Home Beyond learning opportunities: focused encounters in a sociocognitive approach to second language acquisition and teaching
Article Open Access

Beyond learning opportunities: focused encounters in a sociocognitive approach to second language acquisition and teaching

  • The Happy Cactus Group , Dwight Atkinson EMAIL logo , Elif Burhan-Horasanlı , Anamaría Sagre Barboza , Jorge Andres Mejía-Laguna , Verónica Oguilve and Amable Daiane Custodio Ribeiro
Published/Copyright: September 20, 2023

Abstract

This paper introduces the sociological concept of focused encounters (FEs) as a tool for researching interaction in second language acquisition and teaching. FEs are face-to-face interactions affording joint attention, intersubjectivity, interaffectivity, and interactional alignment, considered key contributors to learning in some SLA/T approaches. First, we introduce the FE concept. Second, we situate FEs within a sociocognitive approach to SLA/T. Third, we apply the FE concept in investigating interaction in an Australian English as a second language classroom. Fourth and finally, we discuss our analysis’s wider implications for learning and teaching: Studying interaction in FEs from a sociocognitive perspective enables investigation not just of language learning opportunities but of moment-by-moment learning processes.

1 Introduction

Second language acquisition and teaching (SLA/T)[1] researchers generally agree that “there is a robust connection between interaction and learning” (Gass and Mackey 2020: 193), yet they differ on the nature of this connection, as well as the concepts of interaction and learning themselves (Ellis and Shintani 2013; Markee 2015; Pekarek Doehler 2018). Cognitive-interactionists, for instance, view interaction as a powerful means of making input processible for an information-processing brain; learning in this view is extracting linguistic data from the environment and building brain-internal language systems (Behney and Gass 2021). In contrast, conversation analysts view interaction as the product of an “interactional competence” substantially shared by all socialized humans. Learning in this view is using pre-existing interactional abilities to develop these abilities further (Pekarek Doehler 2018). Sociocultural theorists, for their part, view interaction as affording “zones of proximal development” wherein learners learn with assistance from others before internalizing knowledge for themselves; learning here equates with the progressive ability to consciously self-regulate mental action, including language learning.

No single review, to our knowledge, has covered all interaction-in-SLA/T approaches. Pekarek Doehler (2013), however, identified conversation analysis (CA) and sociocultural theory (SCT) as the two main “social-interactional approaches” to SLA, while Markee (2015) introduced five main approaches to classroom interaction, teaching, and learning research: the three approaches mentioned above plus educational discourse analysis, language socialization, and critical theory. Fruitful combinations of interactional approaches have also emerged; e.g. Hellerman and Thorne (2022) combined CA and SCT approaches.

Our own sociocognitive approach to SLA/T examines how interactors learn by multimodally adapting to or aligning with their environment, where environment’ includes both other humans and non-human (but often human-made) affordances (e.g. Atkinson et al. 2007, 2018]). Learning in this view is durable change in learners’ relations with their environment, leading to their greater environmental functionality. Teaching, in turn, is conceptualized as an evolved human disposition to helping others adaptively learn, as explained below. Compared to the interaction-in-SLA/T approaches reviewed above, a sociocognitive approach views learning as progressive integration of learner with environment, rather than extraction, construction, or refinement of individual or shared knowledge/competences.[2]

The purpose of this paper is to develop a novel approach to interaction-in-SLA/T based on the sociological concept of focused encounters (FEs), framed within sociocognitive theory. To that end, Section 2 introduces sociologist Erving Goffman’s understanding of social interaction and its key concept, FEs. Section 3 then outlines a sociocognitive approach to SLA/T. Section 4 introduces an exploratory application of the FE concept to studying interaction-in-SLA/T in an Australian ESL classroom. Section 5 describes and interprets the results of our exploratory analysis. Section 6 discusses implications of the approach to interaction-in-SLA/T developed here, and Section 7 briefly concludes.

2 Focused encounters in interaction

2.1 Interaction

Sometimes considered the greatest sociologist of the latter 20th century (e.g. Collins 1988), Goffman defined social interaction as “the reciprocal influence of individuals upon one another’s actions when in one another’s immediate physical presence” (1959]: 15).[3] Goffman (e.g. 1961]) further proposed that focused face-to-face interaction creates its own powerful form of immediate social reality; thus, if you and I are engaged in serious conversation, our primary reality is the joint-attentional, intersubjective, interaffective, and multimodal reality we are co-constructing and experiencing at that moment; everything else is secondary. Additionally, our normative social conduct in such interactions – gesture, gaze, facial expressions, tone of voice, body positioning, etc. – not only shapes that reality but can destroy it: We are extremely sensitive to others’ moment-to-moment expressive actions because they are the ultimate stuff human cooperation is made of, based on which “hyperprosocial” (Marean 2015) human animals survive/exist. The general question we investigate in this paper is: If learning/teaching are interactional, as is widely agreed in SLA/T studies, and if Goffman’s distinctive understanding of interaction has merit, then how can it help us to understand the role of interaction in SLA/T?

2.2 Focused encounters

Goffman (e.g. 1961], 1963]) referred to focused face-to-face interactions variously – as “encounters”, “focused gatherings”, “situated activity systems”, etc. – which Kendon (1990) helpfully reduced to “focused encounters” (FEs). Goffman (1961) described FEs as:

one type of social arrangement that occurs when persons are in one another’s immediate physical presence …. For the participants this involves: a single visual and cognitive focus of attention; a mutual and preferential openness to verbal communication; a heightened mutual relevance of acts; an eye-to-eye ecological huddle … maximiz[ing] each participant’s opportunity to perceive the other participants’ monitoring of him [sic]. Given these communication arrangements, their presence tends to be acknowledged or ratified through expressive signs, and a “we rationale” is likely to emerge, … a sense of the single thing that we are doing together at the time …. [FEs] provide the communication base for a circular flow of feeling among … participants, as well as corrective compensation for deviant acts. (17–18)

Let us unpack this dense description, simultaneously suggesting relevances to current issues in SLA/T studies. In our view, FEs have seven major features:

  1. Joint Attention (“Single visual and cognitive focus of attention”) – Interactors sharing a common attentional focus are engaged in joint attention, which has been widely studied for its role in human development and communication (e.g. Tomasello 2014). In contrast, individual attention has played a key role in the historically dominant cognitivist school of SLA/T studies (Schmidt 2010), including the cognitive-interactionist approach introduced above. Cognitivists often treat individual attention as a requirement for in-the-head learning, whereas joint attention is a sociocognitive phenomenon enabling intersubjectivity, interaffectivity, and ecosocial action. Heft (2013) further regards “joint attention [as] the basis of all pedagogy” (165; see also O’Madagain and Tomasello 2022).

  2. Openness to Communication/Participation (“Mutual and preferential openness to … communication”) – This term signifies a pro-communicative orientation, a prerequisite for all communication. van Lier (1996) proposed receptivity – an “active response” to language featuring “curiosity” and “exploration” – as an underlying condition for SLA/T, leading to “engagement” (see below). Partly following Goffman, Evnitskaya and Berger (2017) introduced the concept of willingness to participate (WTP), as marked by students’ multimodal displays of willingness to respond to teacher prompts, or to show attentiveness if unable to do so. WTP is a social variant of the individual-differences variable willingness to communicate (MacIntyre et al. 1998) and incorporates Goodwin’s notion of “embodied participation framework”, described below.

  3. Engagement (“Heightened mutual relevance of acts”) – Goffman (1963) employed “engagement”, “engrossment”, and “involvement” synonymously to characterize states of “cognitive and affective engrossment …, some mobilization of one’s psychobiological resources” (36): “When an individual becomes engaged in an activity …, it is possible … to become caught up …, carried away …, engrossed in it …. By this spontaneous involvement in the joint activity, the individual becomes an integral part of the situation, lodged in … and exposed to it, infusing himself [sic] into the encounter” (1961]: 38).

    In a revolutionary move in SLA/T studies, van Lier (1996) proposed that “language development is primarily a question of active engagement, not the transfer of linguistic items from one person to another” (67). He offered “warmth”, “connectedness”, “investment”, “intensity”, “socio-biological vigilance”, and “participatability” as partial synonyms for engagement, citing conversation as a paradigm context and quoting philosopher Merleau-Ponty: “In … conversation … my thoughts and his [sic] make up a single tissue, … insert[ing] themselves in a common operation of which neither of us is the sole creator. A double-being comes about, … we are … collaborators in … reciprocity, our perspectives glide one into the other” (168).

  4. Engagement Processes (“Eye-to-eye ecological huddle … maximiz[ing] each participant’s opportunity to perceive the other participants’ monitoring of him [sic]” as “ratified through expressive signs”) – Goffman described the dynamic processes producing/sustaining engagement: Consciously or unconsciously, we continuously emit “expressive signs” of our ongoing alignment with our ecosocial environment, and humans are monumentally expressive, head to toe. Likewise, we continuously monitor each other’s expressive actions, monitor each other’s monitoring, monitor our monitoring being monitored, etc., leading to continuous mutual behavioral alignment. This occurs most intensively in FEs. An increasing focus on multimodality in SLA/T studies gives embodiment a role (e.g. Block 2014), but Goffman’s concept presents powerful new perspectives.

  5. Intersubjectivity (“A ‘we rationale’ is likely to emerge, … a sense of the single thing that we are doing together”) – Mutual access to others’ expressiveness promotes intersubjectivity, widely considered crucial for social learning. For Goffman, intersubjectivity comprises individuals’ overlapping “definitions of the situation” – sociocognitive frameworks answering the crucial question: “What is … going on here?” (1974/1986: 8), thereby enabling a “working consensus” for cooperative action. In SLA/T studies, conversation analysts have studied intersubjectivity as the ongoing achievement of social organization via talk-in-interaction, often in tandem with embodied action.

