Home The inhibitory effect of conditioned pain modulation on temporal summation in low-back pain patients
Article Publicly Available

The inhibitory effect of conditioned pain modulation on temporal summation in low-back pain patients

  • Søren O’Neill ORCID logo EMAIL logo , Liam Holm , Johanne Brinch Filtenborg , Lars Arendt-Nielsen and Casper Glissmann Nim
Published/Copyright: May 27, 2021
Become an author with De Gruyter Brill

Abstract

Objectives

The literature on conditioned pain modulation (CPM) is inconclusive in relation to low-back pain and it is unclear how CPM affects temporal summation as a proxy of central pain integration. The aim of this study was to examine whether the CPM effect would be different on pain induced by temporal summation than single stimuli in a group of low back pain patients.

Methods

A total of 149 low-back pain patients were included. CPM was examined using single, repeated and temporal summation (repeated-single difference) of mechanical pressure pain as test stimuli at an individualized, fixed supra-pain-threshold force, before and after 2 min of cold pressor test (0–2 degrees Celsius). Participants were categorized as CPM responders or non-responders according to three different criteria: simple (any pain inhibition), strict (pain inhibition of more than 10VAS) and reversed (pain inhibition or facilitation of less than 10VAS). Clinical data on back pain was collected for correlation and descriptive purposes.

Results

Significant modulation was observed for all three test stimuli. Effects sizes were comparable in relative terms, but repeated pressure pain modulation was greater in absolute terms. No correlations to clinical data were observed, for any measure.

Conclusions

The current data suggests that repeated pressure pain may be better suited as the CPM test stimuli, than single pressure pain and temporal summation of pressure pain, as the CPM effect in absolute terms was greater. Employing temporal summation as the test stimulus in a CPM paradigm may be more sensitive than a single test stimulus.

Introduction

The Conditioned Pain Modulation (CPM) test paradigm quantifies the pain-inhibits-pain effect of an intense conditioning stimulus on a test stimulus. CPM is considered a reflection of brainstem mediated, diffuse descending inhibition of nociception [1] and as such may play a role in clinical pain conditions.

The literature on CPM in clinical cohorts seems more disparate than that of simpler Quantitative Sensory Tests (QST) and controversies exist in the field of low-back pain. A recent systematic review of QST in Low Back Pain (LBP) [2] found that Pressure Pain Thresholds (PPT) were almost uniformly lower compared to healthy controls. By contrast, group differences in CPM effects were found in two [3], [4] out of four studies. Differences in CPM effects have been reported within LBP populations and Gerhardt et al. [5] speculate that it “might be because many patients with [chronic low back pain] report pain in further areas of the body, and altered CPM might influence spatial extent of pain rather than chronic back pain per se.”

The reliability of CPM tests has been reported as good or excellent but “is heavily dependent on stimulation parameters and study methodology” [6] and Martel et al. [7] reported important differences in the temporal stability of CPM responses between the sexes. Despite concerns about the robustness of CPM, it arguably could have important clinical relevance. Although LBP is a heterogeneous diagnostic group, widespread hyperalgesia and painful comorbidity are common findings [2], [8], which arguably could result from such attenuated CPM effects.

Most CPM procedures employ a simple test stimulus such as the detection threshold of pressure pain or heat pain. Perhaps more complex testing stimuli which rely on spinal integration of noxious stimuli could have different sensitive to CPM effects than simpler stimuli. Temporal summation is one such complex pain test, reflecting an additive or facilitatory effect of repeated stimuli on wide-dynamic range (WDR) neurons in the spinal dorsal horn [9]. The descending noxious inhibition which CPM test procedures are intended to induce are also believed to affect the WDR neurons, albeit in an inhibitory fashion [10]. Temporal summation and CPM thus likely exert opposing effects on the WDR neurons. As TS of pain is typically perceived as more painful than single stimuli, the CPM could arguably be more pronounced on TS, although this is uncertain.

Little has been published on the CPM effect on complex pain stimuli like TS in humans, and two smaller human volunteer studies reported conflicting findings. Holden et al. [11] found no CPM effect on TS, whereas Sirucek et al. [12] did. Attenuated descending pain inhibition could be an explanation for widespread increased pain sensitivity in chronic low back pain patients. To our knowledge no studies have been published on CPM effects on TS in a clinical population.

The present study investigated the CPM effect on three different test stimuli: Single- and repeated pressure pain intensity and temporal summation of pressure pain intensity.

The hypothesis was that the CPM effect would be different on pain induced by temporal summation compared to single stimuli. Secondarily, we examined whether being a CPM responder or non-responder was associated with clinical pain measures.

Methods

A cross-sectional, experimental study of a consecutive, convenience sample of low back pain patients. The study was approved by the Regional Committee on Health Research Ethics, Denmark (ref. number S-20180098).

Participants

All patients referred between February and October (2019) to the Spine Center of Southern Denmark, Lillebaelt Hospital with low back pain as the primary complaint were invited to participate in the study. The Spine Center of Southern Denmark is a large regional spine-care unit which attends to patients on referral from other hospital departments and general medical practitioners, chiropractors and medical consultants in private practice.

Inclusion criteria were: 18+ years of age, ability to speak and understand Danish, referred with a primary complaint of low back pain (defined as dorsal pain, muscle tension, or stiffness localized below the lower costal margin and above the inferior gluteal folds, with or without sciatica), a completed SpineData questionnaire [13], written consent for the use of SpineData for research, and no severe psychiatric disorders.

Patients were excluded from the study if they later withdrew their consent for participation, if the experimental procedure was not tolerated, or in the event of unexpected technical difficulties.

