Posttraumatic growth after the loss of a loved one in relation to ruminations and core beliefs
-
Lucia Záhorcová
Abstract
The goal of this study was to examine the relationship between posttraumatic growth, ruminations, and core beliefs in grieving individuals after the loss of a loved one. The participants were 140 bereaved individuals (122 women, 18 men; M = 38.40; SD = 13.96). The results showed that posttraumatic growth was positively related to intrusive and deliberate ruminations; however, it was positively predicted only by deliberate ruminations. Core beliefs disruption was also positively related to deliberate and intrusive ruminations, but only deliberate ruminations positively predicted core beliefs. Moreover, posttraumatic growth is positively related to grief after the loss but unrelated to the time elapsed since loss. Higher posttraumatic growth was found in individuals who had experienced a sudden loss and loss of a close family member. Overall, the study shows that deliberate ruminations, for example about the meaning of the loss and its consequences, can stimulate posttraumatic growth in bereaved individuals.
Introduction
Several empirical studies (e.g. Calhoun, Tedeschi, Cann, & Hanks, 2010; Taku, Calhoun, Cann, & Tedeschi, 2008; Taku, Tedeschi, & Cann, 2015) found that after difficult, traumatic situations, such as bereavement, individuals can experience not only negative consequences but also positive changes in terms of posttraumatic growth. Posttraumatic growth refers to how individuals can overcome adversity or trauma with improved psychological functioning, higher level of adaptation, and understanding of life (Tedeschi, Park, & Calhoun, 1998). It is important to stress that posttraumatic growth does not mean that an individual does not experience any pain or grief (Calhoun & Tedeschi, 2001), but that posttraumatic growth coexists with the distress caused by trauma (Tedeschi & Calhoun, 2004).
For posttraumatic growth to occur, an important factor seems to be the level to which a traumatic event disrupts or threatens core beliefs, related to the individual’s understanding of life, his or her meaningfulness of life, identity, and future, which all contribute to the assumptive world of the individual (Calhoun & Tedeschi, 2006; Cann, Calhoun, Tedeschi, & Solomon, 2010a; Janoff-Bulman, 2006; Lindstrom, Cann, Calhoun, & Tedeschi, 2013; Linley & Joseph, 2004). After the loss, the individual has to deal not only with the distress caused by trauma, but also with the disruption of core beliefs, which can eventually even increase the distress (Calhoun et al., 2010). While the individual is confronted with the disruption of core beliefs, he or she needs to cognitively process the event by repeatedly thinking about what happened, with the aim of understanding the traumatic situation (Cann et al., 2010a; Lindstrom et al., 2013). Initially, these repeated thoughts, ruminations, were described as entirely negative thoughts, related to less adaptive coping, more intensive grief, and a higher risk of depression (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2001). However, Nolen-Hoeksama and Davis (2004) later defined two types of ruminations. Intrusive ruminations happen when unwanted thoughts interfere with the individual’s cognitive world and are related to various negative consequences (e.g. “Thoughts about the event came to mind and I could not stop thinking about them”, Cann et al., 2011b). Deliberate ruminations happen when an individual voluntarily thinks about the trauma with the aim of understanding the trauma, changes caused by the trauma, and the consequences for the future (e.g. “I thought about whether I have learned anything as a result of my experience”, Cann et al., 2011b); these ruminations lead to constructive problem-solving and meaning-making (Calhoun et al., 2010; Cann et al., 2010; Nolen-Hoeksema & Davis, 2004). It seems that during the grieving process, both types of ruminations tend to coexist and oscillate (Ramos & Leal, 2013; Stroebe & Schut, 1999). People ruminate about the loss until they re-develop their assumptions of the world, schemes, values, and personal stories that have been threatened by the loss, so that they can become more resilient to another possible disruption of core beliefs. These results are experienced as posttraumatic growth (Calhoun & Tedeschi, 2006; Tedeschi & Calhoun, 2004; Tedeschi, Park, & Calhoun, 1998).
The aim of our study is to gain a deeper understanding of the specifics of posttraumatic growth in bereaved individuals, and particularly how it relates to core beliefs disruption and different types of ruminations. Research studies in this field have been done mostly in the American population, with contradictory findings (e.g. Cann et al., 2010a; Lindstrom et al., 2013; Losavio et al., 2011; Taku & Oshio, 2015), and we are not aware of any similar study in Slovakia. Moreover, a partial goal of our study is to test the relationship between posttraumatic growth and the intensity of grief—which seems to be unclear—and to measure possible differences in posttraumatic growth based on the type of loss, relationship with the deceased, and time since loss.