  6. Interaffectivity (“[FEs] provide the communication base for a circular flow of feeling among … participants”) – Although usually considered a cognitive phenomenon, intersubjectivity is also affective (Trevarthen 2006). For Goffman, the circulation of shared emotion – or interaffectivity’ – is a primary feature of FEs. SLA/T scholars have likewise noted a possible “affective turn” in the field (Prior 2019).

  7. Normativity (“[FEs] provide … corrective compensation for deviant acts”[4]) – Goffman regarded social action as inherently normative, based on “ground rules of a restrictive and enabling kind” (1971]: x). As participants become engaged in FEs they present themselves as committed, agreeable, emotionally stable, mentally competent, etc. vis-à-vis the conventional norms of the activity engaged in. Such normative “presentations of self” (1959]) are continuously monitored and regulated by other participants through the engagement processes described above, resulting in convergent behavior: “The individual tends to modify this activity, … with its public character in mind”, yielding “an idiom of appearances and gestures that calls forth in the actor what it calls forth in … others” (1963]: 33). These presentations of self leave participants changed: Self/identity is constructed moment-by-moment in interaction for Goffman – we express who we are/are becoming through language use and embodied action – and is “accumulative” (Goodwin 2018). “Deviant acts” are substantially self-correcting via this process, but wayward participants can be nudged into line with a glance, intonation, etc. without explicitly disrupting interaction. The normative power of FEs thus operates both bottom-up and top-down as group members monitor, self-regulate, and align, thereby constructing social selves. Such normativity is central to learning/teaching, as specified below.

2.3 Extensions of FE concept

2.3.1 Embodied participation frameworks

Following Goffman, Goodwin (2013) developed the concept of embodied participation frameworks (EPFs) – semi-conventionalized “laminations” (Goffman 1974/1986) of expressive practices, material structure, sociocognitive frameworks, and social actors whereby communities accomplish social action. EPFs extend the FE concept by: (1) broadening its focus to include potentially any relevant environmental feature; (2) making it more dynamic/processual; and (3) connecting it directly to learning. Regarding point 3, Goodwin (2007, 2018] showed how EPFs’ hybrid multimodal-multisensory-material nature makes them publicly accessible, ready-at-hand foundations for “educating attention” regarding how to accomplish new social action, including language learning. Such “co-operative action” takes pre-existing material, e.g. something someone just said, and adaptively builds on/transforms it, thereby constituting a form of “natural pedagogy” (2018] and below).

2.3.2 Emotion/affect in focused encounters

Collins (2004) developed the crucial role of emotion in FEs: “Occasions … combin[ing] a high degree of mutual … attention … with a high degree of emotional entrainment result in feelings of membership … [,] emotional energy …, confidence, enthusiasm, and desire for action in … a [normatively construed] morally proper path …. We are constantly being socialized by our interactional experiences throughout our lives” (42–44). Emotion-sharing in FEs thus promotes the development of convergent social identities, which are expressed in and simultaneously promote convergent social behavior, including socialized language learning/use. For much of its history, emotions remained understudied in SLA/T studies; e.g. MacIntyre et al. (2019) stated that “given their ubiquity and importance, it is surprising that emotions have not … enjoyed …. greater prominence in the literature on motivation for language learning”. However, SLA/T studies may be undergoing an “affective turn” (Prior 2019), particularly in the area of individual differences. Emotion/affect is also central to the sociocognitive approach to SLA/T described next.

3 Sociocognitive approach to SLA/T

Focused encounters are here investigated from a sociocognitive perspective on SLA/T. The FE and sociocognitive concepts dovetail due to their ecological nature – both focus on the integration of mind, body, and world. The sociocognitive perspective was originally developed in response to the cognitivism historically pervading the SLA/T field (Atkinson 2002). Cognitivist SLA theories are “cold”: Learners are input processors, learning is extraction and processing of linguistic input, and interaction is input-preprocessing. Yet real learners are “warm”: We learn as we live in order to survive and prosper together in our ever-changing world; valuing, engaging, and responsively adapting are therefore at the heart of learning (van Lier 1996). Four features of sociocognitive theory are briefly introduced here (see note 2 for detailed descriptions).

3.1 Alignment

Humans dynamically and sensitively adjust/adapt to their ever-changing ecosocial “surrounds” (Goffman 1981). Language and other communicative modes afford such alignment; language learning thus means learning how to coordinate adaptive action together – an ongoing organic, affective, relational process – rather than the private acquisition of linguistic systems. For van Lier (2004), “knowledge of language for a human is like knowledge of the jungle for an animal. The animal does not ‘have’ the jungle; it knows how to use and live in it. Perhaps we can say … that we do not ‘have’ or ‘possess’ language, but that we learn to use it and ‘live in it’” (253).

Language learning from this perspective is more about learning to (co)operate effectively in a human world/ecology – a “highly structured form of mutual fatefulness” (Goffman 1961: 35) – than internalizing grammatical code. The brain in this view is primarily a “relational organ” (Fuchs 2018), not an internal information-processing device.

3.2 Embodied multimodality

Our primordial form of human sociality – face-to-face interaction – is highly embodied and highly multimodal. For Goffman and his followers, expressive multimodal behavior connects cognition, embodiment, and social action because it indexes individuals’ cognitive perspectives, attitudes, values, and attentional and affective orientations vis-à-vis social others, who we mutually and continuously monitor and align with. Multimodal behavior also connects us to our material world (e.g. through pointing, gazing, and manipulating objects). In fact, from a sociocognitive viewpoint, “internal” thought, perception, affect, knowledge/competences, etc. are not separable from “external” behavior/action or the material environment; instead, they exist and co-constitute one another in an integrated “mindbodyword” ecology (Atkinson 2014). Multimodal action likewise impacts learning “because it occurs within an embodied participation framework that creates a visible, public locus for attention and action that includes both relevant structure in the environment and the actions and bodies of other participants. This is especially important in situations of apprenticeship and education” (Goodwin 2007: 59).

3.3 Natural pedagogy (NP)

Teaching-based learning is a powerful form of ecosocial adaptivity that is innate in humans (Csibra and Gergely 2009; O’Madagain and Tomasello 2022; Tomasello 2016). That is, as a species occupying almost every terrestrial habitat on earth, our main adaptive strategies have been cultural rather than genetic; e.g. Arctic peoples developed complex dwelling and clothing technologies to survive the cold rather than growing fur (Boyd et al. 2011). Yet such cultural survival strategies (1) can’t be transmitted genetically; (2) are usually technologically complex and therefore non-intuitive/“opaque”; and (3) therefore require novel forms of inculcation/learning. NP evolved to fill this gap. Atkinson and Shvidko (2019) reviewed NP research across disciplines vis-à-vis SLA/T, while Goodwin (2018) tied NP directly to his theory of co-operative action within EPFs, showing how the solution to the problem “of creating actors with specific relevant understanding of objects that are intrinsically opaque … emerge[s] systematically from the organization of co-operative action” (308).

3.4 Emotion/affect/value

Cognitivism adopts an engineering perspective: The brain is a data-processing machine. Yet humans are biological organisms, their brains are in their bodies, and their first priority as such is to keep their bodies viable in dynamic environments filled with affordances and threats. Valuing and affect are therefore at the heart of human behavior, perception, cognition, and learning/teaching (van Lier 1996). Furthermore, as organisms dependent on cooperative action for survival we are highly sensitive to interpersonal environments and relations; in Goffman’s words, “there seems to be no agent more effective than another person in bringing a world for oneself alive or, by a glance, a gesture, or a remark, shriveling up the reality in which one is lodged” (1961]: 41). Classrooms are affectively fraught in this sense because their very existence requires displays of incompetence and associated judgments, which are inherently face- and identity-threatening. A basic requirement for learning, as well as for who one is learning from/with, therefore seems to be that they have value for the learner.

We conclude this section by noting that our sociocognitive approach does not seek to replace other SLA/T theories, but to dialectically complement them and thereby broaden understanding of the enormously complex phenomenon of SLA/T. Cognitivism, in particular, is not simply wrong from a sociocognitive perspective; rather, like all theories of human action/behavior, it is incomplete.

4 Exploratory study: research approach

In this section and the next we describe an exploratory application of the FE concept to video-recorded classroom interaction, framed in a sociocognitive approach to SLA/T. As exploratory qualitative research, both our research approach and analytic findings were emergent and interactively/cyclically developed, rather than pre-determined and top-down (Pailthorpe 2017). Our research question was the general one motivating this study (see Section 1): How does Goffman’s understanding of interaction/FEs, framed within a sociocognitive approach, help us understand the role of interaction in SLA/T?

4.1 Data collection

Data were collected via convenience sampling (Patton 2002): We searched the internet for videos of classroom interaction wherein participants’ multimodal expressive actions were maximally visible and audible. Of 21 videos found, we chose the video best meeting this criterion. One might critique this approach as a form of ‘cherry-picking’ – we found data we liked and used them to support our approach. This is true, but only trivially: A precondition for multimodal interaction analysis (see below) is that interactors’ use of embodied expressive tools is visible, audible, and therefore available for analysis; this was all we knew when we selected the data, as explained below. A second possible critique is that the video selected represents a single case, with nothing relevant to say about other cases. In contrast, we believe that anyone with any experience with classroom interaction will be able to ascertain familiar shapes and patterns of classroom interaction by merely inspecting the data. In fact, a nearly unique strong point of our data is their free and full accessibility to readers, unlike virtually any other classroom data set we know of. The video chosen is a 13-min YouTube video of an ESL lesson filmed in Australia, described further below and publicly available at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=DhCUVPkpLdk .

