Home Changing Attitudes? Investigating the Link between Couples’ Pandemic Behavior, Cognitive Dissonance, and Gender Role Attitudes in Germany
Article Open Access

Changing Attitudes? Investigating the Link between Couples’ Pandemic Behavior, Cognitive Dissonance, and Gender Role Attitudes in Germany

  • Katrin Firl

    Katrin Firl, geb. 1996 in Köln. Studium der Sozialwissenschaften, Soziologie und Demografie in Köln und Groningen. Seit 2023 Promotion an der Universität Mannheim. Seit 2022 wissenschaftliche Mitarbeiterin bei GESIS – Leibniz-Institut für Sozialwissenschaften. Seit 2024 im Team Family Surveys zur Erhebung des FReDA-Paneldatensatzes tätig. Forschungsschwerpunkte: Familiensoziologie; Geschlechtergerechtigkeit.

    Wichtigste Publikation: (2024). FReDA – The German Family Demography Panel Study. GESIS, Cologne. ZA7777 Data File Version 4.0.0. http://dx.doi.org/10.4232/1.14195 (mit M. Bujard, T. Gummer, K. Hank, F. J. Neyer, R. Pollak, N. Schneider, C. K. Spieß, C. Wolf, I. Bauer, S. Börlin, D. Bretschi, K. Brüggemann, P. Christmann, R. Edinger, F. Eigenbrodt, L. C. Frembs, K. Groß, S. Hoherz, T. Kunz, D. Lück, R. Naderi, E. Naumann, T. Nutz, A.-S. Oehrlein, K. Ruckdeschel, L. Schmid, A. Schumann, N. Schumann, A. Stein, C. Thönnissen, E. Ullrich).

    EMAIL logo
Published/Copyright: August 22, 2024
Become an author with De Gruyter Brill

Abstract

Research found that not only macro-level processes like cohort replacements foster gender role attitude (GRA) change, but also life events and changes in lived realities. Framing the COVID-19 pandemic as a ‘natural experiment’ allows to reduce endogeneity when examining the association between emerging cognitive dissonance and GRA change. Estimating asymmetrical fixed effects regressions, I investigate the influence of changes in the division of family labor on GRAs for respondents with no pre-pandemic attitude-behavior discrepancy, using pairfam waves 11 and 13. Indeed, changes in behavior for respondents who had aligned behaviors and attitudes before the pandemic can be associated with GRA adaptation: A change toward an egalitarian division of family labor during the pandemic can be associated with more egalitarian GRAs. Likewise, a change toward an inegalitarian division can be associated with less egalitarian GRAs. These findings support the usefulness of cognitive dissonance as one explanatory mechanism for intra-individual attitudinal change.

Zusammenfassung

Nicht nur gesamtgesellschaftliche Prozesse wie der Generationenwechsel kann Wandel in Geschlechterrollen-Einstellungen (GRE) fördern, sondern auch Veränderungen gelebter Realitäten. Die COVID-19-Pandemie als ‚natürliches Experiment‘ zu betrachten ermöglicht, Endogenität zu reduzieren und den Zusammenhang zwischen aufkommender kognitiver Dissonanz und GRE-Wandel zu untersuchen. Mithilfe asymmetrischer Fixed-Effects-Regressionen analysiere ich in den pairfam Wellen 11 und 13 den Einfluss von Veränderungen in der Aufteilung von Familienarbeit auf GRE für Befragte ohne prä-pandemische Einstellungs-Verhaltens-Diskrepanz. Tatsächlich können Verhaltensänderungen bei diesen Befragten mit GRE-Anpassungen einhergehen: Eine Veränderung zu einer egalitären Aufteilung von Familienarbeit während der Pandemie kann mit egalitäreren GRE verbunden werden. Ebenso kann eine Veränderung zu einer nicht-egalitären Aufteilung mit weniger egalitären GRE einhergehen. Diese Ergebnisse zeigen, dass kognitive Dissonanz ein erklärender Mechanismus für intraindividuelle Einstellungsänderungen ist.

1 Introduction

How gender role attitudes (in the following: ‘GRA’s) evolve in society and individually change over the life course has been subject to multiple studies (Baxter et al. 2015; Grinza et al. 2022; Knight & Brinton 2017; Preston 2023; Schober & Scott 2012; Zoch & Schober 2018). Research found that next to macro-level developments like cohort replacement, micro-level experiences like life events and lived realities can influence the development of individuals’ GRAs. Hence, in contrast to the widespread assumption that GRAs are stable over the life course, empirical evidence shows that intra-individual attitude change does occur (e. g. Baxter et al. 2015).

An example of this is the transition into parenthood: Scholars found that parents often hold less egalitarian GRAs after their first childbirth while also practicing a less egalitarian division of family labor (i. e., housework and childcare) than before the first childbirth (Baxter et al. 2015; Grinza et al. 2022). Also, policies that influence lived realities and hence, behavior, are found to affect GRAs: For example, both the expansion of childcare provision (Zoch & Schober 2018) as well as paternal leave provisions (Tavits et al. 2023) can be connected to more egalitarian GRAs among parents.

A prominent explanation for the change in GRAs regarding such life events and lived realities is that cognitive dissonance may arise due to a mismatch of behavior and attitudes. Following cognitive dissonance theory (Festinger 1957), such a mismatch might trigger attitudinal change to reduce cognitive dissonance. For example, Schober and Scott (2012) found that attitudinal change for first-time parents is significantly more likely for parents whose post-natal practices are in conflict with their pre-natal attitudes.

However, research on life events like the transition to parenthood is likely biased by endogeneity, as persons usually self-select into parenthood. In contrast, the global COVID-19 pandemic can be understood as a ‘natural experiment’ that hit people unexpectedly and altered lived realities. Exploiting the pandemic as a ‘natural experiment’ allows to reduce endogeneity when examining the relationship between behavior and attitudes (Hudde et al. 2022).

In Germany, the COVID-19 pandemic starting in mid-March 2020 has led to periodically active national containment measures like social distancing requirements, curfews, closures of businesses like restaurants and shops, and closures of formal childcare facilities like nurseries, kindergartens, and schools. The last pandemic-related measurements were resolved in March 2023 (Bundesministerium für Gesundheit 2023).

Because of the constraints on outsourcing family labor especially parents had to take over persisting and additional family labor responsibilities besides their gainful employment, creating a double burden (e. g. Kohlrausch & Zucco 2020) and the necessity to re-negotiate and adapt the division of family labor responsibilities. The research examining how (heterosexual) couples distributed the load of family labor responsibilities during the pandemic, like (additional) housework, childcare, and homeschooling is ambiguous. Many found that women, especially mothers, took over the lion’s share of the extra family labor while reducing hours in paid employment (e. g., Kohlrausch & Zucco 2020). Other researchers found that men, especially fathers, stepped in more (in relative terms) as a reaction to the crisis (e.g., Kreyenfeld & Zinn 2021), possibly leading to a more gender egalitarian division of family labor in the longer term.

How far the pandemic has influenced GRAs has only been subject to few studies (Reichelt et al. 2021; Vandecasteele et al. 2022). Following the concept of cognitive dissonance and similar literature on attitudinal change, my research question is whether and how the COVID-19 pandemic has influenced GRAs by reorganizing paid and unpaid working arrangements and hence, influencing the discrepancy between attitudes and behavior. I focus on the emergence of cognitive dissonance during the pandemic as the cognitive dissonance theory assumes attitudinal change to be a response to cognitive dissonance. To examine the effect of emerging cognitive dissonance, I focus only on respondents who reported no discrepancy between gender attitudes and behavior prior to the pandemic. Using an asymmetrical fixed effects approach, I can investigate whether and how changes in behavior can be associated with changes in attitudes for respondents who experienced no cognitive dissonance prior to the pandemic. Examining this research question within the pandemic context allows adding to the greater context of how changing behavior and attitudes may be interrelated while reducing endogeneity.

I analyze representative panel data from the German Family Panel ‘pairfam’ and investigate the change in GRAs from before to during the COVID-19 pandemic. Understanding how life events and lived realities can influence behavioral and attitudinal change is crucial to for example analyzing the effect of social policy interventions that aim to foster gender equality (Grinza et al. 2022). Until today, not only in Germany, women are disadvantaged amongst others when it comes to performing the lion’s share of unpaid labor, earning potentials, and being represented full-time in the labor market (Grinza et al. 2022). Due to the interrelation between attitudes and behavior, GRAs can play an important role in shaping patterns of gender inequality over the life course and within society (e. g., Nitsche & Grunow 2016).

2 Background

2.1 Cognitive dissonance and gender role attitude change

In Germany and most other European countries, gender roles have become more egalitarian in the late 20th and early 21st centuries (Boring & Moroni 2022). Two major drivers of change were expanding education and women’s entry into the labor market in the 1960s (Nitsche & Grunow 2016). The pandemic-related constraints on outsourcing housework and childcare, the additional family labor responsibilities, and the disruption in the labor market could reorganize the division of family labor, and “might cause gender relations to shift at a societal scale” (Reichelt et al. 2021: 229).