Posttraumatic growth and ruminations
Several studies (e.g. Cann et al., 2010; Lindtrom et al., 2011; Taku & Oshio, 2015; Triplett, Tedeschi, Cann, Calhoun, & Reeve, 2011) have been conducted on the relationship between posttraumatic growth and ruminations, and clearly show that deliberate ruminations are an important predictor of posttraumatic growth. On the other hand, intrusive ruminations have been shown to be related to negative consequences, such as distress (e.g. Calhoun, Cann, Tedeschi, & McMillan, 2000; Cann et al., 2010a). Taku et al. (2008) found in a sample of bereaved university students that deliberate rumination soon after the event predicted higher posttraumatic growth. Intrusive ruminations have been shown to be positively predictive of distress, but not predictive of posttraumatic growth. However, in research by Lindstrom et al. (2013), both deliberate ruminations and intrusive ruminations positively predicted posttraumatic growth, while deliberate ruminations were more strongly related to posttraumatic growth. A clear positive relationship has been shown to exist between deliberate rumination and posttraumatic growth, so we formulated hypothesis H1: We assume that there is a positive relationship between deliberate ruminations and posttraumatic development in bereaved individuals. Based on the inconsistent findings about the relationship between intrusive ruminations and posttraumatic growth, research question Q1 was formulated: Is there a relationship between posttraumatic growth and intrusive ruminations in bereaved individuals? To better understand relationships between types of rumination and posttraumatic growth, research question Q2 was formulated: How do deliberate and intrusive ruminations predict posttraumatic growth?
Posttraumatic growth and core beliefs
Research studies in bereaved individuals suggest that the loss of a loved one can disrupt the individual’s core beliefs. The core component of the posttraumatic growth model is the premise that the process that eventually leads to posttraumatic growth starts with events which question or disrupt important aspects of assumptions about the world and therefore also disrupt core beliefs (Linley & Joseph, 2004). It has been shown that those who experience higher disruption of core beliefs also experience higher posttraumatic growth (Cann et al., 2010a, 2010b; Lindstrom et al., 2013). Similarly, research into stress-related growth after minor stressful events (Losavia et al., 2011) has shown that stress-related growth is also positively related to disruption of core beliefs. Challenges to our basic assumptions about the world, and therefore to our core beliefs, seem to be an important factor and perhaps also a causal predictor of posttraumatic growth. Therefore, we formulated hypothesis H2: We assume that the disruption of core beliefs positively predicts posttraumatic growth in bereaved individuals.
Core beliefs and ruminations
After the loss of a loved one, the disruption of assumptions about the world is likely to trigger other processes, such as deliberate and intrusive ruminations (Linley & Joseph, 2004; Calhoun & Tedeschi, 2006). Triplett, Tedeschi, Cann, Calhoun and Reeve (2012) and Losavio et al. (2011) found that a challenge to core beliefs is significantly related to both types of ruminations—deliberate and intrusive ruminations. Based on these research findings, we formulated two research hypotheses, H3a: We assume that disruption of core beliefs is positively related to deliberate ruminations in bereaved individuals. H3b: We assume that disruption of core beliefs is positively related to intrusive ruminations in bereaved individuals. To better understand the relationship between these types of ruminations and core beliefs, we formulated research question Q3: How do deliberate and intrusive ruminations predict core beliefs?
Grief and posttraumatic growth
Some studies have examined the relationship between grief and posttraumatic growth after the loss of a loved one and found the relationship to be positive (e.g. Ens & Bond, 2005; Hirooka, Fukahori, Ozawa, & Akita, 2017), while other investigations have indicated no relationship at all (Caserta, Lund, Utz, & Vries, 2009; Yilmaz & Zara, 2016). These results are contradictory both in relation to these variables and to the different types of loss. For example, a study by Büchi et al. (2007) found a positive relationship between grief and posttraumatic growth in grieving parents, while Martinčeková and Klatt (2017) found no relationship between grief and posttraumatic growth in grieving mothers. Based on these inconsistent research findings we formulated research question Q4: Is there a relationship between grief and posttraumatic growth after the loss of a loved one?