4.2 Data analysis

Our data were analyzed using multimodal interaction analysis (MMIA), as employed in our past research and detailed in Atkinson et al. (to appear).[5] Goffman, arguably the conceptual originator of MMIA, mandated that “the study of every unit of social organization must … lead to an analysis of the interaction of its elements” (1961]: 7), focusing, in the case of FEs, on “the glances, gestures, positionings, and verbal statements that people continuously feed into the situation, whether intended or not. These are the external signs of orientation and involvement – states of mind and body ….beginning with the littlest, for example the fleeting facial move an individual can make in the game of expressing his [sic] alignment to what is happening” (1967]: 1). More specifically, we (1) focused on interactional episodes that appeared rich vis-à-vis Goffman’s description; (2) transcribed them using modified CA and multimodal transcription conventions;[6] and (3) analyzed them in detail, integrating Goffman’s approach with our own past use of MMIA (Atkinson et al. to appear).

4.3 Contextualization: site, participants, and teaching approach

In order to contextualize our multimodal interaction analysis, we interviewed the teacher by Zoom.[7] The teacher, John Bartik, told us that the class took place in March 2014 in a church in Sydney, Australia. The church offered ESL classes, and John was tasked with training teachers. He had previously completed a one-month CELTA (Certificate in Teaching English to Speakers of Other Languages) course, and taught at local language schools.

The students were 10 Mandarin-speaking residents of Australia, placed by their “beginner” level and L1 background. The class met weekly for 2 h, but John taught it just once, in order to: (1) model an active “communicative” teaching approach; and (2) test his personal theory of language learning/teaching:

Language acquisition is all about setting up scaffolded sentences and changing one thing at a time. And so … you’ve got “Do you like my [blank]”, and then just having that as my template and then substituting – it was all about substitute one word to build that deep construct. That’s what was going on in my mind ….You’ve got a stock sentence and you substitute, “Do you”? “Do I”? “Do you like my jacket?” …, making sure that [students are] good at substituting things in and out. (Interview, 8/10/20)

John’s actual teaching approximated this account: He used a single question form, “Do you like my ____?”, filling the blank with lexical items related to his person. The four resulting prompts in the video’s first 6:20 – when John was sole prompter/question-asker – were “Do you like my” (1) “jacket”; (2) “blazer”; (3) “haircut”; and (4) “breath”.[8] After introducing each prompt, John (1) elicited repetition and provided feedback in full-class mode, and (2) prompted individual students, providing feedback on grammar and pronunciation. He sometimes did so in a dramatized way, which elicited much laughter.

5 Exploratory study: analysis

In this section, we first describe the two initial elements in Goffman’s definition of FEs – joint attention and openness to communication/participation – as exemplified in our data. Second, we describe the initial EPF constituted in this class and the learning affordances it provided. Third, we present a short data excerpt, showing how the elements in Goffman’s definition work together to enable learning processes. Fourth, we present a longer data excerpt more fully evidencing sociocognitive learning.

5.1 Initial elements in classroom FE

From 00:50 to 6:20 on the video counter, the classroom is configured as in Figure 1: Students (Ss) stand in a rough semi-circle around teacher (T).

Figure 1: 
Classroom configuration from 0:50 to 6:20 of video; “S1” = Student 1, etc.; “T” = Teacher.
Figure 1:

Classroom configuration from 0:50 to 6:20 of video; “S1” = Student 1, etc.; “T” = Teacher.

5.1.1 Joint attention

Goffman (1961] and above) identified “visual and cognitive” attention-sharing as key to establishing/maintaining FEs. Here we first use Goffman’s “visual and cognitive” formulation in our analysis before complicating it.

Once the initial participation framework emerges around 00:50 (see Section 5.1.2), Ss evince joint visual attention, gazing actively at T when he speaks, or at fellow Ss individually prompted by T. Figure 2 shows Ss’ shared gaze-following as the FE progresses: In 2A, T asks S10 “Do you like my blazer?”; in 2B, T prompts S1 to repeat this question; and in 2C, T playfully responds to S8’s response to a different question. In all cases, Ss attend either T or fellow Ss prompted by him closely.

Figure 2: 
Joint attention.
Figure 2:

Joint attention.

Visual attention is also a proxy for cognitive attention (McMains and Kaster 2009), the latter further evidenced by the “conditional relevance” (ten Have 2007) of Ss’ responses. Thus, T’s initial query, “Do you like my jacket?”, receives three immediate “yes” responses, one apparent question confirmation, and “I like”, “Yeah”, and “(It’s) beautiful” simultaneously or in tight consecutive order, the latter three responses accompanied by pointing gestures. Ss thus show that they are cognitively attending T’s question by responding appropriately, and jointly construct the order and form of their responses.

In fact, Ss don’t just attend visually and cognitively but with their whole bodies (Figure 2), evincing a shared “ethos, … spirit, … [or] emotional structure” (Goffman 1963: 19) of embodied attention. Thus, they stand with hands clasped in front of them, at their sides, behind their backs, or arms folded; their gaze and facial expressions indicate attentiveness; and their bodies and language generally align with T and fellow Ss. This style of attention might reasonably be called “serious, alert attention,” and is highly affective: “A heightened sense of moral responsibility seems to develop” (89). Affectivity is also evidenced in the group’s sense of humor/play (e.g. Figure 2C).

5.1.2 Openness to communication/participation

This criterion is exemplified in the video’s first 50 s, as the class’s initial EPF forms (Figure 1). T first addresses Ss sitting at long tables, explaining he is there “to help English classes”. Ss respond with a jumble of affiliative responses, prominently including “Yeah” and an echoic “help”; the only two Ss visible in the initial video frames nod positively. T then requests permission to video-record, asks Ss to “stand up” and “come here”, spreading his arms in a dramatic summoning/gathering gesture, and points to the floor beneath him. Although Ss show no understanding of T’s larger purpose, they participate in highly affiliative ways: standing, nodding, repeating T’s words, laughing, and gesturing while gathering around. Then, at 00:45, T steps away, turns around, and explains, “I want to TEACH you STANDING UP.”

5.2 Embodied participation framework

By 00:50, the EPF shown in Figure 1 has formed, “creat[ing] a visible, public locus for attention and action that … is especially important [for] apprenticeship and education” (Goodwin 2007: 59 and above) and providing at least five learning/teaching affordances. First, the semi-circular arrangement gives Ss maximal access to each other’s embodied action, enabling joint attention, mutual monitoring, and engagement. Second, standing provides greater interactional mobility and flexibility than sitting. Third, height differences between T and Ss – an ethological component of power/dominance (Stulp et al. 2015) – are reduced, thereby promoting affiliation. Fourth, the semi-circular linear arrangement affords a queue-like organization (Hutchins 2005) providing notable learning/teaching affordances: (a) predictable order – on introducing new prompts, T first addresses the full class, then moves up- or down-line prompting individual Ss in roughly consecutive order, thereby indicating what and where social action is likely next and therefore “how to go on” (Wittgenstein 1958); (b) Flexible sizing and shaping of “participation units” – T can address/respond to Ss whole-group, individually, in clusters, or Ss can organize themselves (see Excerpts 1 and 3); and (c) The queue is used to mark episode boundaries – T usually ends a prompting cycle around queue’s end, thus further structuring the learning environment. Fifth and finally, the EPF features linguistic routines, thereby further constraining the sociocognitive action-space by enabling predictable language behavior. T thus begins each prompting cycle with a “Do you like my ____?” question, which Ss are initially guided to answer positively. One overall effect of this EPF is to afford a stable substrate (Goodwin 2013) of social organization on which change – learning – can occur.

5.3 FE-enabled learning: an introductory example

While describing their features discretely, Goffman (e.g. 1964]) treated FEs as integrated interactional microecologies. Here we provide an introductory example from our data, showing how different elements in Goffman’s definition function together to afford learning through focused interaction.

As noted above, T’s initial prompt was “Do you like my jacket?” T subsequently substitutes “blazer” for “jacket”, models “blazer” for choral repetition, then starts a new prompting round with “Do you like my blazer?” After first eliciting choral response, T models both question and answer, and Ss repeat. T then moves to near-end of line and engages S10, first prompting repetition of the full question, then focusing on pronouncing “blazer”. S10’s responses are mostly inaudible, but S6 is hearable responding “I like your bla/dʒ/er”, then repeating “bla/dʒ/er” twice as T models “blazer” for S10. At this point S7, who has been observing T intently, intervenes. Excerpt 1 and Figure 3 capture the action.

Figure 3: 
Pronouncing “blazer”.
Figure 3:

Pronouncing “blazer”.

