/10.1250/ast.33.215.Suche in Google Scholar
Napoli, Donna Jo, Nathan Sanders & Rebecca Wright. 2014. On the linguistic effects of articulatory ease, with a focus on sign languages. Language 90(2). 424–456. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2014.0043.Suche in Google Scholar
Ohala, John. 1997. Comparison of speech sounds: Distance vs. cost metrics. In Shigeru Kiritani, Hajime Hirose & Hiroya Fujisaki (eds.), Speech production and language, 261–270. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110809411.261Suche in Google Scholar
Paradis, Carole & Jean-François Prunet (eds.). 1991. The special status of coronals: Internal and external evidence. Phonetics and phonology, vol. 2. San Diego: Academic Press.Suche in Google Scholar
Romani, Cristina, Claudia Galuzzi, Cecelia Guariglia & Jeremy Goslin. 2017. Comparing phoneme frequency, age of acquisition, and loss in aphasia: Implications for phonological universals. Cognitive Neuropsychology 34(7–8). 449–471. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2017.1369942.Suche in Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, Carol. 1985. Phonetic inventories, 15–24 months: A longitudinal study. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research 28. 505–512. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.2804.505.Suche in Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, Carol. 2011. Relationships between lexical and phonological development in young children. Journal of Child Language 38(1). 1–34. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000910000425.Suche in Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, Carol & Judith A. Cooper. 1984. Patterns of early lexical and phonological development. Journal of Child Language 11. 247–271. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900005766.Suche in Google Scholar
Stokes, Stephanie & Dinoj Surendran. 2005. Articulatory complexity, ambient frequency, and functional load as predictors of consonant development in children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 48(3). 577–591. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/040).Suche in Google Scholar
Wedel, Andrew, Scott Jackson & Abby Kaplan. 2013. Functional load and the lexicon: Evidence that syntactic category and frequency relationships in minimal lemma pairs predict the loss of phoneme contrasts in language change. Language and Speech 56. 395–417. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830913489096.Suche in Google Scholar
Wichmann, Søren, Eric W. Holman & Cecil H. Brown (eds.). 2020. The ASJP Database (version 19). Available at: https://asjp.clld.org/.Suche in Google Scholar
© 2023 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston
Artikel in diesem Heft
- Frontmatter
- Articles
- Current research in phonological typology
- Investigating the ‘what’, ‘where’ and ‘why’ of global phonological typology
- Canonical phonology and criterial conflicts: relating and resolving four dilemmas of phonological typology
- Refining explanation in Evolutionary Phonology: macro-typologies and targeted typologies in action
- The prosodic foot beyond prosodic prominence: a preliminary survey
- On the comparability of prosodic categories: why ‘stress’ is difficult
- Bootstrap co-occurrence networks of consonants and the Basic Consonant Inventory
- Frequent violation of the sonority sequencing principle in hundreds of languages: how often and by which sequences?
- Diachronic phonological typology: understanding inventory structure through sound change dynamics
- Place typology and evolution of implosives in Indo-Aryan languages
- Estimating areal effects in typology: a case study of African phoneme inventories
- Word prosody of African versus European-origin words in Afro-European creoles
- Towards a phonological typology of the Kalahari Basin Area languages
- The typological frequency of consonants is highly predictive of their order of acquisition in English
Artikel in diesem Heft
- Frontmatter
- Articles
- Current research in phonological typology
- Investigating the ‘what’, ‘where’ and ‘why’ of global phonological typology
- Canonical phonology and criterial conflicts: relating and resolving four dilemmas of phonological typology
- Refining explanation in Evolutionary Phonology: macro-typologies and targeted typologies in action
- The prosodic foot beyond prosodic prominence: a preliminary survey
- On the comparability of prosodic categories: why ‘stress’ is difficult
- Bootstrap co-occurrence networks of consonants and the Basic Consonant Inventory
- Frequent violation of the sonority sequencing principle in hundreds of languages: how often and by which sequences?
- Diachronic phonological typology: understanding inventory structure through sound change dynamics
- Place typology and evolution of implosives in Indo-Aryan languages
- Estimating areal effects in typology: a case study of African phoneme inventories
- Word prosody of African versus European-origin words in Afro-European creoles
- Towards a phonological typology of the Kalahari Basin Area languages
- The typological frequency of consonants is highly predictive of their order of acquisition in English