Posttraumatic growth and factors of loss
Several studies have focused on the differences in posttraumatic growth based on relationship with the deceased, time elapsed since loss, and type of loss. A study by Ogińska-Bulik (2014) found that people who experienced the loss of a parent or a sibling experienced greater positive change compared with individuals who had lost a child. Also, the loss of a brother or sister leads to higher posttraumatic growth than does the loss of a close friend. Cofini et al. (2014) found that posttraumatic growth was a little higher in individuals who had lost a close family member compared to those who had lost a friend or a lover. Armstrong and Shakespeare-Finch (2011) found highest posttraumatic growth in individuals who had lost a close member from a first-degree relationship (a spouse, parent, sibling, child), slightly lower posttraumatic growth in individuals who had lost a second-degree family member (a grandparent, niece, nephew) and lowest posttraumatic growth in individuals who had lost a friend. Based on these findings we formulated hypothesis H4: We assume that posttraumatic growth will be higher in individuals who have lost a first-degree family member than in those individuals who have lost a second-degree family member.
Posttraumatic growth is a process which can take some time, and therefore it is possible to assume that length of time since the loss might be related to higher posttraumatic growth. This assumption has been confirmed in some previous studies in individuals who had lost a loved one (e.g. Cofini et al., 2014; Yilmaz & Zara, 2016). In a study by Caserta et al. (2009), at least 20% of participants experienced medium to high posttraumatic growth between two and six months after the death of a loved one. However, time since loss alone was not related to posttraumatic growth, which could have been affected by the fact that the study comprised participants who had recently experienced a loss. Based on these unclear research findings, we formulated research question Q5: Is there a relationship between time since loss and posttraumatic growth after the loss?
The intensity of posttraumatic growth in bereaved individuals may be influenced by type of loss, that is, whether the loss was sudden or anticipated. Currier, Holland, and Neimeyer (2006) found that, after a loss caused by accident, murder, or suicide, young adults experienced higher levels of grief and had more problems finding meaning in their loss than individuals who had lost a close person for natural reasons. Currier, Mallot, Martinez, Sandy, & Neimeyer (2013) found that individuals who had lost a loved one due to an accident, suicide, or homicide, perceived higher positive changes in their life, despite simultaneously experiencing higher distress levels related to the traumatic event. These findings suggest that sudden loss may lead to higher distress and to higher posttraumatic growth at the same time. In view of these assumptions, we formulated research hypothesis H5: We assume that individuals experiencing a sudden loss experience higher posttraumatic growth than individuals experiencing the anticipated loss of a loved one.
Death of a loved one is often related to higher levels of posttraumatic growth in the domains of appreciation of life and relationships with others, and to relatively low posttraumatic growth in the domains of new possibilities and personal strength (Shakespeare-Finch & Armstrong, 2010; Taku et al., 2015). Similarly, a study by Ogińska-Bulik (2014) found highest posttraumatic growth in the domains relating to others and appreciation of life. A high level of posttraumatic growth in the relating to others domain has been found in other studies of bereaved individuals (Cofini et al., 2014; Currier et al., 2013; Taku & Oshio, 2015). Based on these research findings, we formulated hypothesis H6: We assume that the highest posttraumatic growth in bereaved individuals will be in the domains of relationships with others and appreciation of life.
Material and method
Research sample
The study comprised 140 individuals. The participants had to fulfil the following criteria: experienced the loss of a loved one at least one year previously; aged 18 years or older at the time the questionnaire was completed. The research sample consisted of 87.1% women (n = 122) and 12.9% men (n = 18). It is common for research samples of bereaved individuals to contain a low percentage of men (e.g. Ogińská-Bulik, 2014). Ages ranged from 18 to 72 years old (M = 38.40; SD = 13.96). Most of the participants had completed secondary school (n = 76), Bachelor’s degree (n = 23), Master’s degree (n = 31), Doctoral degree (n = 5) and some had completed lower secondary school (n = 5). Time since the loss ranged from one year to 42 years (M = 7.01; SD = 7.60). Type of loss was divided into different categories based on relationship with the deceased: loss of a loved one from a first-degree relationship (n = 97; loss of a parent, n = 33; child, n = 30; spouse, n = 22; sibling, n = 12) and loss of a loved one from a second-degree relationship (n = 43; loss of a grandparent, n = 29; a friend, n = 7; an uncle, a cousin, n = 7). Cause of loss was divided into specific categories: sudden loss—such as a heart attack or car accident (n = 93), and anticipated loss—such as anticipated death (n = 47).