Excerpt 1
(1:51–2:03): “Blazer” (arrows mark Ss’ productions of “blazer”)
1 S7: ((Turning to S6 with right-hand beat
–> 2 gesture)) oBla/z/ero ((Facing away Figure 3A
3 from camera, repeats gesture while
–> 4 appearing to repeat “blazer"))
5 (1.5)
–> 6 S6: ((Shifts gaze briefly to S7)) Bla/dʒ/er. Figure 3B
7 S5: ((Takes 2 steps forward, gazing at T,
8 then turns toward S6 with eyebrow
–> 9 flash)) BLA/s/ER. (.5) Bla/ʒ/er. Figure 3C
10 S7: ((As T passes by, points & gazes at him))
–> 11 I like bla/z/er. Figure 3D

First, consider the role of joint attention and engagement/mutual monitoring as Excerpt 1 commences. Preceding the excerpt, T had prompted S10 with “Do you like my blazer?”, then focused joint attention on pronouncing “blazer”. Ss aligned with T by experimentally pronouncing “blazer”, repeating it at least six times in the 8 s preceding Excerpt 1, five coming from either S5 or S6 in close consecutive order. In doing so, Ss 5 and 6 offer what may be competing candidates for the most teacher-like pronunciation, S5’s “blazer” being inaudible and S6 saying “bla/dʒ/er”. Thus, in addition to maintaining joint attention with T, S5 and S6 appear to mutually monitor each other’s pronunciation, and may be producing competing, norm-seeking pronunciations of “blazer” on that basis.

Second, mutual monitoring and norm-seeking appear explicitly when S7 turns to S6 and presents her own pronunciation of “blazer”, apparently repeating it twice (lines 2–4) with gestures. The normative (i.e. teacher-ratified) focus of joint attention – the accurate pronunciation of “blazer” – is thereby amplified/supercharged by mutual monitoring and action, revealing a substantially more complex “mutual entanglement” (Clark 2022) than found in much classroom interaction research, e.g. instead of a top-down imposition of Initiation–Response–Feedback (IRF), we see a complex, patterned microecology of interaction.

Third, consider the social action afforded by the EPF in Excerpt 1. Following S7’s intervention, S6 momentarily shifts attention to S7, pauses, and repeats “bla/dʒ/er” (6). S5 then joins in, affectively upgrading S7’s action by stepping forward, turning toward S6, flashing eyebrows, and demonstratively responding “BLA/s/ER. Bla/ʒ/er”, marked verbally by increased volume and repetition (7–9). Neither S7’s nor S5’s actions are predictable/conventionalized parts of the pedagogical action/EPF; they are nonetheless afforded by it.

Fourth, consider the two final points of Goffman’s (1961] and above) definition: “[FEs] provide the communication base for a circular flow of feeling among … participants, as well as corrective compensation for deviant acts” (18). First, feeling/affect apparently circulates between S7 and S5, who are physically closest to S6 due to the EPF’s configuration, and therefore directly privy to her behavior. They appear to feel that S6 needs to be corrected, or at least made aware of the targetlike form: S7 begins by pronouncing “bla/z/er” quietly but insistently and then repeating it, accompanied by beat gestures while turning toward S6. Following S6’s reiteration of “bla/dʒ/er”, S5 intensifies the shared action with body movement, facial expression, and raised volume. In this sense, Ss5-7 constitute a temporary participation unit (Goffman 1981) within the larger participation structure, although S6 neither acknowledges nor responds to S5.

Second, it seems clear that Ss7 and 5 are attempting to norm S6’s pronunciation of “blazer”. In Goffman’s (1971) terms, they collectively “sanction” S6’s pronunciation, first mildly then more demonstratively. From another perspective, however, this “sanctioning” may constitute cooperative co-teaching: Ss7 and 5 try to help S6 produce a valued norm. Goffman’s (1967) notion of face may apply here – participants actively protect one another’s positive image/identity – or face – in order to preserve group functionality.

5.3.1 Where is learning?

So far, it is evident that much more than top-down teaching is occurring here. But where exactly is learning? Hanks (1991) provides guidance: “Learning is a process that takes place in a participation framework, not in an individual mind. This means … that it is mediated by the differences of perspective among the coparticipants …. Learning is … distributed among coparticipants, not a one-person act” (15).

Hanks here challenges traditional views of learning/teaching emphasizing individual over group, product over process, and cognitive internalization over sociocognitive alignment/interaction. In what follows, we suggest how the FE concept, complemented by a focus on sociocognitive alignment, complicates these views in Excerpt 1.

5.3.2 Learning as process

A longstanding critique in learning studies (e.g. Doughty 2003 for SLA) is that whereas learning is a process, researchers typically study products – static, before-and-after artifacts. This is because, when learning is conceptualized as a private/privatizing mental process, it must be studied indirectly. Thus, the historically dominant approach to studying interaction in SLA/T, cognitive interactionism, compares the linguistic intake into the invisible, internal learning mechanism with the linguistic output that results.

In contrast, learning is a public process in the approach adopted here: If the primary function of cognition is to afford ecosocial adaptivity/integration, then learning can only be an integrated sociocognitive phenomenon. The action portrayed in Excerpt 1 is thus an actual learning process – a making-and-becoming process – wherein Ss explore how to form a meaningful sound-object, “blazer”, as the focus of joint attention and action within an FE. Larsen-Freeman’s (2020) description of learning from a complex dynamic systems theory perspective resonates with our approach:

[Dynamic systems] are about becoming, not being ….There is a great deal of variability in a learner’s language resources …. [These] are not fixed internal representations but rather continuously assembled in real time, depending on the real-time interactions between person- and context-specific properties …. Furthermore, the learning trajectory is not necessarily linear because the learner’s language resources are constantly under construction and in flux as usage environments change …. What a learner attends to and makes use of in a particular instance is determined by the “reciprocal relationship between an organism and a particular feature of its environment”. (249–260, partly quoting van Lier)

Our analysis extends and complexifies Larsen-Freeman’s description by indicating (1) how such variability functions not just for individual learners but inter-individually/ecologically; and (2) the particular interactional micro-dynamics involved. These two points are developed in the next two subsections.

5.3.3 Learning as “we-process”

The learning process represented in Excerpt 1 is a “we-process”, wherein social action is a product of whole-group dynamics, not individuals’ aggregated acts (Atkinson et al. to appear). Examples include pair/group dancing, wherein individuals move collectively, and conversation, wherein “a double-being comes about” (Merleau-Ponty in van Lier 1996, quoted above). FEs are primary loci of we-processes, as both emergent outcome and generator of Goffman’s “we rationales”. FEs afford the co-construction of new social action in public space, and are thus key learning environments (Goodwin 2018).

One way of conceptualizing we-process learning is as merely an initial phase: After participants operate together to solve a problem – e.g. the normative pronunciation of “blazer” – they individually internalize the result. This is the usual pedagogical logic of group work, peer interaction, and task-based language learning (Long 2015). A more seriously social view might hold that language-as-social practice is never simply “acquired”/privately “owned” but rather communally owned, experienced, and perpetually operating within we-processes (Bakhtin in Wertsch 1998). Thus, language practices continuously evolve in speech communities, leading users to continuously adapt. SLA as traditionally viewed may therefore represent just the earlier and more overt phases of a lifelong process, at least given appropriate exposure to a speech community. Larsen-Freeman (2006) held that there is “no end-state” in language learning, while Hopper (1998) observed that “children do not seem to learn sentences, but … to adapt their behavior to increasingly complex surroundings ….[,] an enterprise that … continues throughout a lifetime” (161–162).

5.3.4 Learning as interactional alignment

Many explanations have been proposed for humans’ breathtaking ability to coordinate cognition as well as action: theory of mind, distributed cognition, mirror neurons, intersubjectivity, ethnomethods, intercorporeality, to name a few. While differing in details, all such proposals seek to explain how humans can “act as one” for adaptive action. Excerpt 1 presents a paradigm case: Participants function as a unit in constructing a linguistic form representing a relevant feature of the ecosocial environment. Thus, after T’s introduction, “blazer” is repeated six times over 12 s by three Ss in three different permutations, accompanied by sophisticated use of gaze, gesture, facial expression, body orientation, volume, and affect. Through such skillful, actively constructed sayings, hearings, and doings, Ss are agentively learning by bringing themselves into coordinated alignment via an interactive we-process. And although there is no final resolution, they are clearly negotiating shared agreement on the shape of a social object – a communally owned and approved norm – the collective search for which both motivates and organizes their social action. Learning-through-alignment is further illustrated in the following section.

5.4 Extended example of FEs in action

5.4.1 Context

T introduces a new prompt at 2:22: He smiles, takes three steps along the student line while sweeping it with his gaze (Figure 4A), stops and leans in (4B), then points to his right temple while flashing eyebrows, smiling, and intoning rather quietly, “Do you like my haircut?” (4C). After a one-second pause, Ss respond with laughter. T then asks S9 or 10 (off-camera) the same question with raised pitch (4D), and, following another one-second pause, asks S8 the same question with further pitch rise (4E). Following yet a third one-second pause, S8 answers “Yeah” while nodding (4F), to which T responds “YES” with a smile and quick palm-up gesture (4G), turns left, takes a step, puts RH to forehead in a theatrical “whew” gesture while exclaiming “Whe:w” (4H), turns away from Ss and then back, grinning broadly. Ss laugh enthusiastically (4I).

Figure 4: 
Initial set-up and prompting of “Do you like my haircut?”
Figure 4:

Initial set-up and prompting of “Do you like my haircut?”

5.4.2 Learning action

Excerpts 2–3 and Figures 5 and 6 present learning action occurring simultaneously with the foregoing. Here, as T begins prompting Ss with “Do you like my haircut?”, S6 undertakes a quest for the meaning of “haircut”. What ensues illustrates the inter-individual, multimodal, natural pedagogical character of learning/teaching within FEs.

Figure 5: 
“Cut” + embodied action.
Figure 5:

“Cut” + embodied action.