Previous research has shown that life events or exogenous shocks can affect the division of family labor and might consequently influence GRAs to match the behavior (Danzer et al. 2021; Schober & Scott 2012). An example for this is the transition into parenthood: Previously more egalitarian couples distribute labor more traditionally after the first childbirth (Kühhirt 2012) and also shift their attitudes toward a less egalitarian gender ideology (Baxter et al. 2015; Grinza et al. 2022).

Also, lived realities that were affected by policies reforms can influence GRAs: For instance, in Estonia, mothers and fathers who were affected by a policy reform that extended fathers’ parental leave threefold stated more gender egalitarian views due to the more gender equal sharing of childcare resulting from the paternity leave (Tavits et al. 2023). Similarly, in West Germany, Zoch and Schober (2018) found changes towards more egalitarian GRAs among mothers related to the childcare expansion. The latter studies give support to the notion that lived realities can alter GRAs also in the short-term (Zoch & Schober 2018).

While empirical evidence shows that individuals do change their attitudes over the life course, the theoretical and psychological mechanisms remain unclear. One theory explaining a shift in attitudes following a change in behavior is the cognitive dissonance theory (Festinger 1957). Thereby, a discrepancy between behavior and attitudes can produce a state of discomfort which individuals are motivated to resolve. Often, attitudes are more easily adaptable than behavior (Schober & Scott 2012). This is especially pronounced if the behavior is more resistant to change due to external constraints and outer circumstances (Harmon-Jones & Harmon-Jones 2007), like produced by the COVID-19 pandemic and its containment measures. Concluding, cognitive dissonance theory states that personal experiences can promote attitudinal change, mainly when dissonant cognitions based on one’s attitudes and behavior arise.

I follow Schober and Scott (2012), who connect cognitive dissonance to GRAs by referring to literature on gender identity (West & Zimmermann 1987) that suggests “that male and female roles as partners or parents serve as core standards on which to evaluate gendered aspects of paid work and childcare arrangements” (Schober & Scott 2012: 516). Schober and Scott link GRAs to gender identity and assume that discrepancies between gendered standards and actual behavior likely lead to changes in GRAs to match behavior. However, like Schober and Scott I acknowledge that cognitive dissonance most likely is not the only motivator and theoretical mechanism behind intra-individual attitudinal change.

While most studies on GRA change only examine differences between GRAs before and after a specific life event or policy reform, only few studies actually consider the effect of attitude and behavior discrepancy on GRA change. For instance, Schober and Scott (2012) and Grinza et al. (2022) find that attitudinal changes after the transition to parenthood are more likely among men and women for whom postnatal behavior clashes with prenatal attitudes, supporting the expectations of cognitive dissonance theory.

However, research about life events, for example the transition to parenthood, is likely affected by endogeneity, because people usually self-select into becoming parents. In contrast, the COVID-19 pandemic can be seen as an exogenous macro-level shock that hit people unexpectedly (Vandecasteele et al. 2022). Exploiting the COVID-19 pandemic as an exogeneous shock and framing it as a ‘natural experiment’ hence allows to reduce endogeneity issues that affect other research on GRA change (Hudde et al. 2022).

Despite this advantage, it should be kept in mind that the pandemic was an extraordinary situation, and pandemic behavior does not necessarily translate into post-pandemic patterns (Boll et al. 2021). It might be that individuals only reacted to this extreme situation in a pragmatic way, without consequently adjusting their attitudes (like assumed by Hiekel & Kühn 2023) because they do not expect the situation to last. However, some policy interventions like paternity leaves also just affect a smaller period of time and can be related to GRA change (Tavits et al. 2023). Furthermore, it can be argued that the pandemic most likely will have long-term effects on gender relations (Danzer et al. 2021) not only because pandemic measures lasted for three years but also because pandemic practices like home office arrangements and flexible working hours are prolonged beyond the pandemic and because gender relations were prominently discussed throughout the pandemic.

A few studies have examined changes in GRAs during the pandemic; however, none of them focuses specifically on changes in the division of family labor as a predictor for GRA change. Both Reichelt et al. (2021) and Vandecasteele et al. (2022) use a form of the cognitive dissonance theory to explain the relationship between labor market transitions and GRAs.

Reichelt et al. (2021) showed that for the US, Germany, and Singapore, changes in the employment constellation of heterosexual couples affected the GRAs of men and women when changes in employment clashed with gendered norms. Thereby, “men express more egalitarian gender-role attitudes if they became unemployed but their partners remained employed, while women express more traditional attitudes if they became unemployed and their partners remained employed” (Reichelt et al. 2021: 228). These findings reinforce the theoretical assumption of pandemic-related behavior influencing attitudes to match lived realities. However, using a cross-sectional design, Reichelt et al. (2021) are not able to examine actual changes in GRAs.

For the Netherlands, Vandecasteele et al. (2022) examined the effect of the COVID-19 pandemic on GRAs regarding maternal employment. Vandecasteele et al. found that individuals experiencing a change in paid workload that clashes with traditional gender norms expressed more gender egalitarian attitudes concerning maternal employment than before the pandemic. For women living with children in couple households, Vandecasteele et al. found a halt in their progression toward more egalitarian attitudes.

Building on the literature on GRA change both during and before the pandemic by following the cognitive dissonance theory and only considering respondents who reported no attitude-behavior discrepancy (i. e., no cognitive dissonance) before the pandemic, I test the following hypotheses:

If women take over more family labor responsibilities during the pandemic, and thus, the division of labor becomes more inegalitarian than before the pandemic, cognitive dissonance emerges and GRAs become less egalitarian to match the changed behavior (H1). Likewise, if men spend more time on family labor during the pandemic and hence, the division of labor becomes more egalitarian than before the pandemic, cognitive dissonance emerges and GRAs become more egalitarian to match the changed behavior (H2). Because of gendered norms and their greater behavioral implications for women (Grinza et al. 2022; Zoch & Schober 2018), women’s attitudes are more strongly expected to change (H3).

2.2 The division of family labor during the COVID-19 pandemic

Because the hypotheses based on the cognitive dissonance theory build on a change in behavior (i. e., the division of family labor) due to pandemic constraints, in the following I summarize the numerous research findings on heterosexual couples’ pandemic behavior. To gain as much insight as possible on gendered behavior and attitudes, when analyzing family labor, housework and childcare responsibilities need to be distinguished. Compared to childcare, housework is often viewed as more feminine, less enjoyable, postponable, and more flexibly performable (Sullivan 2013).

As a consequence of the pandemic, housework responsibilities increased because external services like restaurants or cleaners were no longer available. Additionally, people spent more time at home due to social distancing and curfews, increasing household labor (Reichelt et al. 2021). Childcare responsibilities especially increased due to the closures of childcare facilities and schools and the obligation to quarantine in case of a COVID-19-contact or -infection. Additionally, informal childcare arrangements became inaccessible in times of strict social distancing restrictions when social contacts were limited to members of the same household.

Consequently, housework, childcare, and additional responsibilities like homeschooling were primarily performed by parents (Kohlrausch & Zucco 2020). Due to the constraints on outsourcing family labor and the additional tasks resulting from the pandemic situation, most likely an increase in the amount but also possibly a change in the division of family labor took place.

The findings regarding the division of family labor during the pandemic are ambiguous: While some found support for a ‘re-traditionalization’ hypothesis (Allmendinger 2020) with women stepping in even more regarding both housework and childcare (e. g., Illing et al. 2022; Kohlrausch & Zucco 2020; Naujoks et al. 2022; Zoch et al. 2021; Zucco & Lott 2021), others found a relative increase in men’s contribution, especially with regard to childcare and hence, more gender equal family labor arrangements (e. g., Bujard et al. 2020; Globisch et al. 2022; Jessen et al. 2021; Kreyenfeld & Zinn 2021; Zinn et al. 2020). Fathers may have had more time availability due to short-time work, reduced hours, home office, or unemployment and were pushed to increase their time spend on childcare, particularly if a reduction of hours in paid employment for fathers was accompanied by mothers working more than 20 hours per week on-site (Globisch et al. 2022). That some studies found that men increased their relative share more than women could also be a ceiling effect: Women often already performed the greater or total share, which is why they could not increase it much further.

Using pairfam data, Hank and Steinbach (2020) found an almost equal split between those couples in which the female share of housework and childcare increased and those in which it decreased. They concluded that there is not one observable trend of how couples reacted to the COVID-19 crisis regarding the division of labor but rather heterogeneous responses (Hank & Steinbach 2020). The heterogeneity in results is also shown by other researchers who found no change in the division of family labor during the pandemic but stability of pre-pandemic arrangements (e. g., Boll et al. 2021; Möhring et al. 2020; Zucco & Lott 2021). All studies agree that both before and during the pandemic women perform the greater absolute share of unpaid family labor.