Methods
The questionnaire contained items relating to sociodemographic data (age, gender, and highest level of education completed) and data concerning the loss (time since loss, cause of loss, type of relationship to deceased and closeness of relationship to deceased).
To assess posttraumatic growth, we used the Posttraumatic Growth Inventory (PTGI; Tedeschi & Calhoun, 1996), which is one of the most commonly used questionnaires to assess posttraumatic growth. The questionnaire is used in clinical praxis as well as in research, and there are many studies verifying its psychometric properties and factor structure (Tedeschi & Calhoun, 1996). The questionnaire consists of 21 items where the individual has to complete the statement “As a result of my loss I have experienced...” The individual rates the item on a 6-point Likert scale (0 = I did not experience this change; 5 = I experienced this change to a very great degree). The questionnaire consists of 5 scales: Relating to Others (7 items, e.g. “I have a greater sense of closeness with others.”); New Possibilities (5 items; e.g. “I developed new interests.”); Personal Strength (4 items, e.g. “I know better that I can handle difficulties.”); Spiritual Change (2 items, e.g. “I have a better understanding of spiritual matters.”); Appreciation of Life (3 items, e.g. “I changed my priorities about what is important in life.”). The internal consistency of the PTGI was very good for the whole questionnaire α = .96, and for the individual domains: relating to others α = .91; new possibilities α = .91, personal strength α = .84, spiritual change α = .68, appreciation of life α = .81.
The Event-related Rumination Inventory (ERRI; Cann et al., 2011b) was used to assess ruminations in the context of loss. The scale consists of 20 items assessed on a 4-point Likert scale from “not at all” to “often”. It consists of two subscales: deliberate rumination (e.g. “I thought about whether I could find meaning from my experience”) and intrusive rumination (e.g. “I thought about the event when I did not mean to”). The internal consistency of this scale was very good: α = .96 (intrusive rumination: α = .96, deliberate rumination: α = .94).
Core Bereavement Items (CBI1; Burnett et al., 1997) is a 17-item questionnaire measuring the intensity of grief reactions, specifically the emotions, thoughts, and behaviours relating to the loss of the loved one. Items are assessed on a 4-point Likert scale, for example “Do reminders of X such as photos, situations, music, places etc. cause you to feel sadness?” and “Do you find yourself thinking of reunion with X?”. The internal consistency of the questionnaire was found to be very good, α = .93.
Core Beliefs Inventory (CBI2; Cann et al., 2010b) was developed to assess changes in core beliefs due to a certain traumatic event. The scale consists of 9 items assessed on a 6-point Likert scale (from 0 – not at all to 5 – extremely; e.g. “Because of the event, I seriously examined my beliefs about the meaning of my life.”). The items are focused on disruption of core beliefs in the following areas: religious and spiritual beliefs, human nature, relationships with other people, meaning of life, and personal strengths and weaknesses. The reliability of the questionnaire was high, α = .90.
The data collection process
The questionnaire was distributed in electronic form and shared among various support groups for the loss of a loved one (e.g. the organizations “Kolobeh života” and “Viaticus” cooperated with us and distributed the questionnaire). Prior to completing the questionnaire, participants signed an informed consent form. They were informed that their participation was voluntary and that they could discontinue their participation at any time.
Results
The means, standard deviations, and correlations between the variables are presented in Table 1.
Means, standard deviations and correlations between variables
1. | 2. | 3. | 4. | 5. | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. posttraumatic growth | - | ||||
2. deliberate ruminations | .63** | - | |||
3. intrusive ruminations | .41** | .65** | - | ||
4. core beliefs | .59** | .59** | .36** | - | |
5. grief | .45** | .54* | .54** | .41** | - |
M | 58.34 | 19.46 | 21.73 | 25.47 | 27.59 |
SD | 27.22 | 8.52 | 8.44 | 10.71 | 11.89 |
-
Note: N=140 *p<0.05 **p<0.001
Hypothesis H1 was formulated as: “We assume that there is a positive relationship between deliberate ruminations and posttraumatic development in bereaved individuals” and research question Q1 was formulated as: “Is there a relationship between posttraumatic growth and intrusive ruminations in bereaved individuals?” The results of the Pearson correlational analysis showed that there was a strong positive relationship between deliberate ruminations and posttraumatic growth (r = .63) and a medium positive correlation between intrusive ruminations and posttraumatic growth (r = .41). Hypothesis H1 was supported.