Excerpt 2
(2:33–2:45): “Cut. Cut. Cut.” (Marginal numbers mark S6’s repetitions of “cut”)
1 T: Do you like my haircut? Figure 5A
2 S8: (1.0) Yeah.
3 [
1–> 4 S6 oCuto ((gazing at T)) Figure 5B
5 T: ((Gazing at S8)) YES. (.9)
6 Whe:w
7 [
2–> 8 S6: oCuto ((after tilting head L while Figure 5C
9 gazing intently at T))
10 Ss: ((Laugh enthusiastically, except S6))
3–> 11 S6: ((Visibly utters “cut”, still gazing at T. Figure 5D
12 Turns L to Ss7&8 with RH extended,
4–> 13 establishes eye contact, then visibly repeats Figure 5E
14 “cut” with downward beat gesture))
15 (.8)
16 S7: ((Puts RH on head)) Haircut Figure 5F
17 S6&8: ((Put hands to head simultaneously & Figure 5G
18 move fingers. S6 lowers hand & refocuses
19 gaze on T))
20 S7: (Jiǎn de). Jiǎn tóu fa.[Cut. Haircut.]

Excerpt 2 begins with T asking S8, “Do you like my haircut?” (line 1; Figure 5A). As S8 replies, S6 utters “cut” in a quiet, somewhat breathy voice while gazing at T, apparently indicating non-understanding of “haircut” (4; 5B). S6 then utters “cut” twice more in T’s direction, the second visible but inaudible due to group laughter (8 & 11; 5 C & D). Instead of “letting it pass” (Firth 1996) and co-laughing, however, S6 initiates a “side-involvement” (Goffman 1963) word search, turning to Ss7 and 8 while extending RH, securing their attention, and visibly repeating “cut”, punctuated by a downward beat gesture (12–14; 5E). In response, S7 quickly raises RH to head (16; 5F) and utters “haircut”, followed immediately by Ss6 and 8 raising hands to heads and moving fingers there for about 1 s (17–18; 5G). S6 then shifts her gaze/attention back to T, and S7 utters (Jiǎn de). Jiǎn tóu fa. [Cut. Haircut.] (20).

When T comes back down-line 2.7 s later, S6, who was smiling before commencing her word search (Figure 4D), is smiling again. T questions S7, then moves to Ss9 and 10 (off-screen), whom he prompts extensively, supplemented by translation help from Ss7 and 8. Excerpt 3 and Figure 6 show what follows.

Figure 6: 
“I don’t like your haircut”.
Figure 6:

“I don’t like your haircut”.

Excerpt 3
(3:13–3:17): “I don’t like your haircut” (Arrows mark S6’s responses)
1 T: ((Moving up-line, gazing toward Ss1 & 2)) Figure 6A
2 S6 ((Leans forward, tilts head, with serious
–> 3 face)) I don’t, I don’t like (.3) oyour
4 haircut.o ((Raises L index finger on “your”))
5 T: ((Stops at “I don’t”, shifts gaze to S6 while
6 putting hands together, leans forward, flashes
7 eyebrows, & points)) Good. ((Steps back as
8 Ss laugh)) (1.5) Say I don’t Figure 6B
9 [
–> 10 S6: (Very long) ((points at T
11 with R index finger on “very”, then puts RH
12 to head on “long” & laughs))
13 T: I don’t like your haircut. ((Extends RH & Figure 6C
14 gestures on syllable beats))
–> 15 S6: ((Smiles broadly & nods)) I don’t like (.5) Figure 6D
16 your haircut.
17 T: ((Continues extending RH)) >Good good<.

As T reverses direction and starts up-line, S6 leans forward, tilts head back, and declares, “I don’t, I don’t like (0.3) your haircut” (3; Figure 6A). As S6 speaks, T pauses mid-stride, shifts gaze to her, and puts hands together (5–6). He then moves closer, leans forward, points briefly with R index finger, responds “Good”, then pauses a second-and-a-half, steps back, leans forward again, points, and says “Say I don’t” (8; 6B). S6 overlaps T’s speech with “(very long)”, apparently justifying her negative assessment, and laughs (10–12; 6B). T then restarts and finishes his utterance, “I don’t like your haircut” (13; 6C), which S6 repeats while smiling broadly and nodding (15–16; 6D). T responds, “Good good” (17) and moves on.

To summarize and interpret this extended example of FE-based learning/teaching from a sociocognitive perspective: T introduces a major new semiotic object, “haircut”, into the focused learning/teaching encounter, which S6 doesn’t understand. She therefore takes adaptive action by initiating an environmental search. Her first stop is T himself–S6 utters “cut” three times in T’s direction, receiving no response. She then searches elsewhere in the environment, enlisting Ss7 and 8 in her search. This approach works: S7 answers verbally (“Haircut”) and gesturally, Ss6 and 8 repeat and elaborate the gesture, and S7 adds Chinese translation. What has transpired so far, then, is rich, complex, and ecologically distributed learning/teaching action within an FE: Following a “we rationale” (Goffman 1961), Ss and T operate as an integrated organism.

The action continues: Rather than simply storing the new word in her head, S6 redistributes it in the environment. As she does so she transforms it, creatively embedding the new object within meaning objects already accessible to her – “I don’t like your haircut” – thereby undertaking co-operative action. This utterance is further noteworthy for what it adds to the learning/teaching event: S6’s contribution marks the first appearance of negation in the class – a powerful tool for the activity they are engaged in – answering “Do you like my ____?” questions/opinion-sharing. S6 therefore increases the class’s learning potential significantly, which T and other Ss actualize by incorporating negation into the lesson: The next three teacher-student exchanges (not included here) feature negative answers. In sum, the total teaching/learning action is highly “inter-” – ecological, distributed, intercorporeal, and interaffective; it is thus the product of dense sociocognitive alignment, or, in Goffman’s (1961) terms, a focused encounter. It is furthermore a natural pedagogical encounter, because it is grounded in intrinsic skills of human interaction: gaze, gesture, naming/labeling, repetition, body movement, etc.

6 Discussion

It is universally acknowledged that language learning is an incremental process – language-in-the-making versus language-already-made. Yet in many SLA/T approaches only learning outcomes – post-process artifacts – are treated as observable, quantifiable, durable, and therefore real (e.g. Ellis 2015; Long 2015). Doing so may be like studying movie-making by studying final cuts, versus examining the always-messy, always-dynamic movie-making process per se.

We have investigated learning-in-the-making by employing Goffman’s focused encounters concept as guide and tool, complemented by a sociocognitive theory of SLA/T. In this section we discuss seven features of learning from this perspective, and how they may contribute to a better understanding of SLA/T. Please note, however, that: (1) Although presented discretely, the learning features described here are not discrete/separable, but different angles on an ecological whole; and (2) In critically discussing other SLA/T approaches, our purpose is to dialectically complement and deepen them.

6.1 Joint attention and action

Schmidt (2010) influentially defined attention for SLA/T studies as a set of internal cognitive mechanisms “controlling information processing and behavior when existing skills and routines are inadequate” (no page numbers), hypothesizing that SLA requires attention to occur. Following Schmidt, a major goal in instructed SLA studies is to direct conscious attention to linguistic form, which may temporarily preempt meaning-based communication (Long 2015).

In our view, however, attention is an ecological/sociocognitive relation: Organisms naturally attend to what their environment affords their adaptivity and survival (Gibson 1979). For humans, this regularly means attending with and to others, because our main survival strategy is acting adaptively in groups: Joint attention is essential for cooperative action.

In FEs, joint attention further includes mutual monitoring – participants monitor each other’s actions in order to co-align. Such attention may be intensely focused, but occurs naturally without requiring highly conscious behavior: “The individual … can become unthinkingly and impulsively immersed … Conjoint spontaneous involvement is a unio mystico, a socialized trance” (Goffman 1957: 47). Accommodation, interpersonal synchrony, co-construction, intersubjectivity, and intercorporeality are other concepts indicating how we naturally co-orient/align. In this sense, attention represents ongoing ecological sensitivity and connectedness, not an internal cognitive mechanism requiring special stimulation to occur.

FE-based joint attention may affect SLA/T as follows. First, it may require no special cognitive effort or artificial techniques, only “a sense of the … thing … we are doing together – a ‘we rationale’” (Goffman 1961: 18, quoted above). Second, per Goffman’s just-quoted words, joint attention guides joint action, including learning. Third, it is highly embodied, as shown in Figure 2, where students track their teacher with their whole bodies. Fourth, it is highly affective: The students in our data adopt a shared ethos of serious attentiveness along with a sense of play/humor. Fifth, it is guided by the ecological “surround” (see Section 6.2 below).

Ingold (2001) and Goodwin (2007) treat learning as “the education of attention” for adaptive ecosocial action. Learners don’t internalize representations – cognitive knowledge structures replicating something in the world – but learn through guided experience to act in/on their environment in adaptive ways. What else could brains-in-bodies-in-the-world be for than responsively acting in/on worldly environments?

6.2 Rich, structured, multimodal environment

For cognitive philosopher Clark, being human involves “radical entanglement” (2022]) of mind, body, and “a complexly structured environment apt for engagement and action” (2016]: xv). For Goffman (1964), FEs are “microecological”: faces, hands, utterances, bodies, material settings, and social practices intertwine in “ecological huddles”. Goodwin’s embodied participation framework significantly develops Goffman’s vision, including how it contributes to learning.

The learning affordances of embodied participation frameworks are well-displayed in our data: Students’ semi-circular standing arrangement allows for both maximal interactional connectivity and specific pedagogical action/response. Through the teacher’s prompting of individual students in queue-like order, students know roughly when their turn is, and can use classmates’ prior responses to calibrate their own. This participation framework also affords emergent participation units of smaller clusters of students, whereby they co-teach and co-learn, e.g. their distributed engagement around the word “haircut”.