Who took over the (additional) tasks in heterosexual couples during the pandemic depended on multiple factors. An important aspect was the employment situation of couples before and during the pandemic, as the COVID-19 pandemic led to many employment transitions like unemployment, reduced or increased hours, short-time work, and home office arrangements (Möhring et al. 2020; Kohlrausch & Zucco 2021).

For the UK, Hudde et al. (2021) found no evidence for pre-pandemic GRAs influencing the distribution of housework during the pandemic like it would be expected by gender ideology theory (Davis & Greenstein 2009). In the discussion about which causal order the relationship between attitudes and behavior has, the null findings from Hudde et al. supports the notion of examining the effect of behavior on attitudes. However, the feedback effects between attitudes and behavior should always be kept in mind when researching on them (e. g., Preston 2023).

3 Data and Methods

3.1 Data

The following analyses are based on release 13.0 of the German Family Panel ‘pairfam’ (Brüderl et al. 2022; Huinink et al. 2011). Pairfam is a representative panel study launched in 2008. It has a multi-cohort design, which means that the initially more than 12,000 anchors (i. e., primary respondents) were randomly drawn from four nationally representative birth cohorts: 1971–1973, 1981–1983, 1991–1993, and 2001–2003. A more detailed description of the data can be found in Huinink et al. (2011).

In the analyses, the last pre-pandemic wave (11) and the first full pandemic wave (13) were used. Wave 12 was not included in the analyses as GRA items were only reported every other year. Wave 11 was conducted from mid-October 2018 to the end of May 2019 and contains 9,435 interviews. Wave 13 was conducted from October 2020 to April 2021 and contains 7,009 interviews.

From November till December 16th, a so-called ‘lockdown light’ was active, followed by a more restrictive lockdown from mid-December till May 2021. The more restrictive lockdown was accompanied by higher infection-numbers and, starting in mid-April, included a curfew from 9:00 pm till 5:00 am. Although schools and childcare facilities were technically open, they were also affected by quarantine regulations and there were time periods in which only emergency caretaking was offered (between December 16th and January 10th) (Bundesregierung, 2020). Hence, the pandemic situation was quite tense during the data collection period, with a less and a more restrictive lockdown influencing peoples’ lives.

Wave 11, like all pre-pandemic waves was conducted using only computer-assisted personal interview (CAPI) methods. Because of the pandemic, wave 13 was conducted using a mixed-mode strategy. Respondents and interviewers could choose between computer-assisted telephone interviews (CATI) and CAPI methods. Roughly two thirds of the respondents in wave 13 were interviewed using CATI methods.

3.2 Variables

The dependent variable in the following analyses is the anchor’s GRA. Pairfam contains four GRA items:

  1. “Women should be more concerned about their family than about their career.”

  2. “Men should participate in housework to the same extent as women.”

  3. “A child under 6 will suffer from having a working mother.”

  4. “Children often suffer because their fathers spend too much time at work.”

For all items, respondents could indicate on a 5-point-scale whether they 1 “completely disagree” or 5 “completely agree”. For comparability, I poled all variables in the same direction so that higher values represent less egalitarian attitudes.

I calculated Cronbach’s alpha to test whether creating an additive index is justifiable, however, it was too low to do so for all or various combinations of the items (alpha ~ 0.5). This is in line with previous findings regarding gender ideology: That GRAs towards housework do not necessarily reflect attitudes about childcare is explained by the multidimensionality of gender ideology (Grunow et al. 2018; Knight & Brinton 2017).

Hence, additionally to analyzing the division of housework and childcare separately, I analyze GRA items separately. Following Nitsche and Grunow (2016) I use item 2) to operationalize GRAs concerning the division of housework. To operationalize GRAs towards childcare, following Zoch and Schober (2018) I use item 1) and item 3), as they both concern attitudes about maternal employment. Because the alpha-value is too low to combine both items, I assume that they each cover a distinct area of maternal employment attitudes and analyze both items separately.

Following Nitsche and Grunow (2018), I neglected item 4) in all analyses, because egalitarian and inegalitarian persons could likewise agree with the statement.

The main independent variable to test the hypotheses is the division of family labor, specifically the division of housework (i. e., doing laundry, cooking, and cleaning) and childcare. For both kinds of family labor, respondents were asked to which extent, on a 5-point scale, they or their partner perform those tasks. Possible answers were: “(almost) completely my partner”, “for the most part my partner”, “split about 50/50”, “for the most part me”, and “(almost) completely me”. Respondents who claimed another person was responsible for those tasks or for whom none of the answers were applicable were removed from the sample.

The independent variables including information on how couples were sharing family labor, are structured as follows: Anchors were assumed to have an ‘inegalitarian’ arrangement if a female respondent claimed to do most or all family labor or if a male respondent claimed his partner was doing most or all work. Because the share of couples in which the man did most or all family labor is very small, they were combined with respondents who claimed to share labor about 50/50 and labeled as having an ‘egalitarian’ arrangement.

My assumptions about the relationship between changes in behavior and changes in attitudes are based on the cognitive dissonance theory. I assume that a pandemic-related emergence of cognitive dissonance can be associated with a change in GRAs. Hence, to test my hypotheses it is relevant to only examine respondents who had no attitude-behavior discrepancy before the pandemic and thus, are considered to not have experienced pre-pandemic cognitive dissonance.

To ease the operationalization of pre-pandemic cognitive dissonance, I created three dummy variables based on the GRA items, indicating ‘egalitarian’ or ‘inegalitarian’ GRAs. Given the skewness of the GRA items in pairfam towards egalitarianism and following Hiekel and Kühn (2023), I categorized respondents answering 1 or 2 as ‘egalitarian’ and those answering 3 or more as ‘inegalitarian’.

Separated by all three GRA items and both, the division of housework and of childcare, respondents who in wave 11 were egalitarian in their GRAs and their family labor division or were inegalitarian in their GRAs and their family labor division were categorized as having no pre-pandemic attitude-behavior discrepancy and hence, ‘no cognitive dissonance’. Those who had an egalitarian GRA but distributed family labor in an inegalitarian way or had an inegalitarian GRA but distributed family labor in an egalitarian way were categorized as having ‘cognitive dissonance’.

3.3 Sample selection

In the analyses, I use balanced panel samples of respondents who participated in waves 11 and 13. Because the gendered division of family labor is the main independent variable, I only kept individuals living in a cohabitating, heterosexual relationship (married and unmarried) who were 18 years or older in wave 11.

I analyze three sub-samples of individuals experiencing ‘no cognitive dissonance’ from two separate superordinated samples, one in which all individuals in cohabitating relationships are included (‘cohabitating sample’), and one in which only parents living with at least one child (‘parents sample’) are included. The influence of changes in the division of housework is analyzed in both samples, because of the assumed differences between parents and non-parents and the influence of changes in the division of childcare only for parents.

Respondents who did not answer the items concerning their GRAs in wave 11 or wave 13 were removed. Those who did not report their division of family labor in wave 11 or wave 13 were removed. Eventually, the superordinated ‘cohabitating sample’ consists of 3,169 respondents, and the superordinated ‘parents sample’ consists of 1,933 respondents. The number of cases dropped on the single items is shown in Appendix Table A2.

Because I am interested in a pandemic induced emergence of cognitive dissonance, I only analyze those respondents from the superordinated samples who were categorized as having ‘no cognitive dissonance’ before the pandemic in three sub-samples: For GRA item 2) and the division of housework: n=1,566, for GRA item 1) and the division of childcare: n = 1,096, and for GRA item 3) and the division of childcare: n = 994.

In Appendix Table A5, the distribution of cognitive dissonance for the superordinated samples by GRA item can be seen. Roughly 50 % of the respondents in all three sub-samples report no attitude-behavior discrepancy before the pandemic. If attitude-behavior discrepancies are reported, for housework, most respondents report a more egalitarian attitude than behavior, while for childcare and the two maternal employment GRAs, the picture is more divers even though more respondents report egalitarian GRAs and inegalitarian behavior. Especially women report more discrepancy between attitudes and behavior, and thereby more often report a more egalitarian attitude than behavior. Hence, Table A5 shows that attitude-behavior discrepancies also exist within other framings than the pandemic and therefore, that cognitive dissonance is not the only cause of attitude formation (Schober & Scott 2012). It also emphasizes that I only analyze a selective group of those who had aligned attitudes and behavior prior to the pandemic, which should be kept in mind when interpreting the results.

Table 1:

Means of time-variant and time-invariant covariates in the three ‘no cognitive dissonance’ sub-samples. pairfam, own calculations

Superordinated sample:

Cohabitating sample (IV: Division of housework)

Parents sample (IV: Division of childcare)

No attitude-behavior discrepancy between behavior and GRA item:

GRA item 2)

(Men should participate in housework to the same extent as women)

GRA item 1) (Women should be more concerned about their family than their career)

GRA item 3)

(A child under 6 will suffer from having a working mother)

Variable

Obs.

Mean

Obs.

Mean

Obs.