Descriptive and clinical data

Descriptive and clinical data was collected using an electronic, online questionnaire (SpineData). Descriptive data included age, sex, height and weight. Clinical data included current LBP intensity, worst and average LBP intensity over the last 14 days (0–10 Visual analog scale), pain area (quantified from digital pain drawings as convex hull polygons [14]) and LBP duration [13].

QST protocol

Participants were assessed by a research assistant trained in performing a standardized QST test protocol. The current study represents a pre-planned limited analysis of data collected as part of a larger QST test battery. An overview of the complete QST test protocol will be published elsewhere.

For the current analyses, the following QST data were included (see Figure 1):

Figure 1: 
            Study flow diagram.
Figure 1:

Study flow diagram.

Pressure pain threshold

Participants were instructed to point out the most clinically painful area or the center of clinical pain in the lower back. Pressure was applied just lateral to the midline at that segment, on the most painful side. Patients unable to identify a most painful area, were tested at the L4 segment. If a patient could not identify a most painful side, pressure was applied on the dominant side.

A series of 10 custom built, spring-loaded pressure probes were used to apply pressure for assessment of pain threshold and intensity with single and repeated pressure. The probes were constructed in a manner to allow for precise calibration by increasing or decreasing the pre-tension of the steel spring. The 10 probes were calibrated using a digital weighing cell mounted in a test-bench, to apply pressure from 1 to 10 kg in steps of 1 kg. The probe contact surface was flat, circular with an area of 1 cm2, had slightly rounded edges and were covered in 2 mm thick hard foam material. Pressure was increased smoothly over approximately ½ s, perpendicular to the skin, and maintained for approximately 1 s, before being discontinued.

Participants were instructed to indicate verbally whether an applied stimulus was perceived as painful or non-painful and the pressure pain threshold was determined using a ‘split-middles’ approach: Initially the 5 kg probe was applied. If the participant perceived this as being non-painful, the middle probe of the heavier probes (6–10 kg) was used (7.5 kg rounded up to the 8 kg probe). If the 8 kg probe was perceived as painful, the middle probe of the lighter probes (6–7 kg) was used (6.5 rounded up to the 7 kg probe). Conversely, if the participant perceived the initial 5 kg stimuli as painful, the middle lighter probe (1–4) was applied (2.5 kg rounded up to 3 kg). PPT was determined as the smallest pressure perceived as painful by the participant. Two trials were performed and the mean was calculated (mPPT).

Conditioned pain modulation test stimuli

Test stimuli consisted of mechanical pressure pain intensity, quantified using an electronic visual analogue pain scale (0–100 VAS), anchored as ‘No pain’ and ‘Worst pain imaginable’ at either end, and quantified numerically as integers between 0 and 100.

For test stimuli, the individual mean pressure pain threshold (mPPT) rounded up to nearest probe plus 1 kg (max 10 kg) was used. Thus, pain intensities were tested using a supra-threshold stimulus.

Three different test stimuli were quantified: Pain intensity with a single stimulation, repeated stimulation and temporal summation.

Initially, a single stimulation of 1 s of mechanical pressure was applied and the participant indicated pain intensity on the electronic VAS – this was recorded as Single Stimulation pain intensity. After a short pause (10 s), 10 repeated stimulations of 1 s duration (with 1 s intervals) were applied and the participant indicated pain intensity on the VAS as it changed over time – the VAS recording at the final stimulation was recorded as Repeat Stimulation pain intensity. An illustrative video of the test procedure (with a different pressure probe and test site) is available at http://www.smerteforskning.dk/videolicens/TS.mp4.

Temporal summation (TS) was calculated as the difference between repeated and single stimuli: TS=VASstim10VASstim1, i.e. positive numbers for an increase in pain intensity and vice versa.

Cold-pressor test

Cold Pressor Test (CPT) was used as the conditioning stimulus.

A 5 L water container was kept refrigerated at 0–2 °C (Mobicool C40; Dometic WAECO, Dubai, United Arab Emirates), with the temperature monitored using a thermometer. Water was kept circulating using a submersible pump (Barvig Tauchpumpe model 03, 12 V, 0.6 bar, 12 L/min). Participants were instructed to immerse their non-dominant, non-clenched hand to the wrist in the circulating water and to keep their hand immersed for 2 min, or until the pain became unbearable.

CPT pain was assessed continuously using an electronic (0–100) VAS with a frequency of 1 Hz. The CPT pain data was stored electronically and summarized as Cold Pain Detection Threshold (time from immersion to first VAS>0), Maximum Cold Pain Intensity (maximum VAS recorded), Time to Maximum Cold Pain Intensity (time from immersion to first maximum VAS recorded) and Area Under the Curve (Sum of VAS over immersion time).

Conditioned pain modulation

Pain responses to single and repeated stimuli were assessed at baseline and again immediately following completion of the CPT.

Conditioned pain modulation (CPM) was calculated as the difference between pressure pain intensity before and after the cold-pressor test, for both single and repeated stimuli and temporal summation. The CPM was calculated as CPM=VASafterVASbefore, i.e. positive numbers for an increase in pain intensity and vice versa.

The change in pain sensitivity (CPM response) is a continuous variable with no obvious or natural cut-off point for defining CPM responders and non-responders. Therefore, participants were categorized as CPM non-responders (and by inference as CPM responders) in relation to both Single and Repeated stimuli as well as Temporal summation by three different criteria: A simple criteria in which no change or any increase in test stimulus pain intensity (CPM≥0) was categorized as a CPM non-response. A strict definition in which only an increase of 10 VAS or more was categorized as a CPM non-response (CPM≥0). And a reverse definition in which any increase or even a small drop in VAS (CPM≥−0) was categorized as a CPM non-response.