SLA studies typically views learning environments thinly – as indirectly influencing or “socially gating” internal acquisition processes (Atkinson 2013). Even conversation analysis, a major social perspective on SLA/T, views interaction as produced by a semi-autonomous “machinery” based on sequential organization, leading Goodwin to critique its “punctual, categorical view of action and actors”, versus “an interactive field … pulling upon distributed resources” (Goodwin and Salomon 2019: no page numbers), while Deppermann and Streeck (2018) recommended refocusing conversation-analytic studies on “simultaneity, multi-party settings, the materiality of multimodal resources, multi-activity, objects, space, and movement” (7).

Viewed ecologically, the world is overflowing with “information”, which we use to continuously orient and act in our world, including through language. Contra many SLA approaches, ecological information is highly affective and value-laden, based on “a defining feature of life, … responsivity” (Sheets-Johnstone 2009: 376). Far from being input processors or interaction machines, real learners are highly responsive humans-in-(e)motion.

6.3 We-process

As quoted previously, Hanks (1991) viewed learning as occurring “in a participation framework, not … an individual mind … Learning is … distributed among coparticipants” (15). This view hardly denies that individual minds, or – better – “mindful bodies” (Sheets-Johnstone 2011), exist, but treats them as fundamentally interconnected and ecologically embedded. Language is a tool for coordinating ecosocial action, which essentially shapes its learning; language learning is therefore a public/relational “we-process” (Atkinson et al. to appear), here as afforded by embodied participation frameworks in FEs. Classrooms are key sites for such learning, although typically viewed as promoting individual knowledge acquisition as measured by individual assessment.

The teacher curates the learning process in our data, but it is solidly grounded in basic processes of social interaction. That is, although novices in English, the students are experts in social interaction. Thus, in Excerpt 1 students produce differing phonological manifestations of “blazer”, but they do so using familiar interactional tools: gaze, volume, repetition, gestures, intonation, movement, facial expressions, etc. Therefore, although discourse/conversation analysts have typically treated classroom interaction substantially sui generis, it is grounded in basic building blocks of social interaction.

6.4 Alignment

Most generally, alignment is organisms’ ongoing responsiveness to their environment. More specifically, alignment is ecosocial – human ecologies are socially constructed, organized, and maintained. More specifically still, alignment is micro-interactional: Individuals conduct focused social action in/through embodied participation frameworks – dynamic micro-niches enabling responsive adaptation via multimodal we-processes.

Alignment has been variously defined in human studies. Conversation analysts, for instance, define it as “the structural level of cooperation”, versus affiliation – “the affective level of cooperation” (Hall and Looney 2019 for SLA/T; Stivers et al. 2011: 20–21). For sociocognitivists alignment and affiliation are inseparable: Signaling cooperation/interaction automatically entails taking an affiliative/affective stance. We align with what is valuable, and therefore meaningful, in order to survive: Feeling and doing are mutually constituted in ecosocial engagement.

A key concept in cognitivist SLA/T studies is feedback – “correction of production” (Behney and Gass 2021) of learners’ formally inaccurate speech/writing, typically by more proficient language users. Feedback is considered necessary to promote conscious attention to form, leading potentially to cognitive restructuring, leading ultimately to accurate output.

Yet feedback can be construed much more broadly, as basically every environmental happening organisms are responsive to. Viewed thusly, feedback represents the “differences that make a difference” in an ecosocial feedback loop (Bateson 1972). Goffman (1981) envisioned effective language use as adaptive responsiveness in presenting self to others, moment to moment, in order to maintain cooperative relations, versus externalizing internalized grammar: “What the speaker is engaged in … is to meet whatever occurs [in the interaction] by sustaining or changing footing …, select[ing] the footing which provides … the least self-threatening position in the circumstance, … the most defensible alignment” (323). How such interactional responsivity may motivate language learning is discussed in Section 6.6.

6.5 Variation

Modern L1A and SLA/T studies were largely founded on the insight that variation is essential for learning, i.e. errors are hypothesis-testing learning tools (Corder 1967). Contemporary SLA/T approaches increasingly give variation a significant role (Larsen-Freeman 2020; MacIntyre 2020), viewing each occurrence of linguistic form/function as a learning increment: “In every recurrence there is a compromise of iteration and innovation” (Brown 2005: 27). L2 teachers have likewise long known that error is essential in language learning; as veteran teacher educator/researcher Reppen (2021) stated: “We want our students to make errors …. If they aren’t … they’re in the wrong level, … not having to push themselves, … not having encounters with communicative, purposeful, meaningful language”.

The importance of variation, then, is well-established in SLA/T studies. Yet variation-as-we-process – variation occurring in/for group learning – has not been investigated to our knowledge. In our data, when S6 produces “bla/dʒ/er”, S7 responds “bla/z/er”, and S5 responds “BLA/s/ER. bla/ʒ/er”, along with gaze, gestures, body movement, and facial expression in an embodied participation framework, the students are learning together, not (just) in their heads.

6.6 Normativity, identity, self

FE participants monitor each other’s embodied expressivity, interpret it as expressing alignment to the group, and respond to it actively in co-regulating group action. For Goffman, this process is norm-driven and identity-driven: By presenting themselves positively vis-à-vis norms of social interaction, participants qualify as eligible interactors – crucial for a species dependent on group survival. General norms for FEs include presenting attentive, competent, prosocial selves/identities, while specific norms in language classrooms may include attentive responsiveness to pedagogical action, pursuit of accurate and fluent language behavior, openness to feedback/correction, and consideration/support for fellow students.

For Goffman, selves are relational: Who we are is based on how we present ourselves socially, and how we present ourselves socially is based on ideal, socially normative versions of self. However, Goffman didn’t view people as rote rule-followers, but rather as responding sensitively and continuously to normative values and practices, including resistantly and partially, in dynamic moment-to-moment interaction.

Goffman’s view resonates with those of major theorists of language and society, including Gee (2015): “Being a who (identity) engaged in a what (activity) is a performance …, visible in the world and situated within ongoing, but changing social conventions. If there are no conventions (social agreements and alignments) there is no understanding to be had, no matter what is in your head” (302), and Bakhtin, “To be means to be for the other …. Man [sic] has no internal sovereign territory, he is all and always on the boundary; looking within himself, he looks … through the eyes of the other …. I must find myself in the other, finding the other in me” (In Wertsch 1998: 116).

For Goffman, Gee and others, identities are continuously formed, negotiated, and transformed in interaction: If interaction is “the reciprocal influence of individuals upon one another’s actions when in one another’s … presence” (Goffman 1959: 15 and above), then we are always being shaped in non-trivial ways. This is patently true in child socialization, wherein children’s identities are continuously being formed, but, as Hopper (1998, quoted previously) highlighted, socialization is lifelong. This shaping is typically norm-convergent – we try to meet the expectations embodied in whatever group/s we identify with.

One major form of norm–convergent shaping is learning. During interaction, as interactors continuously shape each other’s actions according to norms/targets, new social action emerges. Thus, in Excerpts 2 and 3, S6 responsively enacts a self which is both norm-adherent – e.g. in her accurate learning of the form/meaning of “haircut” and use of negation in “I don’t like your haircut” – and norm-innovating – she introduces a consequential new tool into the class, which it then actively takes up — negation functioning in response to the teacher’s basic question: “Do you like my ____?” Significantly, S6 also does so with humor, thereby honoring the sense of play that is a local classroom norm. For Goffman (1971), norms are of both “a restrictive and enabling kind” (x), and in this case, when the class undertakes group learning of a powerful new means of meaningful response, it is enabling. Evidence of enabling norms may also be found in our data when S7 and S5 show regard for S6’s positive face in Excerpt 1. In these ways and others, normativity drives learning.

6.7 Learning as ecosocial homeostasis

In biology, homeostasis is the feedback process whereby bodies maintain viability in a dynamic world: Heartbeat, respiration, hunger, size of iris, etc. automatically adapt to environmental change due to continuous, highly sensitive body–environment feedback loops. Without homeostasis, we couldn’t survive.

When new, potentially useful phenomena appear in our environment, we adapt if possible because we need to be functioning parts of it. As widely acknowledged in the learning sciences, much learning is implicit and incidental, occurring naturally through exposure and experience. Implicit learning can be viewed as adaptive in nature; thus, in the case of language learning, “children do not seem to learn sentences, but … to adapt their behavior to increasingly complex surroundings. It is an enterprise that … continues throughout a lifetime” (Hopper 1998: 161–162 and above.).

Yet simple adaptivity may go only so far – many of the complex technological traditions innovated by humans over generations require more. Humans have therefore evolved an additional form of adaptivity critical to our survival – the ability to adapt culturally, as described in Section 3.3. One powerful form of cultural adaptivity is natural pedagogy – a supercharged ability to convey, receive, and in so doing develop and transform complex cultural solutions to human environmental adaptation problems. A few other species teach (by certain definitions of teaching), but no other species depends so radically on inculcating cumulative traditions of intelligent action for continuous development, refinement, and transformation.

Learning is therefore homeostatic in that when our environments change, we naturally change/adapt with them. Part of this homeostasis, however, is based on a human superpower, which is an additional reason why human learning – and teaching – “is not a one-person act” (Hanks 1991: 15): We learn sociocultural practices together in multiperson settings from more skilled others of all types, including but not limited to classroom teachers.