To answer research question Q2 – How do deliberate and intrusive ruminations predict posttraumatic growth? – we used multiple regression analysis, the ENTER method. The results of the regression analysis show that both predictors explain 39% of variation in posttraumatic growth, (F(2, 137) = 45.84, p < .001). Deliberate rumination was shown to be a significant positive predictor of posttraumatic growth (β = .63; p < .001). The results of the regression analysis are shown in Table 2.
Regression analysis of deliberate and intrusive ruminations as predictors of posttraumatic growth
posttraumatic growth | β | t | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
intrusive rumination | .00 | -.005 | .996 | |
deliberate rumination | .64** | 7.293 | .000 |
-
Note: **p ≤ 0.01
In hypothesis H2 we formulated the assumption that core beliefs disruption positively predicts posttraumatic growth in bereaved individuals. The results of the regression analysis show that disruption of core beliefs positively predicted posttraumatic growth in bereaved individuals (β = .59; p < .001). Hypothesis H2 was supported.
Hypothesis H3a was formulated in the following way: “We assume that disruption of core beliefs is positively related to deliberate ruminations in bereaved individuals.” Hypothesis H3b was formulated in the following way: “We assume that disruption of core beliefs is positively related to intrusive ruminations in bereaved individuals.” The correlational analysis showed that disruption of core beliefs was positively related to deliberate ruminations (r = .59) and that there was a medium relation to intrusive ruminations (r = .36). Hypotheses H3a and H3b were supported. To answer research question Q3 – How do deliberate and intrusive ruminations predict core beliefs? – we used multiple regression analysis, the ENTER method. The results of the regression analysis showed that both predictors explained 34% of variation in core beliefs, (F(2, 137) = 35.94, p < .001). Deliberate rumination was shown to be a significant positive predictor of core beliefs (β = .61; p < .001). The results of the regression analysis are shown in Table 3.
Regression analysis of deliberate and intrusive ruminations as predictors of core beliefs
core beliefs | β | t | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
intrusive rumination | -.30 | -.327 | .744 | |
deliberate rumination | .61** | 6.662 | .000 |
-
Note: **p ≤ 0.001
To analyse research question Q4: Is there a relationship between grief and posttraumatic growth after the loss of a loved one?, we used a correlational analysis. The results of the Pearson correlational analysis show that there was no significant relationship between time since loss and posttraumatic growth (r = -.063; p = .456).
Hypothesis H4 was formulated in the following way: “We assume that posttraumatic growth is higher in individuals who have lost a first-degree family member than in those individuals who have lost a second-degree family member.” The Shapiro-Wilk test of normality showed nonparametric data distribution so we selected a nonparametric U-test for the two independent groups. We compared individuals after the loss of a loved one from a first-degree relationship (loss of a child, parent, spouse, or sibling) and individuals after the loss of a loved one from a second-degree relationship (loss of a friend, uncle, cousin). The
U-test of posttraumatic growth based on the relationship with the deceased
posttraumatic growth | n | Rank Average | U | p | r |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
first degree family member | 97 | 81.57 | 1011.50 | .000 | .04 |
second degree family member | 43 | 45.52 |
-
Note: **p ≤ 0.001
results of the U-test (tab. 4) showed that posttraumatic growth was significantly higher in the group of individuals who had lost a close family member; there was a medium effect size r = .41). Hypothesis H4 was supported.
Hypothesis H5 was formulated in the following way: We assume that individuals experiencing a sudden loss experience higher posttraumatic growth than individuals experiencing the anticipated loss of a loved one. Based on the results of the Shapiro-Wilk test of normality, the distribution of data in the groups was parametric, so we used a parametrical T-test. Based on the findings (Table 5), we can say that posttraumatic growth was significantly higher in the group of people who had lost a loved one due to natural sudden loss. There was a medium effect size (d = .520). Hypothesis H5 was supported.