QST instructions

The participant information about the QST procedures followed a fixed, pre-authored manuscript, which is available (in Danish) on request.

Data management

Study data were collected and managed using REDCap electronic data capture tools hosted by ‘Open,’ a research support initiative in the Region of Southern Denmark. REDCap (Research Electronic Data Capture) is a secure, web-based software platform designed to support data capture for research studies, providing 1) an intuitive interface for validated data capture; 2) audit trails for tracking data manipulation and export procedures; 3) automated export procedures for seamless data downloads to common statistical packages; and 4) procedures for data integration and interoperability with external sources [15], [16].

Statistical analyses

Comprehensive summary statistics of QST data and descriptive variables are presented as both parametric and non-parametric data. Distribution of QST data are presented as density plots.

The assumptions of homoscedasticity and normality of residuals for a two-way, repeated-measures ANOVA test were not fulfilled by the data. Most likely this is a result of the nature of the visual analogue pain scale, which is bounded at both ends, resulting in left-skewed data.

Instead data is presented visually and analyzed using non-parametric tests, which do not permit for analysis of variance or interactions and have less power.

Distributions are presented graphically as density plots. Density plots can be thought of as histograms which have been smoothed out and have the benefit of not being affected by the arbitrary number of bins used in histogram plotting. Also, density plots allow for simultaneous, comparative presentation of several distribution plots without obscuring overlaying plots.

CPM responder/non-responder group differences in clinical variables were analyzed non-parametrically (Wilcoxon test). Comprehensive summary statistics thereof are provided in appendix.

Results

Descriptive summary statistics

A total of 149 patients participated in the study (60 women, and 89 men).

Descriptive data is summarized in Table 1. A frequency table of clinical ICD10 diagnoses (primary and secondary) are presented in Appendix. The most common diagnoses were Low back pain, Muscle strain, Lumbago with sciatica, Lumbar and other intervertebral disc disorders with radiculopathy, Other spondylosis, Spinal stenosis, Other spondylosis with radiculopathy and Other biomechanical lesions.

Table 1:

Clinical data.

Variable n Min Max Median q1 q3 IQR Mad Mean SD SE CI
Age, years 149 21 81 58.0 45.00 69.0 24.00 17.79 55.99 15.13 1.24 2.45
Current LBP, VAS 145 0 10 5.0 4.00 7.0 3.00 2.96 5.23 2.53 0.21 0.42
Typical LBP, VAS 142 0 10 7.0 5.00 8.0 3.00 1.48 5.95 2.44 0.20 0.41
Worst LBP, VAS 145 0 10 8.0 6.00 9.0 3.00 1.48 7.11 2.69 0.22 0.44
Pain area (pixels) 149 0 25,212 1,433.0 522.00 3,428.0 2,906.00 1,690.16 3,123.64 4,437.25 363.51 718.35
Duration, days 144 0 11,355 365.0 120.75 1834.5 1713.75 449.23 1,410.35 2,261.85 188.49 372.58
Height, cm 146 155 200 175.0 169.00 182.0 13.00 10.38 175.53 9.26 0.77 1.51
Weight, kg 146 44 143 83.5 74.00 96.0 22.00 17.05 85.01 17.35 1.44 2.84
  1. Summary of descriptive and clinical data. q1=first (25%) quartile, q3=third (75%) quartile, IQR, inter-quartile range; MAD, median absolute deviation.

Summary statistics of QST

The median pressure probe employed for CPM test stimuli was the 8 kg probe (individual pressure pain threshold + 1 kg) (n=149, median=8, q1=6, q3=10, min=3, max=10).

Distributions of pain intensity (VAS) with single and repeated pressure stimulation before and after CPM are illustrated in Figure 2 as density plots.

Figure 2: 
            Density plot (smoothed histogram) of pain scores (VAS) for mechanical pressure pain, before/after cold pressor test and with single/repeat stimulation. Vertical lines represent mean values.
Figure 2:

Density plot (smoothed histogram) of pain scores (VAS) for mechanical pressure pain, before/after cold pressor test and with single/repeat stimulation. Vertical lines represent mean values.

Summary statistics of quantitative sensory testing is presented in Tables 2 and 3.

Table 2:

QST data.

Stimulus Setting n Min Max Median q1 q3 IQR MAD Mean SD SE CI
Single stim. Before CPT 149 0 74 12 1 31 30 17.79 18.04 19.71 1.61 3.19
After CPT 149 0 74 4 0 22 22 5.93 12.80 16.60 1.36 2.69
Repeated stim. Before CPT 149 0 93 33 19 53 34 28.17 35.46 24.01 1.97 3.89
After CPT 149 0 100 22 5 37 32 25.20 24.47 21.80 1.79 3.53
  1. Summary statistics of raw data: Experimental pressure pain intensity (VAS) grouped by test stimulus and setting. q1=first (25%) quartile, q3=third (75%) quartile, IQR, inter-quartile range; MAD, median absolute deviation; CPM, conditioned pain modulation; CPT, cold-pressor test; TS, temporal summation.

Table 3:

Cold-pressor test.

CPT measure n Min Max Median q1 q3 IQR MAD Mean SD SE CI
AUC (VAS × sec) 149 0 10,162 3,728 1,532 6,305 4,773 3,426.29 4,011.28 2,789.00 228.48 451.51
Max VAS, VAS 149 0 100 87 70 100 30 19.27 79.62 23.22 1.90 3.76
Time, sec 149 10 120 119 36 120 84 1.48 81.90 43.19 3.54 6.99
Time to max VAS, sec 149 0 120 53 28 88 60 42.99 57.11 35.15 2.88 5.69
  1. Summary statistics of raw data: Cold-pressor test pain intensity over time. q1=first (25%) quartile, q3=third (75%) quartile, IQR, inter-quartile range; MAD, median absolute deviation; CPT, cold-pressor test; AUC, area under the curve.