7 Conclusions

In this paper, we employed Goffman’s concept of focused encounters and a sociocognitive theoretical approach to investigate the interaction–learning connection in SLA/T. We believe that our findings show “learning in flight” (Vygotsky 1978), thereby going appreciably beyond simply demonstrating “learning opportunities” (Ellis 2015). We reiterate that our aim has been exploratory and complementary – we have no desire to delegitimize other SLA/T approaches. On the contrary, in order to embrace the enormous complexity of SLA/T, the field needs to be broadened, deepened, and complexified: All roads may lead to Rome, but to properly locate Rome in the first place, the more roads the better.


Corresponding author: Dwight Atkinson, Department of English and Second Language Acquisition and Teaching Graduate Interdisciplinary Program, University of Arizona, P.O. Box 210067, Modern Languages 445, Tucson, AZ, 85721, USA, E-mail:

Acknowledgments

Many thanks to Eton Churchill, Paul Kei Matsuda, and Songhe Wang for their always-wonderful co-cognition and co-action.

References

Atkinson, Dwight. 2002. Toward a sociocognitive approach to second language acquisition. The Modern Language Journal 86. 525–545. https://doi.org/10.1111/1540-4781.00159.Search in Google Scholar

Atkinson, Dwight. 2011. A sociocognitive approach to second language acquisition: How mind, body, and world work together in learning additional languages. In Dwight Atkinson (ed.), Alternative approaches to second language acquisition, 143–166. Oxford: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Atkinson, Dwight. 2013. Social and sociocultural approaches to second language acquisition. In Peter Robinson (ed.), Routledge encyclopedia of second language acquisition, 585–593. Oxford: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Atkinson, Dwight. 2014. Language learning in mindbodyworld: A sociocognitive approach to second language acquisition. Language Teaching 47. 467–483. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0261444813000153.Search in Google Scholar

Atkinson, Dwight, Eton Churchill, Takako Nishino & Hanako Okada. 2007. Alignment and interaction in a sociocognitive approach to second language acquisition. The Modern Language Journal 91. 169–188. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.2007.00539.x.Search in Google Scholar

Atkinson, Dwight, Eton Churchill, Takako Nishino & Hanako Okada. 2018. Language learning great and small: Environmental support structures and learning opportunities in a sociocognitive approach to second language acquisition/teaching. The Modern Language Journal 102. 471–493. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12496.Search in Google Scholar

Atkinson, Dwight & Elena Shvidko. 2019. Natural pedagogy in second language learning and teaching. Tesol Quarterly 53. 1083–1114. https://doi.org/10.1002/tesq.538.Search in Google Scholar

Atkinson, Dwight, Anamaria Sagre Barboza & Elif Burhan-Horasanlı. to appear. Sociocognitive perspectives on second language acquisition, teaching, and use. In Brian Paltridge & Matthew Prior (eds.), Routledge handbook of second language acquisition and discourse. Oxford: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Bateson, Gregory. 1972. Steps to an ecology of mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Search in Google Scholar

Behney, Jennifer & Susan Gass. 2021. Interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/9781108870627Search in Google Scholar

Block, David. 2014. Moving beyond “lingualism”: Multilingual embodiment and multimodality in SLA. In Stephen May (ed.), The multilingual turn, 54–77. London: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Boyd, Robert, Peter Richerson & Joseph Henrich. 2011. The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. PNAS 108. 10918–10925.10.1073/pnas.1100290108Search in Google Scholar

Brown, Jason. 2005. Process and the authentic life: Toward a psychology of value. Frankfurt: Ontos Verlag.10.1515/9783110328202Search in Google Scholar

Clark, Andy. 2016. Surfing uncertainty: Prediction, action, and the embodied mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190217013.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Clark, Andy. 2022. Architectures of the entangled mind. DigitalFUTURES Doctoral Consortium. Available at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=O0ggXvICpv8.Search in Google Scholar

Collins, Randall. 1988. Theoretical continuities in Goffman’s work. In Paul Drew & Anthony Wootton (eds.), Erving Goffman: Exploring the interaction order, 41–63. Cambridge: Polity.Search in Google Scholar

Collins, Randall. 2004. Interaction ritual chains. Princeton: Princeton University Press.10.1515/9781400851744Search in Google Scholar

Corder, H. Pit. 1967. The significance of learners’ errors. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching 4. 161–170. https://doi.org/10.1515/iral.1967.5.1-4.161.Search in Google Scholar

Csibra, Gergely & Gÿorgy Gergely. 2009. Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13. 148–153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005.Search in Google Scholar

Deppermann, Arnulf & Jürgen Streeck. 2018. The body in interaction: Its multiple modalities and temporalities. In Arnulf Deppermann & Jürgen Streeck (eds.), Time in embodied interaction, 1–29. Amsterdam: Benjamins.10.1075/pbns.293.introSearch in Google Scholar

Doughty, Catherine. 2003. Instructed SLA: Constraints, compensation, and enhancement. In Michael Long & Catherine Doughty (eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition, 256–310. Oxford: Blackwell.10.1002/9780470756492.ch10Search in Google Scholar

Ellis, Rod. 2015. Understanding second language acquisition, 2nd edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Ellis, Rod. 2020. A short history of SLA: Where we have come from and where are we going? Language Teaching 53. 1–16.10.1017/S0261444820000038Search in Google Scholar

Ellis, Rod & Natsuko Shintani. 2013. Exploring language pedagogy through second language acquisition research. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.4324/9780203796580Search in Google Scholar

Evnitskaya, Natalia & Evelyne Berger. 2017. Learners’ multimodal displays of willing to participate in classroom interaction in the L2 and CLIL Contexts. Classroom Discourse 8. 71–94. https://doi.org/10.1080/19463014.2016.1272062.Search in Google Scholar

Firth, Alan. 1996. The discursive accomplishment of normality: On “lingua franca” English and conversation analysis. Journal of Pragmatics 26. 237–259. https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-2166(96)00014-8.Search in Google Scholar

Fuchs, Thomas. 2018. Ecology of the brain: The phenomenology and biology of embodied mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/med/9780199646883.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Gass, Susan & Alison Mackey. 2020. Input, interaction, and output in second language acquisition. In Bill VanPatten, Gregory Keating & Stefanie Wulff (eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction, 192–222. London: Routledge.10.4324/9780429503986-9Search in Google Scholar

Gee, James. 2015. Reflections on understanding, alignment, the social mind, and language in interaction. Languages and Dialogues 5. 300–311. https://doi.org/10.1075/ld.5.2.06gee.Search in Google Scholar

Gibson, James J. 1979. The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Search in Google Scholar

Goffman, Erving. 1957. Alienation from interaction. Human Relations 10. 47–60. https://doi.org/10.1177/001872675701000103.Search in Google Scholar

Goffman, Erving. 1959. The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Doubleday.Search in Google Scholar

Goffman, Erving. 1961. Encounters: Two studies in the sociology of interaction. Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merrill.Search in Google Scholar

Goffman, Erving. 1963. Behavior in public places. New York: Free Press.Search in Google Scholar

Goffman, Erving. 1964. The neglected situation. American Anthropologist 66. 133–136. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1964.66.suppl_3.02a00090.Search in Google Scholar

Goffman, Erving. 1967. Interaction ritual: Essays on face–to–face behavior. Pantheon.Search in Google Scholar

Goffman, Erving. 1971. Relations in public. New York: Basic Books.Search in Google Scholar

Goffman, Erving. 1974/1986. Frame analysis: An essay in the organization of experience. Boston: Northeastern University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Goffman, Erving. 1981. Forms of talk. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Search in Google Scholar

Goodwin, Charles. 2007. Participation, stance and affect in the organization of activities. Discourse & Society 18. 53–73. https://doi.org/10.1177/0957926507069457.Search in Google Scholar

Goodwin, Charles. 2013. The co-operative, transformative organization of human action and knowledge. Journal of Pragmatics 46. 8–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2012.09.003.Search in Google Scholar

Goodwin, Charles. 2018. Co-operative action. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/9781139016735Search in Google Scholar

Goodwin, Charles & René Salomon. 2019. Not being bound by what you can see now. Forum Qualitative Sozialforschung/Forum: Qualitative Social Research 20. https://doi.org/10.17169/fqs-20.2.3271.Search in Google Scholar

Hall, Joan Kelly & Stephen Looney (eds.). 2019. The embodied work of teaching. Bristol, England: Multilingual Matters.10.21832/HALL5495Search in Google Scholar

Hanks, William. 1991. Foreward. In Jean Lave & Etienne Wenger (eds.), Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation, 13–24. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511815355.002Search in Google Scholar

Heft, Harry. 2013. An ecological approach to psychology. Review of General Psychology 17. 162–167. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032928.Search in Google Scholar

Hellermann, John & Steven Thorne. 2022. Collaborative mobilizations of interbodied communication for cooperative action. The Modern Language Journal 106. 89–112. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12754.Search in Google Scholar

Hopper, Paul. 1998. Emergent grammar. In Michael Tomasello (ed.), The new psychology of language: Cognitive and functional approaches to language structure, 143–161. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Search in Google Scholar

Hutchins, Edwin. 2005. Material anchors for conceptual blends. Journal of Pragmatics 37. 1555–1577. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2004.06.008.Search in Google Scholar

Ingold, Tim. 2001. From the transmission of representations to the education of attention. In Harvey Whitehouse (ed.), The debated mind: Evolutionary psychology versus ethnography, 113–153. Oxford: Berg.10.4324/9781003086963-7Search in Google Scholar

Kendon, Adam. 1990. Conducting interaction: Patterns of behavior in focused encounters. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Lantolf, James & Matthew Poehner. 2014. Sociocultural theory and the pedagogical imperative in L2 education. London: Routledge.10.4324/9780203813850Search in Google Scholar