Comparative T– test of posttraumatic growth based on the type of loss
posttraumatic growth | n | Mean | SD | t | df | p | d |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
sudden loss | 93 | 62.28 | 24.70 | 2.29 | 140 | .025 | .52 |
anticipated loss | 47 | 50.55 | 30.42 |
-
Note: **p ≤ 0.001
In hypothesis H6 we formulated the assumption that the highest posttraumatic growth would be in the domains relating to others and appreciation of life. We performed an error bars graph which shows differences between the individual means of the subscales based on a 95% confidence interval. Based on Figure 1 and the means of each subscale, we can conclude that the highest posttraumatic growth was found in the domains of appreciation of life (M = 3.26; SD = 1.44) and relating to others (M = 2.95; SD = 1.35). The lowest posttraumatic growth was found in the domain of spiritual change (M = 2.41; SD = 1.67). Personal strength (M = 2.71; SD = 1.35) and new possibilities (M = 2.52; SD = 1.47) had medium posttraumatic growth values. There were visible differences between the domains of posttraumatic growth.

Differences in domains of posttraumatic growth
Discussion
The main aim of this study was to explore posttraumatic growth after various types of loss in relation to ruminations and core beliefs. We will discuss our findings in the context of previous research findings and their implications.
Posttraumatic growth in relation to ruminations and core beliefs
In several research studies (e.g. Losavio et al., 2011; Taku & Oshio, 2015; Triplett et al., 2012), posttraumatic growth was related only to deliberate ruminations. In the current study we decided to examine the relationship between posttraumatic growth and intrusive ruminations as well, as it seems to be inconsistent and many studies have found that intrusive rumination relates only to negative consequences, such as distress and anxiety (e.g. Calhoun et al., 2000; Cann et al., 2010b). Our results show that although both deliberate and intrusive ruminations were related to posttraumatic growth, only deliberate ruminations positively predicted posttraumatic growth. This finding may be explained by the fact that deliberate ruminations includes the process of finding meaning and learning from the experience, which may help individuals to find benefits in their loss. When an individual can control his or her thoughts about the loss, he or she may be more able to cope with the loss, which may eventually lead to higher posttraumatic growth.
Next, we hypothesized that the disruption of core beliefs would positively predict posttraumatic growth, for which support has already been found and is consistent with previous research studies (Cann et al., 2010a, 2010b; Lindstrom et al., 2013). We can therefore say that higher disruption of core beliefs is related to more intensive posttraumatic growth after the loss and that rebuilding core beliefs is key to posttraumatic growth occurring.
The results showed that both deliberate and intrusive ruminations were positively related to core beliefs; however, core beliefs were positively predicted by deliberate ruminations only. Similarly, previous research findings (Lindstrom et al., 2013; Linley & Joseph, 2004; Triplett et al., 2012; Zhou et al., 2015) have shown a stronger relationship between core beliefs and deliberate ruminations compared to intrusive ruminations. This finding can be explained as follows: deliberate thinking about the meaning of loss and the effort to understand the loss and its consequences can support the rebuilding of core beliefs about the world and the role the individual plays in it. Thus, ideally, the frequency and intrusive nature of ruminations should diminish over time, and the individual, by increasing the level of deliberate rumination, reconstructs their view of the world, allowing him or her to find a sense of meaning and perceive positive changes (Tedeschi & Calhoun, 2006).
Our findings suggest that grieving is a long-term process of adaptation to loss that is positively related to posttraumatic growth. This finding is similar to some previous research findings (Büchi et al., 2007; Ens & Bond, 2005; Hirooka et al., 2017; Taku et al., 2008), but contradicts studies showing no relationship between posttraumatic growth and grief (e.g. Caserta et al., 2009; Martinčeková & Klatt, 2017; Yilmaz & Zara, 2016). The inconsistent results of different studies may be related to frequent uneven distribution of research samples, including different types of losses, wide ranges in time since loss and other factors. To state with greater certainty what the nature of the relationship is we would ideally need a longitudinal research design and to assess grief and posttraumatic growth at various points in time.
Factors of loss in relation to posttraumatic growth
The partial aim of this study is to explore posttraumatic growth in relation to several factors of death, such as type of loss, relationship with the deceased, and time elapsed since loss. Based on previous research findings, it was hypothesized that posttraumatic growth would be higher in individuals who had lost a first-degree family member than in individuals who had lost a second-degree family member. Our results indicate highest posttraumatic growth occurred in individuals after the loss of a child, parent, spouse, or sibling. These results are similar to previous research findings (Armstrong & Shakespeare-Finch, 2011; Cofini et al., 2014; Ogińská-Bulik, 2014) showing higher posttraumatic growth in individuals who had lost a close family member than in those who had lost a friend or a member of the wider family. Consistent with the views of other authors (e.g. Cofini et al., 2014; Ogińska-Bulik, 2014), this result may be due to the fact that closer family members may have been more involved in the participants’ lives, had a closer relationship, and therefore the loss of this relationship may have affected this individual more, leading them to feel more intense grief, experience higher disruption of core beliefs, and then experience even higher posttraumatic growth.