Mean

Male

1,566

0.46

1,096

0.42

994

0.46

West Germany

1,566

0.73

1,096

0.72

994

0.70

1971–73

1,566

0.29

1,096

0.34

994

0.36

1981–83

1,566

0.43

1,096

0.57

994

0.55

1991–93

1,566

0.27

1,096

0.09

994

0.09

Age w11

1,566

36.35

1,096

38.68

994

38.83

Low education w13

1,566

0.04

1,096

0.04

994

0.05

Middle education w13

1,566

0.47

1,096

0.49

994

0.51

High education w13

1,566

0.49

1,096

0.47

994

0.44

0 children w13

1,566

0.34

- 

- 

- 

- 

1 child w13

1,566

0.24

1,096

0.26

994

0.26

2 children w13

1,566

0.30

1,096

0.52

994

0.53

3+ children w13

1,566

0.12

1,096

0.22

994

0.21

Children: 0–3 yrs w13

1,028

0.30

1,095

0.23

993

0.22

3–6 yrs w13

1,028

0.20

1,095

0.24

993

0.21

6–12 yrs w13

1,028

0.26

1,095

0.31

993

0.33

12–18 yrs w13

1,028

0.18

1,095

0.20

993

0.21

18 + yrs w13

1,028

0.06

1,095

0.02

993

0.03

Income w11

1,463

3909.76

1,004

4161,82

910

4128.45

Income w13

1,478

4357.22

1,021

4679.21

923

4565.38

Home office w13

1,366

0.42

946

0.44

856

0.40

Job Hours w11

1,344

37.26

890

35.11

831

36.04

Job Hours w13

1,364

36.10

944

34.18

853

35.13

GRA w11

1,566

1.83

1,096

2.78

994

2.48

GRA w13

1,566

1.69

1,096

2.59

994

2.39

GRA change w13-w11

1,566

-0.14

1,096

-0.19

994

-0.09

Inegalitarian division w11

1,566

0.29

1,096

0.67

994

0.49

Inegalitarian division w13

1,566

0.43

1,096

0.58

994

0.48

IV = Independent variable

In Table 1, the summary statistics of the three sub-samples with ‘no cognitive dissonance’ are shown. For comparison, the summary statistics of the whole superordinated samples including weighted means and sex stratified analysis can be found in Appendix Tables A3 and A4.

The GRA item 2) sub-sample is on average younger than the other two sub-samples and has a slightly higher share of highly educated respondents. 34 % of the GRA item 2) sub-sample have no children living in the household. The average household income is lower in the GRA item 2) sub-sample while the average working hours per week are at least 1 hour higher than in both childcare sub-samples.

In comparison to the superordinated samples, the sub-samples with GRA item 2) and item 3) include 5 percentage points more men. In the GRA item 2) sub-sample, there are 7 percentage points more non-parents as compared to the whole cohabitating sample. In all three sub-samples the average GRAs are less egalitarian than the whole superordinated samples. Also, the average change in GRAs is greater in the three sub-samples, especially for GRA item 2) and 3).

Concerning item 2), respondents report on average the most egalitarian attitudes, which is due to almost universal approval to the item possibly resulting from the high social desirability connected to item 2) (Hudde & Engelhardt 2021). Correspondingly, the lowest shares of inegalitarian family labor divisions are reported in the GRA item 2) sub-sample. However, during the pandemic, the share of those reporting an inegalitarian division of family labor increased in the GRA item 2) sub-sample by 14 percentage point, while the share decreased in both other sub-samples considering childcare.

Table 2:

Summary statistics for women and men for waves 11 and 13 in the three ‘no cognitive dissonance’ sub-samples. pairfam, own calculations

Superordinated sample:

Cohabitating Sample (IV: Division of housework)

Parents Sample (IV: Division of childcare)

Sub-sample:

GRA item 2)

GRA item 1)

GRA item 3)

Variable

Women

Men

Women

Men

Women

Men

West Germany

0.74

0.72

0.73

0.71

0.72

0.68

1971–73

0.29

0.30

0.28

0.42

0.30

0.43

1981–83

0.41

0.46

0.61

0.52

0.60

0.50

1991–93

0.30

0.24

0.10

0.06

0.10

0.07

Age w11

36.06

36.69

37.95

39.71

38.06

39.76

Low education w13

0.05

0.03

0.05

0.04

0.06

0.04

Middle education w13

0.48

0.45

0.50

0.46

0.56

0.45

High education w13

0.47

0.52

0.45

0.50

0.38

0.51

Income w11

3826.48

4005.65

4056.69

4305.05

4008.19

4268.13

Income w13

4314.74

4405.60

4467.71

4964.14

4337.82

4830.87

Home office w13

0.41

0.43

0.43

0.45

0.36

0.44

Job Hours w11

32.83

41.74

27.27

43.17

28.78

42.69

Job Hours w13

31.90

40.37

27.38

41.97

28.79

41.28

GRA w11

1.86

1.80

2.87

2.65

2.38

2.60

GRA w13

1.71

1.68

2.66

2.49

2.28

2.52

GRA change w13-w11

-0.15

-0.12

-0.21

-0.16

-0.10

-0.08

Inegalitarian FL w11

0.31

0.29

0.71

0.62

0.48

0.51

Inegalitarian FL w13

0.46

0.39

0.63

0.52

0.50

0.45

FL = Family labor

Looking at the share of respondents who reported an inegalitarian division of family labor in wave 11 shows how the combination of aligned attitudes and behavior is distributed in the three sub-samples. While the GRA item 2) sub-sample only includes 29 % of respondents who report inegalitarian attitudes and behavior, for the other sub-samples and considering childcare, 67 % (GRA item 1)) and 49 % (GRA item 3)) report inegalitarian GRAs and behavior. Appendix Table A3 shows, that in the superordinated samples before the pandemic, the share of those reporting an inegalitarian division of family labor is 62 % in both samples.

In all three sub-samples, the average GRA got more egalitarian during the pandemic. The largest change can be found in the sub-sample with GRA item 1) (19 percentage points).

Table 2 shows the summary statistics separated by sex for all three ‘no cognitive dissonance’ sub-samples. In all samples, women are younger than men and have a lower share of high education. Women report a smaller household income than men in both waves. The biggest sex difference can be found when looking at working hours: Women, and even more pronounced, mothers, work substantially fewer hours than men both before and during the pandemic.

Looking at GRA item 2) and GRA item 3), men on average report less egalitarian attitudes than women. Women only have less egalitarian attitudes than men when looking at the GRA item 1). Both women and men report more egalitarian attitudes during the pandemic. For all three sub-samples, changes in GRAs are greater for women than for men.

3.4 Methods

I estimate asymmetrical fixed effects models (Allison 2019) to explore changes in GRAs of men and women between the last pre-pandemic wave (i. e., wave 11) and during the pandemic (i. e., wave 13). To estimate asymmetric effects, I use first difference methods (Allison 2019). With two waves, first difference models yield the same estimates as fixed effects models, eliminating all time-invariant heterogeneity by exploiting only within-person variability (i. e., intra-individual changes over time) (Allison 2019). Hence, it can be avoided that potentially unobserved time-invariant confounders bias the estimates. Therefore, in the models, it cannot be controlled for time-invariant variables like birth cohort or education. Because of the first difference approach, it is also not possible to control for age. In the models including control variables it is controlled for income, the number of children living in the household and weekly hours spent in paid employment.

Table 3:

Proportional changes in GRAs, proportional changes in the division of family labor and the average GRA change score by changes in the division of family labor between wave 11 and wave 13 for respondents with no pre-pandemic cognitive dissonance. pairfam, own calculations

Cohabitating sample (IV: Division of housework)

Parents sample (IV: Division of childcare)

GRA change

GRA item 2)

GRA item 1)

GRA item 3)

No change

63.9 %

52.0 %

49.3 %

More egalitarian

23.7 %

31.0 %

28.9 %

Less egalitarian

12.4 %

17.0 %

21.8 %

Family labor change

GRA item 2)

GRA item 1)

GRA item 3)

No change

79.1 %

76.1 %

74.5 %

More egalitarian

3.7 %

16.3 %

13.5 %

Less egalitarian

17.2 %

7.6 %

12.0 %

Family labor change

Change in item 2)

Change in item 1)

Change in item 3)

No change

-0.16

-0.19

-0.07

More egalitarian

-0.93

-0.44

-0.63

Less egalitarian

0.12

0.39

0.40

Estimating asymmetric effects means that I am able to distinguish between changes in the division of family labor towards a less egalitarian versus toward a more egalitarian division. To do so, I created two dummy variables: one indicating a change towards a more inegalitarian division and the other one indicating a change towards a more egalitarian division. For both variables, the reference category indicates no change in the division of family labor. For more on the asymmetrical fixed effects approach, see Allison (2019). The dependent variable is the change score of GRAs between wave 13 and wave 11. Hence, the variable ranges from -4 to 4, whereby negative values indicate a change towards more egalitarian GRAs, and positive values indicate a change towards less egalitarian GRAs. 0 indicates no change.