CPM and TS effects

Two-way repeated measures ANOVA (Pillai statistic) indicated statistically highly significant effects of both TS, CPM and their interaction. However, as described in the Method section, the underlying assumptions of ANOVA were not fulfilled.

Unpaired, single-sample Wilcoxon rank-sum test (mu=0) support significant effects of both Temporal Summation and Conditioned Pain Modulation. See Table 4.

Table 4:

Effects of CPM and TS.

Effect …on n min Max Median q1 q3 iqr MAD Mean SD SE CI p-Value
CPM effect Single stim. 149 −74 29 −1 −11 0 11 7.41 −5.24 13.75 1.13 2.23 p<0.0001
Repeated stim. 149 −50 21 −9 −20 0 20 13.34 −10.99 14.64 1.20 2.37 p<0.0001
TS effect 149 −51 62 −4 −16 3 19 14.83 −5.75 16.73 1.37 2.71 p<0.0001
TS effect After CPT 149 −28 78 9 0 19 19 13.34 11.67 15.38 1.26 2.49 p<0.0001
Before CPT 149 −43 77 15 5 28 23 16.31 17.42 17.71 1.45 2.87 p<0.0001
  1. Summary statistics of the effects of conditioned pain modulation (CPM) and temporal summation (TS) on experimental pressure pain intensity (VAS). q1=first (25%) quartile, q3=third (75%) quartile, IQR, inter-quartile range; MAD, median absolute deviation; CPT, cold-pressor test. p-Value from one-sample Wilcoxon test (μ=0).

Unpaired, single-sample Wilcoxon rank-sum test (mu=0) support significant effects of both Temporal Summation and Conditioned Pain Modulation. See Table 4. Significant differences in the CPM effect was observed when comparing single and repeated stimuli (Paired Wilcoxon test, p<0.0001) and when comparing repeated stimuli to temporal summation (Paired Wilcoxon test, p<0.0001), but not when comparing single stimulus to temporal summation (Paired Wilcoxon test, p≥0.05). Similarly, a significant difference in the effect of temporal summation was observed before/after CPM (Paired Wilcoxon test, p<0.0001).

Figure 3 illustrates the distribution of the CPM effect for both single and repeated (TS) stimulation. As evident from Table 4, a significant CPM effect was observed for both test stimuli.

Figure 3: 
            Distribution density plot (smoothed histogram) of the Conditioned Pain Modulation effect on single/repeat mechanical pressure stimulation, i.e. the individual change in pain before/after CPM. Vertical lines represent mean values.
Figure 3:

Distribution density plot (smoothed histogram) of the Conditioned Pain Modulation effect on single/repeat mechanical pressure stimulation, i.e. the individual change in pain before/after CPM. Vertical lines represent mean values.

Similarly, Figure 4 illustrates the distribution for the TS effect on pressure pain before and after CPM. As evident from Table 4, a significant TS effect was observed at both points in time.

Figure 4: 
            Distribution density plot (smoothed histogram) of the Temporal Summation effect before/after cold-pressor test, i.e. the individual difference in pain with single versus repeated pressure. Vertical lines represent mean values.
Figure 4:

Distribution density plot (smoothed histogram) of the Temporal Summation effect before/after cold-pressor test, i.e. the individual difference in pain with single versus repeated pressure. Vertical lines represent mean values.

The effect of Temporal Summation was found to be significantly attenuated after CPT (paired Wilcoxon test, p<0.0001), with the mean TS effect dropping from 17.42 δVAS to 11.67 δVAS.

Effect size

The mean CPM effect of cold-pressor pain on single pressure pain was −5.24 (VAS) (Table 4), corresponding to −29% of mean single pressure pain intensity before CPT (Table 2).

Similarly, the mean CPM effect of cold-pressor pain on repeated pressure pain was −10.99 (VAS) (Table 4), corresponding to −31% of mean repeated pressure pain intensity before CPT (Table 2).

Lastly, the mean CPM effect of cold-pressor pain on TS was −5.75 (VAS) (Table 4), corresponding to −33% of mean TS effect before CPT (Table 4).

CPM responders/non-responders

Using the simple CPM responder criteria, the frequency (number) of CPM responders/non-responders was observed to be 75/74 for Single stimuli, 103/46 for Repeat stimuli, and 84/65 for Temporal summation respectively. For the strict CPM responder criteria the frequencies were 137/12, 142/7, and 124/25 respectively, and for the reverse CPM responder criteria 40/109, 64/85, and 55/94.

Group (CPM responder vs. non-responder) differences in clinical pain are presented in Table 5, subdivided into each of the three non-responder criteria (simple, strict, reversed) and each test stimulus (single, repeated and temporal summation). Significant differences were observed in only six of the 45 tests.

Table 5:

CPM responder status and clinical data.

