Home The use of blocking and inhibition training in processing instruction
Article
Licensed
Unlicensed Requires Authentication

The use of blocking and inhibition training in processing instruction

  • Nick Henry ORCID logo EMAIL logo
Published/Copyright: August 20, 2021

Abstract

Previous research has suggested that L2 learners often use non-target processing strategies to understand sentences, but that these strategies can be changed through targeted instruction that directs their attention to different linguistic forms or structures. The present study explores the effects of pretraining ‘blocking’ practice—a novel type of training designed to help learners inhibit the application of a strict word-order based processing strategy—prior to receiving a traditional Structured Input (SI) training focused on OVS word order and accusative case markers in German. The study compares three groups of third-semester German learners who completed three different activities in one training session: (1) SI with blocking practice (+BP), (2) SI preceded by explicit information (+EI), and (3) SI without EI or blocking practice (−EI). The effects of training were measured by sentence-level interpretation and production tasks administered as a pretest, posttest, and four-week delayed posttest. Learner performance was also assessed during training. Results in all assessment measures indicated that EI was most effective, but that blocking practice lent a slight advantage over −EI groups during and after training. These results are discussed in the context of studies on processing instruction and learned attention.


Corresponding author: Nick Henry, Germanic Studies, The University of Texas at Austin, 2505 University Ave, C3300, Austin, TX, USA, E-mail:

Acknowledgements

I thank the following people for their contributions to this research: the instructors who allowed me into their classrooms; Diane Beer for her support in materials development; and the attendees of the 2019 CALICHE Conference at Texas Tech, Hyoun-A Joo, and Liese Sippel for their helpful comments and feedback.

  1. Research funding: There is no funding information to report.

  2. Conflicts of interest: The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Benati, Alessandro. 2004. The effects of structured input activities and explicit information on the acquisition of the Italian future tense. In Bill VanPatten (ed.), Processing instruction: Theory, research, and commentary, 211–229. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Search in Google Scholar

Benati, Alessandro & James F. Lee. 2008. Grammar acquisition and processing instruction: Secondary and cumulative effects. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.10.21832/9781847691057Search in Google Scholar

Chan, Angel, Elena Lieven & Michael Tomasello. 2009. Children’s understanding of the agent-patient relations in the transitive construction: Cross-linguistic comparisons between Cantonese, German, and English. Cognitive Linguistics 20(2). 267–300. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.2009.015.Search in Google Scholar

Culman, Hillah, Nick Henry & Bill VanPatten. 2009. The role of explicit information in instructed SLA: An on-line study with German accusative case inflections. Die Unterrichtspraxis 42. 19–31. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-1221.2009.00032.x.Search in Google Scholar

Ellis, Nick C., Kausar Hafeez, Katherine I Martin, Lillian Chen, Julie Boland & Nuria Sagarra. 2012. An eye-tracking study of learned attention in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics 1. 1–33. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716412000501.Search in Google Scholar

Ellis, Nick C. & Nuria Sagarra. 2010. The bounds of adult language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 32. 553–580. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0272263110000264.Search in Google Scholar

Ellis, Nick C & Nuria Sagarra. 2011. Learned attention in adult language acquisition: A replication and generalization study and meta-analysis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 33. 589–624. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0272263111000325.Search in Google Scholar

Farley, Andrew P. 2004a. Processing instruction and the Spanish subjunctive: Is explicit information needed? In Bill VanPatten (ed.), Processing instruction: Theory, research, and commentary, 231–243. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Search in Google Scholar

Farley, Andrew P. 2004b. Structured input: Grammar instruction for the acquisition oriented classroom. New York: McGraw Hill.Search in Google Scholar

Fernández, Claudia. 2008. Reexamining the role of explicit information in processing instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 30. 277–305. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0272263108080467.Search in Google Scholar

Fine, Alex B., T. Florian Jaeger, Thomas A Farmer & Ting Qian. 2013. Rapid expectation adaptation during syntactic comprehension. PLoS One 8(10). e77661. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0077661.Search in Google Scholar

