Abstract
Our study investigates the nature of tonogenesis in Sylheti, which was triggered by the merger of aspirated and unaspirated consonants in the language. We propose that the tonogenetic factors contributed by the four-way laryngeal contrast condition a three-way tonal contrast in Sylheti depending on the voicing and syllabic position of the sound. We take into consideration both monosyllabic and disyllabic sets of words and claim that Sylheti has three lexical level tones: high, mid, and low. We have built linear mixed-effect models of f0 and duration to examine the acoustic correlates of tone contrast in the language. We conclude that Sylheti has a three-way tonal contrast distinctively affecting pitch.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank all seven speakers of Sylheti for participating in eliciting the tone data and for providing the speech data with patience.
-
Ethics approval and consent to participate: The first author belongs to the speech community and informal consent was available from the speakers.
Appendix A: Fixed effects of normalized f0 interval contrast between the three tones for monosyllables
Interval | Fixed effects | Estimate | Standard error | t | χ 2 | df | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Interval 10 | Intercept | 0.40757 | 0.19691 | 2.07 | |||
High tone | 0.01086 | 0.35566 | 0.031 | 5.0669 | 1 | 0.024387 | |
Low tone | −0.89706 | 0.22944 | −3.91 | ||||
Interval 20 | Intercept | 0.38775 | 0.19404 | 1.998 | |||
High tone | 0.09456 | 0.3434 | 0.275 | 11.876 | 1 | 0.000569 | |
Low tone | −1.04429 | 0.23202 | −4.501 | ||||
Interval 30 | Intercept | 0.415 | 0.1876 | 2.212 | |||
High tone | 0.2025 | 0.3461 | 0.585 | 16.826 | 1 | 0.000041 | |
Low tone | −1.2038 | 0.2202 | −5.467 | ||||
Interval 40 | Intercept | 0.4396 | 0.1766 | 2.49 | |||
High tone | 0.1603 | 0.3394 | 0.472 | 18.082 | 1 | 0.000021 | |
Low tone | −1.2879 | 0.2101 | −6.131 | ||||
Interval 50 | Intercept | 0.4575 | 0.1707 | 2.68 | |||
High tone | 0.1601 | 0.3364 | 0.476 | 16.575 | 1 | 0.000047 | |
Low tone | −1.3145 | 0.2131 | −6.169 | ||||
Interval 60 | Intercept | 0.4941 | 0.1695 | 2.915 | |||
High tone | 0.1372 | 0.3275 | 0.419 | 16.197 | 1 | 0.000057 | |
Low tone | −1.3331 | 0.2165 | −6.158 | ||||
Interval 70 | Intercept | 0.51836 | 0.17096 | 3.032 | |||
High tone | 0.09841 | 0.33019 | 0.298 | 15.835 | 1 | 0.000069 | |
Low tone | −1.30772 | 0.22225 | −5.884 | ||||
Interval 80 | Intercept | 0.52222 | 0.16375 | 3.189 | |||
High tone | 0.03617 | 0.32985 | 0.11 | 26.894 | 1 | <0.00001 | |
Low tone | −1.24067 | 0.22179 | −5.594 | ||||
Interval 90 | Intercept | 0.46058 | 0.17648 | 2.61 | |||
High tone | 0.07038 | 0.34554 | 0.204 | 14.405 | 1 | 0.000147 | |
Low tone | −1.11528 | 0.22443 | −4.969 | ||||
Interval 100 | Intercept | 0.3945 | 0.163 | 2.421 | |||
High tone | 0.2021 | 0.3497 | 0.578 | 23.98 | 1 | <0.00001 | |
Low tone | −1.0319 | 0.2118 | −4.871 |
Appendix B: Fixed effects of normalized f0 interval contrast between the three tones for disyllables
Fixed effects of normalized f0 interval contrast between the three tones for the first syllable of disyllables.