Clinical pain Test stim. Responder criteria CPM non-responder n Median p-Value
Current pain, VAS Single stim. Simple TRUE 72 5.0
FALSE 73 6.0 0.40
Strict TRUE 12 5.0
FALSE 133 5.0 0.91
Reversed TRUE 106 5.0
FALSE 39 6.0 0.22
Repeated stim. Simple TRUE 44 4.5
FALSE 101 6.0 0.03*
Strict TRUE 7 4.0
FALSE 138 5.0 0.54
Reversed TRUE 82 5.0
FALSE 63 6.0 0.05*
TS effect Simple TRUE 62 5.0
FALSE 83 6.0 0.58
Strict TRUE 24 5.0
FALSE 121 6.0 0.69
Reversed TRUE 91 5.0
FALSE 54 5.0 0.54
Typical pain, VAS Single stim. Simple TRUE 70 7.0
FALSE 72 6.5 1.00
Strict TRUE 12 7.0
FALSE 130 6.5 0.85
Reversed TRUE 103 6.0
FALSE 39 7.0 0.40
Repeated stim. Simple TRUE 44 6.0
FALSE 98 7.0 0.03*
Strict TRUE 7 6.0
FALSE 135 7.0 0.78
Reversed TRUE 80 6.0
FALSE 62 7.0 0.03*
TS effect Simple TRUE 62 7.0
FALSE 80 6.5 0.40
Strict TRUE 24 6.5
FALSE 118 7.0 0.67
Reversed TRUE 89 6.0
FALSE 53 7.0 0.51
Worst pain, VAS Single stim. Simple TRUE 72 8.0
FALSE 73 8.0 0.86
Strict TRUE 12 8.0
FALSE 133 8.0 0.68
Reversed TRUE 106 8.0
FALSE 39 8.0 0.59
Repeated stim. Simple TRUE 44 7.5
FALSE 101 8.0 0.01**
Strict TRUE 7 8.0
FALSE 138 8.0 0.37
Reversed TRUE 82 8.0
FALSE 63 8.0 0.01**
TS effect Simple TRUE 62 8.0
FALSE 83 8.0 0.08
Strict TRUE 24 8.0
FALSE 121 8.0 0.31
Reversed TRUE 91 8.0
FALSE 54 8.0 0.33
Pain duration, days Single stim. Simple TRUE 72 365.0
FALSE 72 336.0 0.82
Strict TRUE 12 265.0
FALSE 132 365.0 0.81
Reversed TRUE 106 380.0
FALSE 38 259.0 0.30
Repeated stim. Simple TRUE 44 396.5
FALSE 100 349.5 0.93
Strict TRUE 7 153.0
FALSE 137 365.0 0.38
Reversed TRUE 82 321.0
FALSE 62 380.0 0.58
TS effect Simple TRUE 62 259.5
FALSE 82 380.0 0.29
Strict TRUE 24 198.0
FALSE 120 380.0 0.15
Reversed TRUE 91 335.0
FALSE 53 394.0 0.24
Pain drawing, pixels Single stim. Simple TRUE 74 1,654.0
FALSE 75 1,399.0 0.23
Strict TRUE 12 2038.5
FALSE 137 1,430.0 0.15
Reversed TRUE 109 1,430.0
FALSE 40 1,462.0 0.80
Repeated stim. Simple TRUE 46 1754.0
FALSE 103 1,430.0 0.76
Strict TRUE 7 2,533.0
FALSE 142 1,393.5 0.10
Reversed TRUE 85 1,491.0
FALSE 64 1,393.5 0.80
TS effect Simple TRUE 65 1,433.0
FALSE 84 1,456.5 0.76
Strict TRUE 25 1,433.0
FALSE 124 1,456.5 0.58
Reversed TRUE 94 1,487.0
FALSE 55 1,328.0 0.97

Discussion

In this large group of low-back pain patients, we found highly significant inhibitory effects of CPM on single- and repeated pressure stimuli, and on the temporal summation of pressure stimuli.

We hypothesized that the sensitivity of TS to the inhibitory effect of CPM would be different from that of a simple phasic test stimuli, but the relative effect size of CPM was comparable between all three test stimuli (single pressure pain, repeated pressure pain and temporal summation of pressure pain).

By contrast, in absolute values a more pronounced CPM effect was seen with repeated pressure pain as compared to both a single test stimulus and TS of pressure pain. This adds to the controversies about how CPM should be assessed and presented.

Conditioned pain modulation of temporal summation of pain

Two previous publications about the CPM effect on TS [11], [12] were not in agreement. Holden et al. [11] reported no significant differences in TS, whereas Sirucek et al. [12] found a significant CPM induced inhibition of TS. The current data align with the findings of Sirucek et al.

Both Holden et al. and Sirucek et al. applied the conditioning and testing stimuli at the same segmental levels of spinal innervation, albeit on contra-lateral sides. By contrast, the current study applied the test and conditioning stimuli at different segments of spinal innervation. Applying the test and conditioning stimuli at the same segmental level could obfuscate any diffuse descending inhibitory effect of the conditioning stimulus: There is evidence that nociceptive input can lead to contralateral sensitization on the same segment [17] and it is thus conceivable that the intense pain of the conditioning stimulus could interfere with the integration of nociceptive input from the test stimulus and the descending diffuse noxious inhibitory control from higher centers.

The two previous and the current study are different in other ways, including the QST methods employed. As Kennedy et al. [6] observed CPM is sensitive to differences in test method and this could also account for the differences observed between the three studies.

Earlier publications have examined the effects of CPM on second pain elicited by electrical or heat stimulation on healthy individuals [18] and fibromyalgia patients [19]. These authors reported greater inhibitory CPM effect on second pain than first pain, dependent on clinical status and sex. There may be an element of slow onset secondary pain to temporal pain summation and vice versa, but the test methods are not directly comparable and the findings should be compared with care.

All-in-all, there is some evidence to suggest that perturbations in CPM is a feature of chronic low-back, but CPM methodology is important and the literature is not in agreement [2].

Clinical considerations

Several other publications have used TS and CPM as indicators of perturbed central pain modulation in chronic clinical pain states – see e.g. O’Brien et al. [20] and den Bandt et al. [2] for reviews on fibromyalgia and low-back pain patients respectively. To our knowledge the effect of CPM on TS of pressure pain in a clinical population has not been reported before.