Foltz, Anouschka. 2021. Using prosody to predict upcoming referents in the L1 and the L2. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 43. 753–780. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0272263120000509.Search in Google Scholar

Gass, Susan M. 1989. How do learners resolve linguistic conflicts? In Sussan M. Gass & Jacquelyn Schachter (eds.), Linguistic perspectives on second language acquisition, 183–200. Cambridge, CT: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781139524544.013Search in Google Scholar

Henry, Nick. Forthcoming. Explicit information, input processing, and SLA. In Wynne Wong & Joe Barcroft (eds.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition and input processing. New York: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Henry, Nick, Hillah Culman & Bill VanPatten. 2009. More on the effects of explicit information in instructed SLA. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 31. 559–575. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0272263109990027.Search in Google Scholar

Henry, Nick, Carrie N. Jackson & DiMidio Jack. 2017. The role of explicit instruction and prosodic cues in processing instruction. The Modern Language Journal 101. 294–314. https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12397.Search in Google Scholar

Henry, Nick, Holger Hopp & Carrie N. Jackson. 2017. Cue adaptivity and additivity in L1 predictive processing. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience 32. 1229–1249. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2017.1327080.Search in Google Scholar

Henry, Nick, Carrie N. Jackson & Holger Hopp. 2020. Cue coalitions and additivity in predictive processing: The use of case and prosody in L2 German. Second Language Research Advance Online Publication.10.1177/0267658320963151Search in Google Scholar

Hopp, Holger. 2016. Learning (not) to predict: Grammatical gender processing in second language acquisition. Second Language Research 32(2). 277–307. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658315624960.Search in Google Scholar

Hopp, Holger. 2020. Morphosyntactic adaptation in adult L2 processing: Exposure and the processing of case and tense violations. Applied Psycholinguistics 41(3). 627–656. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716420000119.Search in Google Scholar

Isabelli, C. A. 2008. First noun principle or L1 transfer principle in SLA? Hispania 91(2). 465–478. https://doi.org/10.2307/20063732.Search in Google Scholar

Jackson, Carrie N. 2007. The use and non-use of semantic information, word order, and case markings during comprehension by L2 learners of German. The Modern Language Journal 91(3). 418–432. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.2007.00588.x.Search in Google Scholar

Jackson, Carrie N & Helena T Ruf The importance of prime repetition among intermediate-level second language learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 40(3). 677–692.10.1017/S0272263117000365Search in Google Scholar

Jaeger, T. Florian & Neal E. Snider. 2013. Alignment as a consequence of expectation adaptation: Syntactic priming is affected by the prime’s prediction error given both prior and recent experience. Cognition 127(1). 57–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.10.013.Search in Google Scholar

Kempe, Vera & Brian MacWhinney. 1998. The acquisition of case marking by adult learners of Russian and German. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 20(04). 543–587. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0272263198004045.Search in Google Scholar

LoCoco, Veronica. 1987. Learner comprehension of oral and written sentences in German and Spanish: The importance of word order. In Bill VanPatten, Trisha Devorak & F. Lee James (eds.), Foreign language learning: A research perspective, 119–129. Cambridge, MA: Newbury House.Search in Google Scholar

Marsden, Emma & Hsin Ying Chen. 2011. The roles of structured input activities in processing instruction and the kinds of knowledge they promote. Language Learning 61(4). https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2011.00661.x.Search in Google Scholar

Sagarra, Nuria & Nick C. Ellis. 2013. From seeing adverbs to seeing verbal morphology. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 35(02). 261–290. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0272263112000885.Search in Google Scholar

Sanz, Cristina & Kara Morgan-Short. 2004. Positive evidence versus explicit rule presentation and explicit negative feedback: A computer-assisted study. Language Learning 54(1). 35–78. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2004.00248.x.Search in Google Scholar