Interval | Fixed effects | Estimate | Standard error | t | χ 2 | df | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Interval 10 | Intercept | 0.54561 | 0.07325 | 7.449 | |||
High tone | 0.90458 | 0.20602 | 4.391 | 17.014 | 1 | 0.000037 | |
Low tone | −0.85158 | 0.15796 | −5.391 | ||||
Interval 20 | Intercept | 0.40182 | 0.06505 | 6.177 | |||
High tone | 0.95797 | 0.20512 | 4.670 | 17.497 | 1 | 0.000029 | |
Low tone | −0.81449 | 0.14983 | −5.436 | ||||
Interval 30 | Intercept | 0.30088 | 0.06288 | 4.785 | |||
High tone | 0.95865 | 0.20636 | 4.645 | 8.6051 | 1 | 0.003352 | |
Low tone | −0.81680 | 0.14902 | −5.481 | ||||
Interval 40 | Intercept | 0.23875 | 0.06205 | 3.848 | |||
High tone | 0.95423 | 0.21004 | 4.543 | 18.559 | 1 | 0.000016 | |
Low tone | −0.84587 | 0.14985 | −5.645 | ||||
Interval 50 | Intercept | 0.17978 | 0.06356 | 2.828 | |||
High tone | 0.95850 | 0.20112 | 4.766 | 18.595 | 1 | 0.000016 | |
Low tone | −0.87533 | 0.14939 | −5.859 | ||||
Interval 60 | Intercept | 0.11750 | 0.06446 | 1.823 | |||
High tone | 0.93856 | 0.18887 | 4.969 | 19.215 | 1 | 0.000012 | |
Low tone | −0.89898 | 0.14715 | −6.109 | ||||
Interval 70 | Intercept | 0.05503 | 0.06590 | 0.835 | |||
High tone | 0.90413 | 0.17839 | 5.068 | 19.273 | 1 | 0.000011 | |
Low tone | −0.92003 | 0.14622 | −6.292 | ||||
Interval 80 | Intercept | −0.00597 | 0.07034 | −0.085 | |||
High tone | 0.96409 | 0.17660 | 5.459 | 26.894 | 1 | <0.00001 | |
Low tone | −0.94118 | 0.14017 | −6.715 | ||||
Interval 90 | Intercept | −0.04224 | 0.07147 | −0.591 | |||
High tone | 0.82451 | 0.20964 | 3.933 | 20.941 | 1 | <0.00001 | |
Low tone | −0.97899 | 0.12821 | −7.636 | ||||
Interval 100 | Intercept | −0.05345 | 0.07053 | −0.758 | |||
High tone | 0.83607 | 0.20896 | 4.001 | 22.627 | 1 | <0.00001 | |
Low tone | −1.03045 | 0.11852 | −8.694 |
Fixed effects of normalized f0 interval contrast between the three tones for the second syllable of disyllables.
Interval | Fixed effects | Estimate | Standard error | t | χ 2 | df | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Interval 10 | Intercept | 0.38626 | 0.07964 | 4.850 | |||
High tone | 0.98351 | 0.23944 | 4.108 | 27.416 | 1 | 0.000037 | |
Low tone | −1.36955 | 0.13871 | −9.874 | ||||
Interval 20 | Intercept | 0.38572 | 0.06502 | 5.933 | |||
High tone | 0.88610 | 0.20690 | 4.283 | 26.152 | 1 | 0.000029 | |
Low tone | −1.35932 | 0.13982 | −9.722 | ||||
Interval 30 | Intercept | 0.3913 | 0.0524 | 7.468 | |||
High tone | 0.7848 | 0.1657 | 4.737 | 25.338 | 1 | 0.003352 | |
Low tone | −1.3534 | 0.1425 | −9.495 | ||||
Interval 40 | Intercept | 0.39452 | 0.04742 | 7.737 | |||
High tone | 0.63345 | 0.14848 | 3.276 | 23.934 | 1 | 0.000016 | |
Low tone | −1.34360 | 0.14642 | −8.896 | ||||
Interval 50 | Intercept | 0.40367 | 0.05217 | 2.828 | |||
High tone | 0.45290 | 0.13827 | 4.766 | 23.738 | 1 | 0.000016 | |
Low tone | −1.33896 | 0.15051 | −5.859 | ||||
Interval 60 | Intercept | 0.41503 | 0.05278 | 7.864 | |||
High tone | 0.27450 | 0.13695 | 2.004 | 23.288 | 1 | 0.000012 | |
Low tone | −1.31613 | 0.15376 | −8.560 | ||||
Interval 70 | Intercept | 0.41452 | 0.05511 | 7.522 | |||
High tone | 0.11043 | 0.1418 | 0.779 | 21.773 | 1 | 0.000011 | |
Low tone | −1.25830 | 0.1507 | −8.352 | ||||
Interval 80 | Intercept | 0.26509 | 0.0861 | 3.079 | |||
High tone | 0.11837 | 0.0979 | 1.209 | 18.334 | 1 | <0.00001 | |
Low tone | −1.06568 | 0.1644 | −6.482 | ||||
Interval 90 | Intercept | 0.1207 | 0.1558 | 0.775 | |||
High tone | 0.1794 | 0.1313 | 1.367 | 12.312 | 1 | <0.00001 | |
Low tone | −0.8993 | 0.2029 | −4.431 | ||||
Interval 100 | Intercept | −0.08418 | 0.1837 | −0.458 | |||
High tone | 0.23961 | 0.1368 | 1.752 | 9.8738 | 1 | <0.00001 | |
Low tone | −0.73748 | 0.2021 | −3.649 |
Appendix C: Predictions for the speaker-wise slope of the effect of tone on pitch

Speaker-wise effect of tone on f0 in disyllables.