The current data is based exclusively on low-back pain patients seen in a specialized hospital spine center. It is therefore not possible to say whether the effects of CPM and TS differ from healthy individuals or other clinical populations.

In any clinical context however, the relevance of a CPM effect is likely to be in the distinction between individuals as CPM responders or CPM non-responders [21]. A CPM non-response could reflect an attenuated diffuse, descending noxious inhibitory control. This in turn is probably important for development of widespread hyperalgesia and chronification of pain [22].

On the face of it, the results presented in Table 5 suggested that significant differences in clinical presentation were observed between CPM responders and non-responders. However, on closer examination this must be questioned: The table included forty-five p-values, which suggests that it would be appropriate to correct for multiple statistical comparisons. Such correction methods (e.g. Bonferroni correction) generally rely on stricter significance criteria, and as the six significant p-values in Table 5 ranged from 0.01 to 0.05, they would likely be concluded insignificant using any correction method. Also, the three clinical measures in question were current paintypical pain and worst pain which are not independent, but correlated (post-hoc Pearson correlation coefficient=[0.75; 0.9]).

Regarding the best way to dichotomize CPM responses into responders and non-responders, the three criteria resulted in very different frequencies. With the strict criteria, very few patients (n=7–25 – see Results subsection CPM responders/non-responders) were categorized as non-responders. Conversely, with the reverse criteria more than half (n=85–109) were categorized as non-responders. As the current study did not include a healthy control group, we can not determine the CPM non-responder criteria with the best discriminatory ability. However, a previous study of healthy controls can be cautiously contrasted with the present study. Locke et al. [23] also used CPT to induce inhibitory CPM responses, but used pressure pain thresholds as the test stimuli (+5% higher thresholds corresponding to an inhibitory CPM effect) and was focused on identifying CPM responders. In other words, Locke et al. considered CPM non-responders to be those participants with either a small or no inhibitory CPM response, which would correspond to our reverse criteria. In contrast to our LBP population, Locke et al. found that only 7% of healthy controls were CPM non-responders by such reverse criteria. Even with the laxer simple criteria a considerably higher proportion of LBP patients (n=46–74) were categorized as CPM non-responders in our population.

No differences in reported LBP duration were found between CPM responders and non-responders, irrespective of the criteria. Additionally, Gerhardt et al. [5] suggested that altered CPM might drive pain extent (area), but we found no difference in pain extent between CPM responders and non-responders in the current data. At first look, this challenges the impact of pain duration on development of perturbed central pain modulation, but it should be recalled that the current study population consisted exclusively of chronic low-back pain patients in whom such perturbed modulation may be common.

Thus, no difference in clinical pain intensity, duration or extent between CPM responders and non-responders should be inferred from these data. We reiterate however, that the present data are from a select subgroup of chronic LBP patients. We can not rule out, that a comparison with healthy controls or acute LBP patients would demonstrate significant differences.

Restrictions imposed by a VAS

The CPM effect was most pronounced in relation to the pain induced by repeated pressure stimuli. This could possibly reflect an undesired characteristic of the Visual Analogue Scale or methodological caveat: As the VAS is fixed at both ends, there is a risk of flooring and ceiling effects which may limit the usefulness of standardized QST tests [24]. If the test stimulus pain intensity prior to CPM is low, there is little remaining room on the VAS to indicate even lower scores following CPM. As the pain induced by repeated pressure was about double that of a single stimulus, there was more room available for a drop in pain scores following CPM. The similarity in relative effect size of CPM on single and repeated pressure stimuli could be interpreted as reflecting such a flooring effect. Thus, in CPM testing care should be taken to ensure that the intensity of the test stimuli is sufficiently high to avoid a flooring effect and ensuing type B error.

Strengths and weaknesses

Clinical pain status was recorded at a single point in time prior to QST testing and we can say nothing about the temporal development of clinical pain in relation to CPM responder/non-responder status.

As discussed above, only chronic LBP patients were included and whilst significant differences in CPM effect have been reported across different chronic pain diagnoses [25], important differences within pain patient groups have also been reported [5, 26, 27]. The current results should not be uncritically extrapolated to other populations and we can not draw conclusions regarding any potential differences between our study population and healthy individuals.

It is known that attention and expectations [28], [29] may influence the CPM response. No attempt was made to control or measure attention in the current study, but conversely a strict and uniform testing protocol was followed throughout.

Conclusions

A significant CPM effect of cold-pressor test was observed on single pressure pain, repeated pressure pain and temporal summation of pressure pain alike and no apparent difference in relative effect sizes were observed.

A more pronounced CPM effect in absolute terms was observed with repeated mechanical pressure as the test stimulus, possibly as a result of less flooring effect compared to single stimuli and temporal summation.

Categorization of participants as CPM responders or non-responders was not associated with significant group differences in clinical pain measures.


Corresponding author: Søren O’Neill, Spine Center of Southern Denmark, University Hospital of Southern Denmark, Rygcenter Syddanmark, Rygmedicinsk Forskning, Middelfart Sygehus, Østre Hougvej 55, 5500 Middelfart, Denmark; and Institute of Regional Health Research, University of Southern Denmark, Odense M, Denmark, Phone: +45 40430817, E-mail:

Funding source: Danish National Research Foundation

Award Identifier / Grant number: DNRF121

Funding source: Danish Foundation for the Advancement of Chiropractic Research and Post-Graduate Education

  1. Research funding: Center for Neuroplasticity and Pain (CNAP) is supported by the Danish National Research Foundation (DNRF121). Liam Holm was funded by a research grant from the Danish Foundation for the Advancement of Chiropractic Research and Post-Graduate Education.