Seibert-Hanson, A. & M. Carlson. 2014. The roles of first language and proficiency in L2 processing of Spanish clitics: Global effects. Language Learning 64(2). 310–342. https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12050.Search in Google Scholar

University of Wisconsin Testing and Evaluation. 2006. German placement test [Assessment instrument]. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin.Search in Google Scholar

VanPatten, B. 2004. Input processing in second language acquisition. In B. VanPatten (ed.), Processing instruction: Theory, research, and commentary, 5–31. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Search in Google Scholar

VanPatten, Bill. 2015. Input processing in adult SLA. In Bill VanPatten & J. Williams (eds.), Theories in second language acquisition, 2nd edn, 113–134. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.10.4324/9780203628942Search in Google Scholar

VanPatten, Bill & Stefanie Borst. 2012. The roles of explicit information and grammatical sensitivity in processing instruction: Nominative-accusative case marking and word order in German L2. Foreign Language Annals 45(1). 92–109. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1944-9720.2012.01169.x.Search in Google Scholar

VanPatten, Bill & Teresa Cadierno. 1993. Explicit instruction and input processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 15. 225. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0272263100011979.Search in Google Scholar

VanPatten, Bill, Erin Collopy, Joseph E. Price, Stefanie Borst & Qualin Anthony. 2013. Explicit information, grammatical sensitivity, and the first-noun principle: A cross-linguistic study in processing instruction. The Modern Language Journal 97(2). 506–527. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.2013.12007.x.Search in Google Scholar

VanPatten, Bill & Soile Oikkenon. 1996. Explanation versus structured input in processing instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 18(4). 495–510. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0272263100015394.Search in Google Scholar

Wong, Wynne. 2004a. The nature of processing instruction. In Bill VanPatten (ed.), Processing instruction: Theory, research, and commentary, 33–67. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Search in Google Scholar

Wong, Wynne. 2004b. Processing instruction in French: The roles of explicit information and structured input. In Bill VanPatten (ed.), Processing instruction: Theory, research, and commentary, 191–209. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Search in Google Scholar


Supplementary Material

The online version of this article offers supplementary material (https://doi.org/10.1515/iral-2021-0068).


Received: 2021-03-30
Accepted: 2021-07-27
Published Online: 2021-08-20
Published in Print: 2023-09-26

© 2021 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Articles in the same Issue

  1. Frontmatter
  2. Research Articles
  3. English and Spanish speakers’ interpretations of L2 Chinese applicative double object constructions
  4. (Mis) perception of consonant clusters and short vowels in English as a foreign language
  5. Phraseological complexity and low- and intermediate-level L2 learners’ writing quality
  6. The use of blocking and inhibition training in processing instruction
  7. Second language listening instruction and learners’ vocabulary knowledge
  8. Acquisition of attributive adjectives and noun adjuncts by L3 learners of French and German: further evidence for the typological primacy model (TPM)
  9. Non-canonical word order as a measure of syntactic complexity in advanced L2 German
  10. Acquisition of morphology by L2 children in naturalistic environments: a case of Japanese case markers
  11. N-gram use in EFL learners’ retelling and monologic tasks
  12. The structure of L2 lexical-semantic networks as seen from a social network perspective
  13. Deciphering the role of multilingualism in creativity at university: the influence of context
  14. The associations between working memory and the effects of multimedia input on L2 vocabulary learning
  15. The acquisition of relative clauses by Spanish-Basque learners of L3 English: does dominance play a role?
  16. The impact of pragmalinguistic support on video-conferenced collaborative suggestion-giving task
  17. L2 writing development through two types of writing task repetition
  18. Learning aspect in Italian as additional language. The role of second languages
  19. Immediate and long-term improvement in lexical stress perception: the role of teacher and peer feedback
  20. Exploring the pseudo-longitudinal development of specific morphosyntactic features and syntactic complexity in CLIL young learners
  21. Measurement of engagement in the foreign language classroom and its effect on language achievement: the case of Chinese college EFL students
Downloaded on 26.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/iral-2021-0068/html
Scroll to top button