Speaker-wise effect of tone on f0 in monosyllables.
References
Bahri, Hardev. 1989. Learners’ Hindi-English dictionary = Siksarthi Hindi-Angrejhi sabdakosa. Delhi: Rajapala.Search in Google Scholar
Bates, Douglas, Martin Maechler, Ben Bolker & Steve Walker. 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67. 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01.Search in Google Scholar
Ben-Akiva, Moshe & Joffre Swait. 1986. The Akaike likelihood ratio index. Transportation Science 20. 133–136. https://doi.org/10.1287/trsc.20.2.133.Search in Google Scholar
Bhattacarya, Subhash. 2003. Samsada Bangala uccarana abhidhana, 2nd edn. Kalikata: Sahitya Samsad.Search in Google Scholar
Chalmers, Rod & Noorul Miah. 1996. Learning Sylheti. London: Centre for Bangladeshi Studies.Search in Google Scholar
Chatterji, Suniti Kumar. 1926. The origin and development of the Bengali language. Calcutta: Calcutta University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Cho, Taehong, Douglas Whalen & Gerad Docherty. 2019. Voice onset time and beyond: Exploring laryngeal contrast in 19 languages. Journal of Phonetics 72. 52–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2018.11.002.Search in Google Scholar
Dixit, R. Prakash. 1989. Glottal gestures in Hindi plosives. Journal of Phonetics 17. 213–237. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0095-4470(19)30431-0.Search in Google Scholar
Dutta, Indranil. 2007. Four-way stop contrasts in Hindi: An acoustic study of voicing, fundamental frequency, and spectral tilt. Urbana: University of Illinois PhD dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Evans, Jonathan, Wen-Chi Yeh & Rukmini Kulkarni. 2018. Acoustics of tone in Indian Punjabi. Transactions of the Philological Society 116(3). 509–528. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-968x.12135.Search in Google Scholar
Faquire, Razaul Karim. 2012. On the classification of varieties of Bangla spoken in Bangladesh. BUP Journal 1(1). 130–139.Search in Google Scholar
Gill, Harjeet Singh & Henry Gleason. 1969. A reference grammar of Punjabi. Patiala: Punjabi University.Search in Google Scholar
Gope, Amalesh. 2016. The phonetics and phonology of Sylheti tonogenesis. Guwahati: IIT Guwahati PhD thesis.Search in Google Scholar
Gope, Amalesh & Shakuntala Mahanta. 2014. Lexical tones in Sylheti. In Paper presented at the 4th international symposium on tonal aspects of languages, Nijmegen, 13–16 May.Search in Google Scholar
Gope, Amalesh & Shakuntala Mahanta. 2015. An acoustic analysis of Sylheti phonemes. In the scottish consortium for ICPhS 2015 (ed.). In Proceedings of the 18th international congress of phonetic sciences. Glasgow: University of Glasgow. Available at: https://www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2015/Papers/ICPHS0630.pdf.Search in Google Scholar
Gope, Amalesh & Shakuntala Mahanta. 2016. Correlation between Sylheti tone and phonation. In Proceedings of speech prosody 2016, 307–311.10.21437/SpeechProsody.2016-63Search in Google Scholar
Haudricourt, Andre-Georges. 1972. Tones in Punjabi. Pakha Sanjam 5. xxi–xxii.Search in Google Scholar
Kar, Somdev & Hubert Truckenbrodt. 2019. Syllable structure and stratification in bangla. Journal of South Asian Languages and Linguistics 6(1). 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1515/jsall-2019-2008.Search in Google Scholar
Khan, Abdul Qadir. 2017. The tonal system of Pahari. Acta Linguistica America 64. 313–324. https://doi.org/10.1556/2062.2017.64.2.7.Search in Google Scholar
Khan, Abdul Qadir, Yi Xu & Ayesha Sohail. 2020. Multidimensionality of tone in Pahari. Lingua 245. 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2020.102923.Search in Google Scholar
Ladefoged, Peter. 1971. Preliminaries to linguistic phonetics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Search in Google Scholar
Ladefoged, Peter. 1975. A course in phonetics. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Search in Google Scholar
Lisker, Leigh & Arthur Abramson. 1964. A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word 20. 384–422. https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1964.11659830.Search in Google Scholar
Mahanta, Shakuntala. 2012. Assamese. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 42(2). 217–224. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0025100312000096.Search in Google Scholar
Mikuteit, Simone & Henning Reetz. 2007. Caught in the ACT: The timing of aspiration and voicing in East Bengali. Language and Speech 50. 247–277. https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309070500020401.Search in Google Scholar