In addition to a model for the overall association between changes in the division of family labor and GRAs, I examined whether the associations differ for men and women by estimating equivalent models separated by sex to test hypothesis H3. Due to the model specification, no sex interaction term can be included in the model. However, performing the analyses in samples separated by sex also allows to examine different tendencies between men and women (e. g., Baxter et al. 2015; Grinza et al. 2022).

The coefficients in all estimated models can be interpreted as the average change in respondents’ GRAs between wave 11 and wave 13 for a one-unit intra-individual change across the two waves on the independent variables. In all models, I estimated panel robust standard errors on the individual level (Brüderl & Ludwig 2014).

Because in pairfam the items on GRAs are captured on a Likert-scale and skewed towards egalitarianism (Hiekel & Kühn 2023), floor effects could occur that might underestimate the effect of the independent variables. To control for floor and ceiling effects I re-estimated all models using Tobit regression (for more on floor and ceiling effects and Tobit regression, see McBee 2010). The Tobit regressions yield very similar results and are shown in the Appendix (Table A6-A8).

4 Results

4.1 Descriptive Results

In this paper, the association between the emergence of cognitive dissonance due to a pandemic-related change in behavior (i. e., division of family labor) and changes in GRAs is of interest. Table 3 shows changes in GRAs, changes in the division of family labor and the average GRA change score by changes in the division of family labor.

In the GRA item 2) sub-sample roughly 37 % changed their GRA while in both other sub-samples, roughly 50 % changed their GRA from wave 11 to wave 13. In all three sub-samples a greater proportion of those who changed their attitudes report more egalitarian attitudes during the pandemic.

Looking at the changes in the division of family labor from before to during the pandemic in all three sub-samples shows that substantially more respondents report no change in the division of family labor than those who report a change. However, in all three sub-samples, at least 20 % of the respondents report change. Especially the division of housework became less egalitarian, while in both sub-samples considering the division of childcare, a greater proportion of those who changed reported a more egalitarian division of childcare. This matches the finding from the literature that if men stepped in more during the pandemic, it were mostly fathers.

Figure 1: Coefficients with 95 % confidence intervals for a change towards an inegalitarian or egalitarian division of housework. Dependent variable: Change in GRA item 2) (“Men should participate in housework to the same extent as women”). Negative values indicate a change in GRAs in a more egalitarian direction, positive values a change in a less egalitarian direction. pairfam, own calculations
Figure 1:

Coefficients with 95 % confidence intervals for a change towards an inegalitarian or egalitarian division of housework. Dependent variable: Change in GRA item 2) (“Men should participate in housework to the same extent as women”). Negative values indicate a change in GRAs in a more egalitarian direction, positive values a change in a less egalitarian direction. pairfam, own calculations

The bottom paragraph of Table 3 shows the average GRA change score by the change in the division of family labor. Matching this paper’s expectations, respondents who experienced emerging cognitive dissonance between pre-pandemic GRAs and pandemic behavior also changed their attitude in the direction of the changed behavior. Interestingly, also respondents who experienced no change in the family labor division changed their attitude on average in a more egalitarian direction. This supports the notion that cognitive dissonance is not the only theoretical mechanism explaining attitudinal change (Schober & Scott 2012). Unfortunately, using the asymmetrical fixed effects approach only allows to analyze the effects of intra-individual changes in the independent variables. Hence, I am unable to examine possible GRA changes for those respondents who experience no change in their division of family labor. This limitation should be kept in mind.

4.2 Asymmetrical Fixed Effects Models

Figures 1, 2 and 3 show the coefficients for changes in the division of housework (Figure 1) and childcare (Figure 2 & 3), derived from the asymmetrical fixed effects regressions. Coefficients are shown for changes towards a more inegalitarian (H1) and towards a more egalitarian (H2) division of family labor. If the intervals do not overlap with zero (dotted line), the coefficients are statistically significant at least on a 5 % level. Coefficients are shown for the whole sub-samples (with and without controls) and separated by sex (without controls). The detailed regression tables can be found in Tables 4–6.

Figure 2: Coefficients with 95 % confidence intervals for a change towards an egalitarian and inegalitarian division of childcare. Dependent variable: Change in GRA item 1) (“Women should be more concerned about their family than about their career”). Negative values indicate a change in GRAs in a more egalitarian direction, positive values a change in a less egalitarian direction. pairfam, own calculations
Figure 2:

Coefficients with 95 % confidence intervals for a change towards an egalitarian and inegalitarian division of childcare. Dependent variable: Change in GRA item 1) (“Women should be more concerned about their family than about their career”). Negative values indicate a change in GRAs in a more egalitarian direction, positive values a change in a less egalitarian direction. pairfam, own calculations

Division of housework

As can be seen in Figure 1, changes in the division of housework towards an egalitarian distribution can be significantly associated with changes in GRA item 2) in a more egalitarian direction for all groups. The effect is largest for women (-1.03) and smallest for parents (-0.72). It should be noted that the number of respondents who changed towards an egalitarian distribution is quite small (see Table 3), which is why the results should be interpreted cautiously. While changes in the division of housework in an inegalitarian direction can be associated with a less egalitarian GRA, the coefficients are small for all groups and only significant for the whole sub-sample and for women. The greatest change can be found for women (0.13).

Division of childcare

Figure 2 shows that changes in the division of childcare towards an egalitarian distribution can be significantly associated with changes in GRA item 1) in a more egalitarian direction for all groups. The association is largest for women (-0.47) and smallest for men (-0.39).

Changes in the division of childcare in an inegalitarian direction can be significantly associated with a change towards less egalitarian GRAs for the whole sample (with and without controls) and for women. For men, the association is positive but not significant. The association found is largest for women (0.52) and smallest for men (0.22).

Table 4:

Asymmetrical fixed effects models from the GRA item 2) sub-sample. Dependent variable: Change in GRA item 2) (“Men should participate in housework to the same extent as women”). Negative coefficients indicate a more egalitarian GRA and positive coefficients a less egalitarian GRA. pairfam, own calculations

GRA item 2)

Whole sample

Whole sample w/ controls

Only women

Only men

Only parents

Housework: Change to egalitarian

-0.948***

(0.12)

-0.827***

(0.13)

-1.030***

(0.16)

-0.840***

(0.19)

-0.722***

(0.14)

Housework: Change to inegalitarian

0.122*

(0.04)

0.106

(0.05)

0.128*

(0.05)

0.113

(0.07)

0.130

(0.06)

Number of children

-0.003

(0.06)

Hours in employment

-0.004

(0.00)

Income

-0.000

(0.00)

Observations

1566

1152

849

717

844

Standard errors in parentheses

* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001

Table 5:

Asymmetrical fixed effects models from the GRA item 1) sample. Dependent variable: Change in GRA item 1) (“Women should be more concerned about their family than about their career”). Negative coefficients indicate a more egalitarian GRA and positive coefficients a less egalitarian GRA. Pairfam, own calculations

GRA item 1)

Whole sample

Whole sample w/ controls

Only women

Only men

Childcare: Change to egalitarian

-0.436***

(0.06)

-0.408***

(0.07)

-0.474***

(0.09)

-0.390***

(0.09)

Childcare: Change to inegalitarian

0.386***

(0.11)

0.316**

(0.10)

0.522***

(0.15)

0.216

(0.14)

Number of children

-0.069

(0.09)

Hours in employment

-0.000

(0.00)

Income

-0.000

(0.00)

Observations

1096

756

640

456

Standard errors in parentheses

* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001

Figure 3 shows that changes in the division of childcare towards an egalitarian distribution can be significantly associated with changes in the GRA item 3) in a more egalitarian direction for all groups. The association is largest for the whole sample with control variables (-0.68). Men change their attitude slightly more than women: -0.65 vs. -0.60 respectively.

Changes in the division of childcare in an inegalitarian direction can be significantly associated with a change towards less egalitarian GRAs for all groups. The association is largest for women (0.43) and smallest for the whole sample with control variables (0.32).

4.3 Sensitivity Analyses

Various sensitivity analyzes were conducted to test the robustness of the models shown above (see Tables A5-A11).

To adjust for potential floor and ceiling effects that might affect the GRA items due to their measurement on a 5-point Likert-scale, I replicated all analyzes using Tobit regressions (McBee 2010). All results from the main analyzes are robust.

Also, pandemic-related methodological issues need to be considered and analyzed. In wave 13, panel stability was lower than in previous pairfam waves (Brüderl et al. 2022). This could be a result to the pandemic but also result from lower panel attachment in the refreshment sample that was introduced in wave 11, especially affecting the younger cohorts (Brüderl et al. 2022). To examine who dropped out of the panel from wave 11 to wave 13, I created summary statistics comparing all those who participated in wave 11 but not in wave 13 (26 %) to all those who participated in both waves (Table A9).