  2. Author contributions: All authors have accepted responsibility for the entire content of this manuscript and approved its submission.

  3. Competing interests: We, the authors state no conflicts of interest in the preparation of this manuscript.

  4. Informed consent: Informed consent has been obtained from all individuals included in this study.

  5. Ethical approval: The study was approved by the Regional Committee on Health Research Ethics, Denmark (ref. number S-20180098).

Appendix A

Diagnoses

Frequency table of clinical diagnoses and sub-diagnoses.

ICD10 n
M54.5 46
M62.6 46
M54.4 27
M51.1 22
M47.8 21
M48.0 18
M47.2 14
M99.8 10
M43.1 9
M79.6 8
M99.6 8
M99.9 6
M16.9 5
M54.9 5
M99.7 4
M54.1 3
M47. 2
M51.2 2
M51.3 2
M53.1 2
M53.9 2
M54.2 2
M54.6 2
M54.8 2
M70.6 2
M99.4 2
M23.9 1
M40.0 1
M46.9 1
M47.9 1
M48.1 1
M53.3 1
M99.0 1

References

1. Yarnitsky, D. Conditioned pain modulation (the diffuse noxious inhibitory control-like effect): its relevance for acute and chronic pain states. Curr Opin Anaesthesiol 2010;23:611–5. https://doi.org/10.1097/ACO.0b013e32833c348b.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

2. den Bandt, HL, Paulis, WD, Beckwée, D, Ickmans, K, Nijs, J, Voogt, L. Pain mechanisms in low back pain: a systematic review with meta-analysis of mechanical quantitative sensory testing outcomes in people with nonspecific low back pain. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther 2019;49:698–715. https://doi.org/10.2519/jospt.2019.8876.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

3. Corrêa, JB, Costa, LOP, de Oliveira, NTB, Sluka, KA, Liebano, RE. Central sensitization and changes in conditioned pain modulation in people with chronic nonspecific low back pain: a case control study. Exp Brain Res 2015;233:2391–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4309-6.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

4. Rabey, M, Slater, H, O’Sullivan, P, Beales, D, Smith, A. Somatosensory nociceptive characteristics differentiate subgroups in people with chronic low back pain: a cluster analysis. Pain 2015;156:1874–84. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000244.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

5. Gerhardt, A, Eich, W, Treede, R-D, Tesarz, J. Conditioned pain modulation in patients with nonspecific chronic back pain with chronic local pain, chronic widespread pain, and fibromyalgia. Pain 2017;158:430–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000777.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

6. Kennedy, DL, Kemp, HI, Ridout, D, Yarnitsky, D, Rice, ASC. Reliability of conditioned pain modulation: a systematic review. Pain 2016;157:2410–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000689.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

7. Martel, MO, Wasan, AD, Edwards, RR. Sex differences in the stability of conditioned pain modulation (CPM) among patients with chronic pain. Pain Med 2013;14:1757–68. https://doi.org/10.1111/pme.12220.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

8. Hestbaek, L, Leboeuf-Yde, C, Manniche, C. Is low back pain part of a general health pattern or is it a separate and distinctive entity? A critical literature review of comorbidity with low back pain. J Manip Physiol Ther 2003;26:243–52.10.1016/S0161-4754(03)00003-4Search in Google Scholar PubMed

9. Herrero, JF, Laird, JMA, Lopez-Garcia, JA. Wind-up of spinal cord neurones and pain sensation: much ado about something? Prog Neurobiol 2000;61:169–203. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0082(99)00051-9.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

10. Nahman-Averbuch, H, Martucci, KT, Granovsky, Y, Weissman-Fogel, I, Yarnitsky, D, Coghill, RC. Distinct brain mechanisms support spatial vs. temporal filtering of nociceptive information. Pain 2014;155:2491–501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2014.07.008.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

11. Holden, S, Petersen, KK, Arendt-Nielsen, L, Graven-Nielsen, T. Conditioning pain modulation reduces pain only during the first stimulation of the temporal summation of pain paradigm in healthy participants. Eur J Pain 2019;23:1390–6. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejp.1408.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

12. Sirucek, L, Jutzeler, CR, Rosner, J, Schweinhardt, P, Curt, A, Kramer, JLK, et al.. The effect of conditioned pain modulation on tonic heat pain assessed using participant-controlled temperature. Pain Med 2020;21:2839–49. https://doi.org/10.1093/pm/pnaa041.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

13. Kent, P, Kongsted, A, Jensen, TS, Albert, HB, Schiøttz-Christensen, B, Manniche, C. SpineData a Danish clinical registry of people with chronic back pain. Clin Epidemiol 2015;7:369–80. https://doi.org/10.2147/CLEP.S83830.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

14. O’Neill, S, Jensen, TS, Kent, P. Computerized quantification of pain drawings. Scand J Pain 2019;20:175–89. https://doi.org/10.1515/sjpain-2019-0082.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

15. Harris, PA, Taylor, R, Thielke, R, Payne, J, Gonzalez, N, Conde, JG. Research electronic data capture (REDCap) a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inf 2009;42:377–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbi.2008.08.010.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

16. Harris, PA, Taylor, R, Minor, BL, Elliott, V, Fernandez, M, O’Neal, L, et al.. The REDCap consortium: building an international community of software platform partners. J Biomed Inf 2019;95:103208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbi.2019.103208.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

17. Shenker, NG, Haigh, RC, Mapp, PI, Harris, N, Blake, DR. Contralateral hyperalgesia and allodynia following intradermal capsaicin injection in man. Rheumatology 2008;47:1417–21. https://doi.org/10.1093/rheumatology/ken251.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