Larsen-Freeman, Diane. 2006. Second language acquisition and the issue of fossilization: There is no end, and there is no state. In Zhaohong Han & Terence Odlin (eds.), Studies of fossilization in second language acquisition, 189–200. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.10.21832/9781853598371-012Search in Google Scholar

Larsen-Freeman, Diane. 2020. Complex dynamic systems theory. In Bill VanPatten, Gregory Keating & Stefanie Wulff (eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction, 248–270. London: Routledge.10.4324/9780429503986-11Search in Google Scholar

Long, Michael. 2015. Second language acquisition and task–based language teaching. Oxford: Wiley.Search in Google Scholar

MacIntyre, Peter. 2020. Expanding the theoretical base for the dynamics of willingness to communicate. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching 10. 111–131. https://doi.org/10.14746/ssllt.2020.10.1.6.Search in Google Scholar

MacIntyre, Peter, Zoltán Dörnyei, Robert Clément & Kimberly Noels. 1998. Conceptualizing willingness to communicate in an L2: A situational model of L2 confidence and affiliation. The Modern Language Journal 82. 545–562. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.1998.tb05543.x.Search in Google Scholar

MacIntyre, Peter, Jessica Ross & Robert Clément. 2019. Emotions are motivating. In Martin Lamb, Kate Csizér, Alistair Henry & Stephen Ryan (eds.), Palgrave handbook for motivation in language learning, 183–202. Cham, Switzerland: Palgrave Macmillan.10.1007/978-3-030-28380-3_9Search in Google Scholar

Marean, Curtis. 2015. An evolutionary anthropological perspective on human origins. Annual Review of Anthropology 44. 533–556. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-anthro-102313-025954.Search in Google Scholar

Markee, Numa. 2015. Introduction. In Numa Markee (ed.), Handbook of classroom discourse and interaction, 3–19. Oxford: Wiley.10.1002/9781118531242.ch1Search in Google Scholar

McMains, Stephanie & Sabine Kastner. 2009. Visual attention. In Marc Binder, Nobutaka Hirokawa & Uwe Windhorst (eds.), Encyclopedia of neuroscience, 4296–4302. Berlin: Springer.10.1007/978-3-540-29678-2_6344Search in Google Scholar

Mejía-Laguna, Jorge Andrés. 2023. Critical learning episodes in the EFL classroom: A multimodal (inter)action analytical perspective. Multimodal Communication 12. 23–44. https://doi.org/10.1515/mc-2023-0006.Search in Google Scholar

Norris, Sigrid. 2004. Analyzing multimodal interaction: A methodological framework. London: Routledge.10.4324/9780203379493Search in Google Scholar

O’Connell, Daniel & Sabine Kowal. 2009. Transcription systems for spoken discourse. In Sigurd D’hondt, Jan-Ola Östman & Jef Verschueren (eds.), Pragmatics of interaction, 240–254. Amsterdam: Benjamins.10.1075/hoph.4.14ocoSearch in Google Scholar

O’Madagain, Michael Tomasello. 2022. Shared intentionality, reason-giving and the evolution of human culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 377(1843). 20200320.10.1098/rstb.2020.0320Search in Google Scholar

Pailthorpe, Brittainy. 2017. Emergent design. In Jörg Matthes (ed.), International encyclopedia of communication research methods. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley–Blackwell.10.1002/9781118901731.iecrm0081Search in Google Scholar

Patton, Michael. 2002. Qualitative research and evaluation methods, 3rd edn. London: Sage.Search in Google Scholar

Pekarek Doehler, Simona. 2013. Social-interactional approaches to SLA: A state of the art and some future perspectives. Language, Interaction and Acquisition 4. 134–160. https://doi.org/10.1075/lia.4.2.02pek.Search in Google Scholar

Pekarek Doehler, Simona. 2018. Elaborations on L2 interactional competence: The development of L2 grammar–for–interaction. Classroom Discourse 9. 3–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/19463014.2018.1437759.Search in Google Scholar

Prior, Matthew. 2019. Elephants in the room: An “affective turn,” or just feeling our way? The Modern Language Journal 103. 516–527. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12573.Search in Google Scholar

Reppen, Randi. 2021. Effective grammar instruction for writing. Academic English Conference 2021. Available at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=SazWgoOdRoI.Search in Google Scholar

Schmidt, Richard. 2010. Attention, awareness, and individual differences in language learning. In Wai Meng Chan (ed.), Proceedings of CLaSIC 2010, Singapore, 721–737. National University of Singapore, Centre for Language Studies.Search in Google Scholar

Sheets-Johnstone, Maxine. 2009. Animation: The fundamental, essential, and properly descriptive concept. Continental Philosophy Review 42. 375–400. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11007-009-9109-x.Search in Google Scholar

Sheets-Johnstone, Maxine. 2011. Embodied minds or mindful bodies? A question of fundamental, inherently inter-related aspects of animation. Subjectivity 4. 451–466. https://doi.org/10.1057/sub.2011.21.Search in Google Scholar

Stivers, Tanya, Lorenza Mondada & Jakob Steensig. 2011. Knowledge, morality and affiliation in social interaction. In Tanya Stivers, Lorenza Mondada & Jakob Steensig (eds.), The morality of knowledge in conversation, 3–24. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511921674.002Search in Google Scholar

Stulp, Gert, Buunk Abraham, Simon Verhulst & Thomas Pollet. 2015. Human height is positively related to interpersonal dominance in dyadic interactions. PLoS One 100. e0117860. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117860.Search in Google Scholar

Ten Have Paul. 2007. Doing conversation analysis: A practical guide, 2nd edn. London: Sage.10.4135/9781849208895Search in Google Scholar

Tomasello, Michael. 2014. A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.10.4159/9780674726369Search in Google Scholar

Tomasello, Michael. 2016. The ontology of cultural learning. Current Opinion in Psychology 8. 1–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.09.008.Search in Google Scholar

Trevarthen, Colwyn. 2006. Stepping away from the mirror: Pride and shame in adventures of companionship. In C. Sue Carter (ed.), Attachment and bonding: A new synthesis, 55–84. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/1476.003.0006Search in Google Scholar

van Lier, Leo. 1996. Interaction in the language curriculum: Awareness, autonomy and authenticity. London: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

van Lier, Leo. 2004. The ecology and semiotics of language learning: A sociocultural perspective. Amsterdam: Kluwer.10.1007/1-4020-7912-5Search in Google Scholar

Vygotsky, Lev. 1978. Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Wertsch, James. 1998. Mind as action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195117530.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Wittgenstein, Ludwig. 1958. Philosophical investigations, 2nd edn. London: Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar

Received: 2023-06-04
Accepted: 2023-08-29
Published Online: 2023-09-20
Published in Print: 2025-03-26

© 2023 the author(s), published by De Gruyter, Berlin/Boston

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Articles in the same Issue

  1. Frontmatter
  2. Editorial
  3. Broadening the appliability of systemic functional linguistics
  4. Research Articles
  5. Functional linguistics in life: an embodied approach in teacher education
  6. Teaching citation to university students
  7. Patterns of interaction between experiential and interpersonal meanings in student texts in Spanish: grounds for system-based applications in an academic writing context
  8. System networks as a resource in L2 writing education
  9. Teaching Chinese grammar through International Chinese Language Education micro-lectures: negotiating mass and presence through multimodal pedagogic discourse
  10. Meaning-making in English-medium instruction science classroom interaction: from the systemic functional linguistics perspective
  11. Scaffolding instruction in an EFL drama lesson: a systemic functional analysis
  12. Teaching mental processes to EFL learners: a blended-learning proposal
  13. SFL as a socially accountable praxis: who and what are we working for?
  14. Regular Articles
  15. The influence of task complexity and task modality on learners’ topic and turn management
  16. Explicit grammar instruction in the EFL classroom: studying the impact of age and gender
  17. Language pedagogies and late-life language learning proficiency
  18. The relative effects of corrective feedback and language proficiency on the development of L2 pragmalinguistic competence: the case of request downgraders
  19. Unraveling the dynamics of English communicative motivation and self-efficacy through task-supported language teaching: a latent growth modeling perspective
  20. Effects of random selection tests on second language vocabulary learning: a comparison with cumulative tests
  21. Determining the L2 academic writing development stage: a corpus-based research on doctoral dissertations
  22. Dynamic development of cohesive devices in English as a second language writing
  23. What pronunciation specialists believe CELTA tutors need to know to prepare student teachers to teach pronunciation
  24. The effect of collaborative prewriting on L2 collaborative writing production and individual L2 writing development
  25. Beyond learning opportunities: focused encounters in a sociocognitive approach to second language acquisition and teaching
  26. Funds of knowledge for synchronous online language teaching: a translanguaging view on an ESL teacher’s pedagogical practices
  27. A frequency, coverage, and dispersion analysis of the academic collocation list in university student writing
  28. Fostering well-being in the university L2 classroom: the “I am an author” project
  29. How teaching modality affects Foreign Language Enjoyment: a comparison of in-person and online English as a Foreign Language classes
  30. Toward a better understanding of student engagement with peer feedback: a longitudinal study
  31. Chinese EFL learners’ basic psychological needs satisfaction and foreign language emotions: a person-centered approach
  32. Are foreign language teaching enjoyment and motivation two sides of the same coin?
  33. Orchestrating listening in EMI university lectures: how listening proficiency and motivation shape students’ use of metacognitive listening strategies
Downloaded on 14.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/iral-2023-0118/html
Scroll to top button