Table 6:

Asymmetrical fixed effects models from the GRA item 3) sample. Dependent variable: Change in GRA item 3) (“A child under 6 will suffer from having a working mother”). Negative coefficients indicate a more egalitarian GRA and positive coefficients a less egalitarian GRA. pairfam, own calculations

GRA item 3)

Whole sample

Whole sample w/ controls

Only women

Only men

Childcare: Change to egalitarian

-0.627***

(0.08)

-0.675***

(0.09)

-0.597***

(0.12)

-0.653***

(0.11)

Childcare: Change to inegalitarian

0.403***

(0.09)

0.322**

(0.11)

0.427***

(0.12)

0.364*

(0.16)

Number of children

-0.042

(0.12)

Hours in employment

0.007*

(0.00)

Income

0.000

(0.00)

Observations

994

703

541

453

Standard errors in parentheses

* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001

Especially younger respondents dropped out of the panel, as well as respondents who were on average more lowly educated, had a lower income, spent more time on the job and had younger children. Hence, it could be that especially time constrained respondents dropped out of the panel. Looking at GRAs, respondents who dropped out had on average less egalitarian attitudes concerning items 1) and 3) than those who participated in waves 11 and 13. Attitudes about item 2) as well as the division of family labor do not vary substantially across samples. This potential selection bias could lead to an underestimation of the real effect and should be kept in mind when interpreting the results.

The mode switch to a mixed-mode design during the pandemic might also bias the results (Groffen et al. 2021). In the case of pairfam, mode-specific selection effects (Fessler et al. 2018) can be neglected, because respondents could self-select into the interview mode. However, mode-specific measurement errors (i. e., respondents answering questions differently across modes) might bias the results. Measurement errors are likely to occur with sensitive questions (Stockdale et al. 2008). Hence, it could be that GRA changes towards less egalitarian attitudes are underestimated in the models because respondents tend to be more biased by social desirability in CATI interviews (Holbrook et al. 2003) and report more moderate attitudes.

To account for potential mode effects, in Tables A9-A11, I replicated the main analyzes while including a control variable for the change in mode. In almost all models, the change in mode has a significant effect and can be associated with a change in GRAs in a more egalitarian direction. Also, the association between a change to an inegalitarian division of family labor and a change toward less egalitarian attitudes become more pronounced. Hence, it could be that respondents who reported more egalitarian attitudes during the pandemic more often self-selected into CATI mode or that attitudes are reported to be more egalitarian in CATI mode. However, because the effects for the main independent variables show the same tendencies, the interpretation of the main analyzes is robust. Nevertheless, it should be noted that it is not possible to fully decompose mode from pandemic effects, and hence, all analyses should be interpreted cautiously (Bozoyan et al. 2021).

5 Discussion

Using data derived from the German Family Panel ‘pairfam’ and analyzing it by employing an asymmetrical fixed effect approach, I explored the influences of the COVID-19 pandemic-related emergence of cognitive dissonance resulting from changes in the division of family labor (i. e., housework and childcare) on GRAs of men and women with no pre-pandemic behavior-attitude discrepancy in cohabitating heterosexual relationships in Germany.

Figure 3: Coefficients with 95 % confidence intervals for a change towards an inegalitarian and egalitarian division of childcare. Dependent variable: Change in GRA item 3) (“A child under 6 will suffer from having a working mother”). Negative values indicate a change in GRAs in a more egalitarian direction, positive values a change in a less egalitarian direction. pairfam, own calculations
Figure 3:

Coefficients with 95 % confidence intervals for a change towards an inegalitarian and egalitarian division of childcare. Dependent variable: Change in GRA item 3) (“A child under 6 will suffer from having a working mother”). Negative values indicate a change in GRAs in a more egalitarian direction, positive values a change in a less egalitarian direction. pairfam, own calculations

The study adds to the body of literature on the development and change of GRAs due to life events or changes in lived realities. It also extents previous research on the relationship between changes in familial behavior and GRAs (Baxter et al. 2015; Grinza et al. 2022; Schober & Scott 2012; Tavits et al. 2023; Zoch & Schober 2018). Additionally, it adds to the literature of pandemic-related changes in GRAs (Reichelt et al. 2021; Vandecasteele et al. 2022).

The hypotheses tested in this study state that the pandemic-related emergence of cognitive dissonance due to changes in the division of family labor (housework and childcare, respectively) toward a more gender inegalitarian (i. e., woman does all or most labor) or egalitarian distribution (i. e., equal distribution or man does all or most labor) can be associated with a change in GRAs toward a less (H1) or more egalitarian (H2) mindset, respectively, in order to reduce cognitive dissonance. Because of the greater implications of gendered norms for women (Grinza et al. 2022), I assume that women more strongly adapt their attitudes to match their behavior (H3). To ensure that changes in the division of family labor actually produce cognitive dissonance, in the analyzes only respondents were considered that reported no attitude-behavior discrepancy before the pandemic. I analyzed single GRA items because of the assumed multidimensionality of GRAs (Grunow et al. 2018).

Overall, I found support for the first two hypotheses in all three sub-samples. However, the findings from the GRA item 2) sub-sample show the least support for an association between emerging cognitive dissonance and attitudinal change: Changes in the division of housework in an egalitarian direction show expected associations, however, the size of the group changing their behavior in this direction is critically small, which is why results need to be interpreted cautiously. While changes in the division of housework in an inegalitarian direction can indeed be associated with less egalitarian GRAs, the association is smaller than in both childcare sub-samples and only statistically significant for the whole sample without control variables and for women. One explanation could be that respondents do not report substantially less egalitarian views on GRA item 2) because it is very skewed towards egalitarianism and affected by social desirability (Hudde & Engelhardt 2021).

Like expected by H3, in all cases, women show greater changes in GRAs associated with emerging cognitive dissonance, besides when looking at the association between a change in the distribution of childcare in an egalitarian direction and GRA item 3). Thereby, the association is greater for men than for women. An explanation could be that men who took over more childcare during the pandemic do not agree that their children “suffer” if it’s not the mother taking primarily care of them like suggested by item 3).

Concluding, the findings within the pandemic setting support the assumption that cognitive dissonance is a useful theoretical and psychological mechanism to explain intra-individual GRA change while reducing endogeneity. However, it most likely is not the only motivator of attitudinal change (Schober & Scott 2012). This can also be seen when looking at the descriptive findings: Also respondents who experience no change in the division of family labor on average changed their GRAs in a more egalitarian direction. One reason for this could be that while the proportional division of family labor remained the same, the total amount of family labor responsibilities increased and hence, cognitive dissonance emerged but is not captured in the models. Another reason for attitudinal change could be the prominent discussion of gender roles and the concern about a ‘re-traditionalization’ of gender roles (Allmendinger 2020) during the pandemic. Being exposed to those discussions might also trigger attitudinal adjustments (Vandecasteele et al. 2022).

The COVID-19 pandemic presents a unique setting in which persisting inequalities became more visible and widely discussed and in which sociological mechanisms and changes can be examined while reducing endogeneity. Because gender relations in Germany are inequal and women took and take over the lion’s share of unpaid family labor (e. g., Hiekel & Kühn 2023), the findings of this paper highlight the influential relationship between behavior and attitudes and hence, the importance of political and institutional settings that encourage gender egalitarian behaviors to further promote gender egalitarian attitudes in the society and foster gender equality. The findings show that settings that complicate gender egalitarian behaviors can indeed encourage less egalitarian attitudes, endangering progresses towards gender equality.

Limitations

The paper has limitations that need to be considered. A conceptual limitation is that reversed causality cannot be ruled out: The causal order of the association between behavior and attitudes is unclear, as they are found to engage in constant feedback (Hudde et al. 2022; Preston 2023). This issue is further enhanced through the fact that attitudes and behavior are measured at the same timepoint in the analyzed data. Hence, further research should analyze data that allow for temporal and causal ordering.

Secondly, I only analyze respondents who reported no pre-pandemic attitude-behavior discrepancy, which I assume can be translated into experiencing no cognitive dissonance. A change in the division of family labor due to (assumingly) external constraints during the pandemic is interpreted as producing cognitive dissonance. However, as the actual feeling of discomfort or dissonance is not asked for in pairfam and a change in family labor not necessarily results from the pandemic, these theoretical mechanisms are only assumptions (Schober & Scott 2012; Vandecasteele et al. 2022).

Considering only respondents who reported no attitude-behavior discrepancy before the pandemic means to only analyzing a very selective group. The mechanisms of changing attitudes for respondents who already experienced attitude-behavior discrepancy before the pandemic hence remain undiscovered. Next to that, due to the first difference method used, mechanisms of changing attitudes when no changes in behavior occur cannot be examined by the analyzes. As the division of family labor is only captured in a relative measure, cognitive dissonance could also emerge due to a change in the absolute amount of family labor, even though the relative division remains the same. These associations are not captured in the model, given the relative measurement of family labor division. Both latter points show that attitudes and behaviors do not always align and support the notion that cognitive dissonance is not the only motivator of attitudinal changes and formation (Schober & Scott 2012). Amongst others, age- and cohort effects might also influence the development of GRAs as well as the division of family labor. Future research should aim to address other mechanisms influencing attitude formation over the life course beyond cognitive dissonance.