18. Price, DD, McHaffie, JG. Effects of heterotopic conditioning stimuli on first and second pain: a psychophysical evaluation in humans. Pain 1988;34:245–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(88)90119-4.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

19. Staud, R, Robinson, ME, Vierck, CJ, Price, DD. Diffuse noxious inhibitory controls (DNIC) attenuate temporal summation of second pain in normal males but not in normal females or fibromyalgia patients. Pain 2003;101:167–74.10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00325-1Search in Google Scholar PubMed

20. O’Brien, AT, Deitos, A, Triñanes Pego, Y, Fregni, F, Carrillo-de-la-Peña, MT. Defective endogenous pain modulation in fibromyalgia: a meta-analysis of temporal summation and conditioned pain modulation paradigms. J Pain 2018;19:819–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2018.01.010.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

21. Arendt-Nielsen, L, Larsen, JB, Rasmussen, KS, Borg, L, Madeleine, P. A novel clinical applicable bed-side tool for assessing conditioning pain modulation: proof-of-concept. Scand J Pain 2020;20:801–7.10.1515/sjpain-2020-0033Search in Google Scholar PubMed

22. Graven-Nielsen, T, Arendt-Nielsen, L. Assessment of mechanisms in localized and widespread musculoskeletal pain. Nat Rev Rheumatol 2010;6:599–606.10.1038/nrrheum.2010.107Search in Google Scholar PubMed

23. Locke, D, Gibson, W, Moss, P, Munyard, K, Mamotte, C, Wright, A. Analysis of meaningful conditioned pain modulation effect in a pain-free adult population. J Pain 2014;15:1190–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2014.09.001.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

24. Mackey, IG, Dixon, EA, Johnson, K, Kong, J-T. Dynamic quantitative sensory testing to characterize central pain processing. J Vis Exp 2017;16:54452. https://doi.org/10.3791/54452.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

25. Lewis, GN, Rice, DA, McNair, PJ. Conditioned pain modulation in populations with chronic pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Pain 2012;13:936–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2012.07.005.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

26. Blumenstiel, K, Gerhardt, A, Rolke, R, Bieber, C, Tesarz, J, Friederich, H-C, et al.. Quantitative sensory testing profiles in chronic back pain are distinct from those in fibromyalgia. Clin J Pain 2011;27:682–90. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0b013e3182177654.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

27. Gerhardt, A, Eich, W, Janke, S, Leisner, S, Treede, R-D, Tesarz, J. Chronic widespread back pain is distinct from chronic local back pain: evidence from quantitative sensory testing, pain drawings, and psychometrics. Clin J Pain 2016;32:568–79. https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.0000000000000300.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

28. Bjørkedal, E, Flaten, MA. Expectations of increased and decreased pain explain the effect of conditioned pain modulation in females. J Pain Res 2012;5:289–300. https://doi.org/10.2147/JPR.S33559.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

29. Nir, R-R, Yarnitsky, D, Honigman, L, Granot, M. Cognitive manipulation targeted at decreasing the conditioning pain perception reduces the efficacy of conditioned pain modulation. Pain 2012;153:170–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2011.10.010.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Received: 2021-02-01
Accepted: 2021-04-22
Published Online: 2021-05-27
Published in Print: 2021-07-27

© 2021 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Articles in the same Issue

  1. Frontmatter
  2. Editorial Comment
  3. Some controversies related to questionable clinical uses of methadone for chronic non-cancer pain and in palliative care
  4. Systematic Reviews
  5. Heart rate variability in patients with low back pain: a systematic review
  6. Prevalence of musculoskeletal chest pain in the emergency department: a systematic review and meta-analysis
  7. Clinical Pain Research
  8. When driving hurts: characterizing the experience and impact of driving with back pain
  9. Sensitization in office workers with chronic neck pain in different pain conditions and intensities
  10. Do chronic low back pain subgroups derived from dynamic quantitative sensory testing exhibit differing multidimensional profiles?
  11. Do people with acute low back pain have an attentional bias to threat-related words?
  12. Effects of dynamic stabilization exercises and muscle energy technique on selected biopsychosocial outcomes for patients with chronic non-specific low back pain: a double-blind randomized controlled trial
  13. Health-related quality of life and pain interference in two patient cohorts with neuropathic pain: breast cancer survivors and HIV patients
  14. Breast reconstruction after breast cancer surgery – persistent pain and quality of life 1–8 years after breast reconstruction
  15. Autonomic dysregulation and impairments in the recognition of facial emotional expressions in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain
  16. Sleep disturbance in patients attending specialized chronic pain clinics in Denmark: a longitudinal study examining the relationship between sleep and pain outcomes
  17. The challenge of recognizing severe pain and autonomic abnormalities for early diagnosis of CRPS
  18. Military veterans with and without post-traumatic stress disorder: results from a chronic pain management programme
  19. Observational Studies
  20. Long-term postoperative opioid prescription after cholecystectomy or gastric by-pass surgery: a retrospective observational study
  21. Criterion validity and discriminatory ability of the central sensitization inventory short form in individuals with inflammatory bowel diseases
  22. Neural activity during cognitive reappraisal in chronic low back pain: a preliminary study
  23. Social deprivation and paediatric chronic pain referrals in Ireland: a cross-sectional study
  24. Original Experimental
  25. The inhibitory effect of conditioned pain modulation on temporal summation in low-back pain patients
  26. “Big girls don’t cry”: the effect of the experimenter’s sex and pain catastrophising on pain
  27. Short Communication
  28. Persistent pain relief following a single injection of a local anesthetic for neuropathic abdominal wall and groin pain
Downloaded on 8.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/sjpain-2021-0025/html
Scroll to top button