Rasinger, Sebastian. 2007. Bengali-English in east london: A study in urban multilingualism. Bern: Peter Lang.10.3726/978-3-0353-0428-2Search in Google Scholar
Simard, Candide, Sarah Dopierala & Marie Thaut. 2020. Introducing the Sylheti language and its speakers, and the SOAS Sylheti project. Language Documentation and Description 18. 1–22.Search in Google Scholar
Simard, Candide, Michael Franjieh, Tom Castle & Mukid Choudhury (2012-Present). Sylheti Project – SOAS in Camden. (Marie Thaut, ed.) Available at: https://sylhetiproject.wordpress.com/eine-seite/ (July 2016).Search in Google Scholar
Turner, Ralph Lilley. 1962–1966. A comparative dictionary of Indo-Aryan languages. London: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Xu, Yi. 1997. Contextual tonal variations in Mandarin. Journal of Phonetics 25(1). 61–83. https://doi.org/10.1006/jpho.1996.0034.Search in Google Scholar
Yip, Moira. 2002. Tone. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781139164559Search in Google Scholar
© 2022 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston
Articles in the same Issue
- Frontmatter
- Research Articles
- Introduction to sound change in endangered or small speech communities
- Where have all the sound changes gone? Phonological stability and mechanisms of sound change
- Where have all the sound changes gone? Examining the scarcity of evidence for regular sound change in Australian languages
- Cross-dialectal synchronic variation of a diachronic conditioned merger in Tlingit
- Vowel harmony in Laz Turkish: a case study in language contact and language change
- The evolution of tonally conditioned allomorphy in Triqui: evidence from spontaneous speech corpora
- Sound change and gender-based differences in isolated regions: acoustic analysis of intervocalic phonemic stops by Bora-Spanish bilinguals
- Place uniformity and drift in the Suzhounese fricative and apical vowels
- Flexibility and evolution of cue weighting after a tonal split: an experimental field study on Tamang
- The emergence of bunched vowels from retroflex approximants in endangered Dardic languages
- The expanding influence of Thai and its effects on cue redistribution in Kuy
- Speech style variation in an endangered language
- Sound change in Aboriginal Australia: word-initial engma deletion in Kunwok
- The dental-alveolar contrast in Mapudungun: loss, preservation, and extension
- Sound change or community change? The speech community in sound change studies: a case study of Scottish Gaelic
- Phonetic transfer in Diné Bizaad (Navajo)
- The evolution of flap-nasalization in Hoocąk
- Sound change and tonogenesis in Sylheti
- Exploring variation and change in a small-scale Indigenous society: the case of (s) in Pirahã
- Rhotics, /uː/, and diphthongization in New Braunfels German
- Generational differences in the low tones of Black Lahu
Articles in the same Issue
- Frontmatter
- Research Articles
- Introduction to sound change in endangered or small speech communities
- Where have all the sound changes gone? Phonological stability and mechanisms of sound change
- Where have all the sound changes gone? Examining the scarcity of evidence for regular sound change in Australian languages
- Cross-dialectal synchronic variation of a diachronic conditioned merger in Tlingit
- Vowel harmony in Laz Turkish: a case study in language contact and language change
- The evolution of tonally conditioned allomorphy in Triqui: evidence from spontaneous speech corpora
- Sound change and gender-based differences in isolated regions: acoustic analysis of intervocalic phonemic stops by Bora-Spanish bilinguals
- Place uniformity and drift in the Suzhounese fricative and apical vowels
- Flexibility and evolution of cue weighting after a tonal split: an experimental field study on Tamang
- The emergence of bunched vowels from retroflex approximants in endangered Dardic languages
- The expanding influence of Thai and its effects on cue redistribution in Kuy
- Speech style variation in an endangered language
- Sound change in Aboriginal Australia: word-initial engma deletion in Kunwok
- The dental-alveolar contrast in Mapudungun: loss, preservation, and extension
- Sound change or community change? The speech community in sound change studies: a case study of Scottish Gaelic
- Phonetic transfer in Diné Bizaad (Navajo)
- The evolution of flap-nasalization in Hoocąk
- Sound change and tonogenesis in Sylheti
- Exploring variation and change in a small-scale Indigenous society: the case of (s) in Pirahã
- Rhotics, /uː/, and diphthongization in New Braunfels German
- Generational differences in the low tones of Black Lahu