Like in other comparable research on GRA change (e. g., Schober & Scott 2012; Zoch & Schober 2018), effect sizes found in the models are moderate. However, it still can provide insights into psychological mechanisms likely influencing attitude formation over the life course.

There are also some important data limitations. Firstly, when only considering respondents who reported no behavior-attitude discrepancy before the pandemic, the sample sizes especially of those who experienced a change in the division of family labor is small, decreasing the statistical power. Other research on cognitive dissonance and GRA change suffers from the same issue (Zoch & Schober 2018). This should be considered when interpreting the results and future research should aim to analyze larger data sets.

Secondly, the items measuring GRAs in pairfam (and many other surveys) are not up to the current stand of research because they are based on a traditional view on gender roles and do not cover variances and differences in egalitarianism (Pérez Sánchez et al. 2021). Future surveys should aim to more validly measure GRAs.

An issue resulting from the data collection is a possible mode effect, as wave 13 was conducted with a mixture of CAPI and CATI methods (Brüderl et al. 2022). Furthermore, pairfam is affected by attrition: A possible selection bias might be that the most time constrained respondents (e. g., parents with young children) dropped out during the pandemic (Bozoyan et al. 2021). Even in light of the sensitivity analyses I conducted, it is not possible to fully separate potential pandemic effects from mode and selection effects. Findings should thus be interpreted with caution.

Keeping the limitations in mind, this paper contributed to both pandemic research as well as research considering the micro-level processes of the behavior-attitude relationship and gender equality in Germany. Analyzing rich longitudinal data allowed for sensitive and detailed analyzes of pandemic-related emergences of cognitive dissonance due to changes in the division of family labor and changes in GRAs during the pandemic, which have not been conducted before. While it cannot be concluded that cognitive dissonance is the only motivator of attitudinal change, aligning with other research, cognitive dissonance shows to be one useful mechanism of explaining heterogenous attitudinal changes across the life course, in a setting in which endogeneity can be reduced.

About the author

Katrin Firl

Katrin Firl, geb. 1996 in Köln. Studium der Sozialwissenschaften, Soziologie und Demografie in Köln und Groningen. Seit 2023 Promotion an der Universität Mannheim. Seit 2022 wissenschaftliche Mitarbeiterin bei GESIS – Leibniz-Institut für Sozialwissenschaften. Seit 2024 im Team Family Surveys zur Erhebung des FReDA-Paneldatensatzes tätig. Forschungsschwerpunkte: Familiensoziologie; Geschlechtergerechtigkeit.

Wichtigste Publikation: (2024). FReDA – The German Family Demography Panel Study. GESIS, Cologne. ZA7777 Data File Version 4.0.0. http://dx.doi.org/10.4232/1.14195 (mit M. Bujard, T. Gummer, K. Hank, F. J. Neyer, R. Pollak, N. Schneider, C. K. Spieß, C. Wolf, I. Bauer, S. Börlin, D. Bretschi, K. Brüggemann, P. Christmann, R. Edinger, F. Eigenbrodt, L. C. Frembs, K. Groß, S. Hoherz, T. Kunz, D. Lück, R. Naderi, E. Naumann, T. Nutz, A.-S. Oehrlein, K. Ruckdeschel, L. Schmid, A. Schumann, N. Schumann, A. Stein, C. Thönnissen, E. Ullrich).

Declarations

  1. Funding and/or Conflicts of interest/Competing interests: On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

  2. This paper was kindly supported by the VolkswagenStiftung.

Acknowledgements

This paper uses data from the German Family Panel pairfam, coordinated by Josef Brüderl, Sonja Drobnic, Karsten Hank, Johannes Huinink, Bernhard Nauck, Franz J. Neyer, and Sabine Walper. The study was funded from 2004 to 2022 as a priority program and long-term project by the German Research Foundation (DFG).

A special thank you to Dorothée Behr, Michael Braun, Karsten Hank, and Anna Hebel for their support and helpful feedback throughout the writing process.

The author wishes to thank the two anonymous reviewers for their extensive and helpful feedback on an earlier version of the article.

Replication Data

The replication data (Stata programme syntax) can be found at the following address: GESIS Archiving https://doi.org/10.7802/2722

Die Replikationsdaten (Programmsyntax Stata) finden sich unter folgender Adresse: GESIS Archivierung https://doi.org/10.7802/2722

Literature

Allison, P. D., 2019: Asymmetric Fixed-Effects Models for Panel Data. Socius: Sociological Research for a Dynamic World 5: 1–12.10.1177/2378023119826441Search in Google Scholar

Allmendinger, J., 2020: Zurück in alte Rollen – Corona bedroht die Geschlechtergerechtigkeit. WZB Mitteilungen Heft 168: 45–47.Search in Google Scholar

Baxter, J., S. Buchler, F. Perales, & M. Western, 2015: A Life-Changing Event: First Births and Men’s and Women’s Attitudes to Mothering and Gender Divisions of Labor. Social Forces 93(3): 989–1014.10.1093/sf/sou103Search in Google Scholar

Boll, C., D. Müller, & S. Schüller, 2021: Neither Backlash nor Convergence: Dynamics of Intracouple Childcare Division after the First COVID-19 Lockdown and Subsequent Reopening in Germany. IZA Institute of Labor Economics 14375: 1–37.10.2139/ssrn.3851051Search in Google Scholar

Boring, A., & G. Moroni, 2022: Turning Back the Clock: Beliefs in Gender Norms during Lockdown. Sciences PO LIEPP Working Paper 133: 1–45.Search in Google Scholar

Bozoyan, C., M. Herzig, R. Preetz, & C. Schmiedeberg, 2021: Effects of Survey Design Modifications in Pairfam Wave 12 Due to the COVID-19 Pandemic. pairfam Technical Paper No. 16.Search in Google Scholar

Brüderl, J., S. Drobnič, K. Hank, F. Neyer, S. Walper, P. Alt, E. Borschel, C. Bozoyan, M. Garrett, S. Geissler, T. Gonzales Avilés, N. Gröpler, K. Hajek, M. Herzig, R. Lenke, R. Lorenz, K. Lutz, T. Peter, R. Preetz, J. Reim, B. Sawatzki, C. Schmiedeberg, P. Schütze, N. Schumann, C. Thönnissen, K. Timmermann, & M. Wetzel, 2022: The German Family Panel (Pairfam).Search in Google Scholar

Brüderl, J., M. Garrett, K. Hajek, M. Herzig, R. Lenke, R. Lorenz, P. Schütze, N. Schumann, & K. Timmermann, 2022: Pairfam Data Manual, Release 13.0. LMU Munich: Technical Report GESIS Data Archive, Cologne.Search in Google Scholar

Brüderl, J., & V. Ludwig, 2014: Fixed-Effects Panel Regression. P. 327–58 in: H. Best & C. Wolf (ed.), The SAGE Handbook of Regression Analysis and Causal Inference. London: SAGE Publications Ltd.10.4135/9781446288146.n15Search in Google Scholar

Brüderl, J, C. Schmiedeberg, L. Castiglioni, O. Arránz Becker, P. Buhr, D. Fuß, V. Ludwig, J. Schröder, & N. Schumann, 2022: The German Family Panel: Study Design and Cumulated Field Report (Waves 1 to 13).Search in Google Scholar

Danzer, N., M. Huebener, A. Pape, C. K. Spieß, N. A. Siegel, & G. G. Wagner, 2021: Cracking under Pressure? Gender Role Attitudes toward Maternal Employment in Times of a Pandemic. DIW Berlin Discussion Papers 1951: 1–7.10.2139/ssrn.3870202Search in Google Scholar

Davis, S. N., & T. N. Greenstein, 2009: Gender Ideology: Components, Predictors, and Consequences. Annual Review of Sociology 35(1): 87–105.10.1146/annurev-soc-070308-115920Search in Google Scholar

Fessler, P., M. Kasy, & P. Lindner, 2018: Survey Mode Effects on Measured Income Inequality. The Journal of Economic Inequality 16(4): 487–505.10.1007/s10888-018-9378-xSearch in Google Scholar

Festinger, L., 1957: A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press.10.1515/9781503620766Search in Google Scholar

Globisch, C., D. Müller, M. Fuchs, B. Christoph, V. Danneck, S. Dummert, M. Volkert, A. Abendroth, S. Anger, C. Boll, T. Carstensen, L. Fervers, L. Hipp, M. Jacob, V. Knize, M. Kreyenfeld, Y. Lott, T. Naujoks, A. Sauermann, S. Schüller, & L. Tobler, 2022: IAB-Kurzbericht – Aufteilung der Sorge- und Erwerbsarbeit zwischen Frauen und Männern: In der Pandemie ändern sich Geschlechterrollen kaum. Institut für Arbeitsmarkt-und Berufsforschung.Search in Google Scholar

Grinza, E., F. Devicienti, M. Rossi, & D. Vannoni, 2022: How Entry into Parenthood Shapes Gender Role Attitudes: New Evidence from the UK. Feminist Economics 28(4): 194–220.10.1080/13545701.2022.2081352Search in Google Scholar

Groffen, D., G. Goris, K. van Berkel, & J. van den Brakel, 2021: Effects of Adjustments in Face-to-Face Data Collection Due to the COVID-19 Pandemic on Survey Results. Conference of European Statisticians – Expert Meeting on Statistical Data Collection (Session 2).Search in Google Scholar

Grunow, D., K. Begall, & S. Buchler, 2018: Gender Ideologies in Europe: A Multidimensional Framework. Journal of Marriage and Family 80(1): 42–60.10.1111/jomf.12453Search in Google Scholar

Hank, K., & A. Steinbach, 2020: The Virus Changed Everything, Didn’t It? Couples’ Division of Housework and Childcare before and during the Corona Crisis. Journal of Family Research 33(1): 99–114.10.20377/jfr-488Search in Google Scholar

Harmon-Jones, E., & C. Harmon-Jones, 2007: Cognitive Dissonance Theory After 50 Years of Development. Zeitschrift Für Sozialpsychologie 38(1): 7–16.10.1024/0044-3514.38.1.7Search in Google Scholar

Hiekel, N., & M. Kühn, 2023: Gender Inequality in Childcare and Parental Mental Health during the Covid-19 Pandemic in Germany. Do Gender Role Attitudes Matter? WP-2023-007. Rostock: Max Planck Institute for Demographic Research.10.4054/MPIDR-WP-2023-007Search in Google Scholar

Holbrook, A. L., M. C. Green, & J. A. Krosnick, 2003: Telephone versus Face-to-Face Interviewing of National Probability Samples with Long Questionnaires. Public Opinion Quarterly 67(1): 79–125.10.1086/346010Search in Google Scholar

Hudde, A., & H. Engelhardt, 2021: Intra-Couple (Dis)Similarity in Gender Role Attitudes and the Transition to Parenthood in Germany. European Sociological Review 36(6): 852–67.10.1093/esr/jcaa024Search in Google Scholar

Hudde, A., K. Hank, & M. Jacob, 2022: Gender Role Attitudes Cannot Explain How British Couples Responded to Increased Housework Demands during the COVID-19 Pandemic. Socius: Sociological Research for a Dynamic World 7: 1–21.10.1177/23780231211064395Search in Google Scholar

Huinink, J., J. Brüderl, B. Nauck, S. Walper, L. Castiglioni, & M. Feldhaus, 2011: Panel Analysis of Intimate Relationships and Family Dynamics (Pairfam): Conceptual Framework and Design. Zeitschrift Für Familienforschung – Journal of Family Research 23(1): 77–101.10.20377/jfr-235Search in Google Scholar

Illing, H., M. Oberfichtner, N. Pestel, J. Schmieder, & S. Trenkle, 2022: IAB-Kurzbericht – Geschlechtsspezifische Arbeitsmarktwirkung der Covid-19-Pandemie. Institut für Arbeitsmarkt- und Berufsforschung.Search in Google Scholar

Jessen, J., C. K. Spiess, S. W., & K. Wrohlich, 2021: Sharing the Caring? The Gender Division of Care Work During the Covid-19 Pandemic in Germany. IZA DP 14457: 1–22.10.2139/ssrn.3870188Search in Google Scholar

Knight, C. R., & M. C. Brinton, 2017: One Egalitarianism or Several? Two Decades of Gender-Role Attitude Change in Europe. American Journal of Sociology 122(5): 1485–1532.10.1086/689814Search in Google Scholar

Kohlrausch, B., & A. Zucco, 2020: Die Corona Krise trifft Frauen doppelt – Weniger Erwerbseinkommen und mehr Sorgearbeit. Policy Brief WSI 40.Search in Google Scholar

Kreyenfeld, M., & S. Zinn, 2021: Coronavirus and Care: How the Coronavirus Crisis Affected Fathers’ Involvement in Germany. Demographic Research 44: 99–124.10.4054/DemRes.2021.44.4Search in Google Scholar

Kühhirt, M., 2012: Childbirth and the Long-Term Division of Labour within Couples: How Do Substitution, Bargaining Power, and Norms Affect Parents’ Time Allocation in West Germany? European Sociological Review 28(5): 565–82.10.1093/esr/jcr026Search in Google Scholar

McBee, M., 2010: Modeling Outcomes with Floor or Ceiling Effects: An Introduction to the Tobit Model. Gifted Child Quarterly 54(4): 314–20.10.1177/0016986210379095Search in Google Scholar

Möhring, K., E. Naumann, M. Reifenscheid, A. G. Blom, A. Wenz, T. Rettig, R. Lehrer, U. Krieger, S. Juhl, S. Friedel, M. Fikel, & C. Cornesse, 2020: Die Mannheimer Corona-Studie: Schwerpunktbericht zu Erwerbstätigkeit und Kinderbetreuung.Search in Google Scholar

Naujoks, T., M. Kreyenfeld, & S. Dummert, 2022: The Division of Child Care during the Coronavirus Crisis in Germany: How Did Short-Time Work Affect Fathers’ Engagement? Journal of Family Research 34(1): 67–98.10.20377/jfr-717Search in Google Scholar

Nitsche, N., & D. Grunow, 2016: Housework over the Course of Relationships: Gender Ideology, Resources, and the Division of Housework from a Growth Curve Perspective. Advances in Life Course Research 29: 80–94.10.1016/j.alcr.2016.02.001Search in Google Scholar

Pérez Sánchez, B., A. Concha-Salgado, A. Fernández-Suárez, J. Juarros-Basterretxea, & F. J. Rodríguez-Díaz, 2021: The Gender Role Attitude Scale (GRAS) as an Alternative for the Crisis in Measurement of Attitudes towards Gender Roles in Latin America: A Study in Chilean University Students. Anales de Psicología 37(3): 567–76.10.6018/analesps.438431Search in Google Scholar

Preston, A., 2023: Changing Gender Role Attitudes and the Changing Gender Gap in Labour Force Participation. Journal of Industrial Relations: 1–29.10.1177/00221856231177118Search in Google Scholar

Reichelt, M., K. Makovi, & A. Sargsyan, 2021: The Impact of COVID-19 on Gender Inequality in the Labor Market and Gender-Role Attitudes. European Societies 23: 228–45.10.1080/14616696.2020.1823010Search in Google Scholar

Schober, P., & J. Scott, 2012: Maternal Employment and Gender Role Attitudes: Dissonance among British Men and Women in the Transition to Parenthood. Work, Employment and Society 26(3): 514–30.10.1177/0950017012438577Search in Google Scholar

Stockdale, J. D., V. E. Thornburg, & A. P. Aldridge, 2008: Same Respondents, Same Questions; Different Modes, Different Responses. Survey Research Methods 14(2): 223–27.Search in Google Scholar

Sullivan, O., 2013: What Do We Learn About Gender by Analyzing Housework Separately From Child Care? Some Considerations from Time-Use Evidence: Gender, Housework, Child Care. Journal of Family Theory & Review 5(2): 72–84.10.1111/jftr.12007Search in Google Scholar

Tavits, M., P. Schleiter, J. Homola, & D. Ward, 2023: Fathers’ Leave Reduces Sexist Attitudes. American Political Science Review: 1–7.10.1017/S0003055423000369Search in Google Scholar

Vandecasteele, L., K. Ivanova, I. Sieben, & T. Reeskens, 2022: Changing Attitudes about the Impact of Women’s Employment on Families: The COVID‐19 Pandemic Effect. Gender, Work & Organization: 1–22.10.1111/gwao.12874Search in Google Scholar

West, C., & D. H. Zimmermann, 1987: Doing Gender. Gender & Society 1(2): 125–51.10.1177/0891243287001002002Search in Google Scholar

Zinn, S., M. Kreyenfeld, & M. Bayer, 2020: Kinderbetreuung in Corona-Zeiten: Mütter tragen die Hauptlast, aber Väter holen auf. DIW Berlin 51: 6.Search in Google Scholar

Zoch, G., A-C. Bächmann, & B. Vicari, 2021: Who Cares When Care Closes? Care-Arrangements and Parental Working Conditions during the COVID-19 Pandemic. European Societies 23: 576–88.10.1080/14616696.2020.1832700Search in Google Scholar

Zoch, G., & P. Schober, 2018: Public Child-Care Expansion and Changing Gender Ideologies of Parents in Germany. Journal of Marriage and Family 80: 1020–39.10.1111/jomf.12486Search in Google Scholar

Zucco, A., & Y. Lott, 2021: Stand der Gleichstellung – Ein Jahr mit Corona. Wirschafts- und Sozialwissenschaftliches Institut (WSI) 64.Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2024-08-22
Published in Print: 2024-08-30

© 2024 bei den Autorinnen und Autoren, publiziert von De Gruyter.

Dieses Werk ist lizensiert unter einer Creative Commons Namensnennung 4.0 International Lizenz.

Downloaded on 8.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/zfsoz-2024-2019/html
Scroll to top button