Posttraumatic growth is a process which needs some time to develop; nonetheless, the results regarding posttraumatic growth and time since loss relationship are inconsistent (Cofini et al., 2014; Yilmaz & Zara, 2016). We found no relationship between time since loss and posttraumatic growth, similarly to a study by Caserta et al. (2009). Our results may have been affected by the wider range of time from loss in our participants and the heterogeneity of the group of participants. For future studies it might be an idea to set a limit on time elapsed since loss and to have more evenly distributed groups. Another explanation for this result could be that other factors, such as relationship with the deceased and type of loss, are more important in explaining posttraumatic growth than time since loss. This can be seen in our results showing that posttraumatic growth was higher in individuals after the loss of a first-degree family member. An alternative explanation might be the fact that posttraumatic growth can occur at various points in time after loss and need not be dependent on time since loss.
The last factor we examined was whether there were any differences in posttraumatic growth based on type of loss, that is, on whether the loss was sudden or anticipated. The results indicated higher posttraumatic growth in individuals after a sudden loss than after an anticipated loss. This supports previous findings in the literature that higher posttraumatic growth occurs in individuals after sudden, and especially violent loss (Currier, Holland, & Neimeyer, 2006; Currier et al., 2013. The explanation might be that sudden and violent losses have a particularly disruptive effect on the individual’s core beliefs (Calhoun et al., 2010) because they are not consistent with natural expectations about how the world functions, and therefore they may lead to higher meaning-making.
Posttraumatic growth is experienced as perceiving various positive changes due to the loss; however, this does not mean that the individual will be happier or see the loss as a positive event in his or her life. It is more about the fact that this negative or traumatic event can be overcome in a way that makes the individual stronger, more resilient and able to face other crisis and challenges in life. We used the PTGI questionnaire (Tedeschi & Calhoun, 1996) to measure positive changes in the following domains: relating to others; new possibilities; personal strength; spiritual change; and appreciation of life. The results of our study show that the highest posttraumatic growth was found in the appreciation of life and relating to others domains, which is similar to the findings of previous studies (Armstrong & Shakespeare-Finch, 2011; Ogińska-Bulik, 2014). Armstrong and Shakespeare-Finch (2011) say that any type of bereavement, regardless of the nature and closeness of the relationship, will result in the person thinking about his or her existence, which can eventually lead to a greater appreciation of one’s life. Similarly, the death of a loved one can lead to relationships with others being perceived with higher importance, as the person may realize how fragile life and relationships with others are. Another explanation could be the fact that bereaved individuals often try to find support, intimacy and closeness with people around them (Cofini et al., 2014) and therefore relationships with others may become more important. The lowest posttraumatic growth was found in the domain relating to spiritual change, as in the study by Armstrong and Shakespeare-Finch (2011). This may be related to the fact that loss of a loved one often threatens or weakens the person’s religious and spiritual beliefs.
It is important to interpret the results of our study bearing in mind some limitations. First, the low percentage of men and the nonhomogeneous groups prevents us from generalizing our findings on the target group of bereaved individuals. Second, the data was obtained through the internet, which has advantages such as providing anonymity and time and space to fill in the questionnaire; however, participants may not understand some of the questionnaire items. Moreover, quantitative research does not allow us to explore the experience of grief and posttraumatic growth more deeply. Therefore we recommend future research use qualitative methodology or mixed-methods research. Despite the above-mentioned limitations, we consider our findings beneficial for future research and praxis.
Conclusion
The results of our study show that posttraumatic growth was related to both deliberate and intrusive ruminations, but was positively predicted by deliberate ruminations only. It also showed that disruption of core beliefs positively predicted posttraumatic growth. Core beliefs disruption was related to both types of ruminations, but was predicted by deliberate ruminations only. The results of our study may be beneficial to grief counsellors and therapists. Grief counselling should not just be aimed at eliminating the negative consequences and negative emotions caused by bereavement because these can serve as a stimulus for positive change and possible posttraumatic growth. Deliberate ruminations can help an individual to think about the loss and its consequences, restore core beliefs, and find meaning in the loss, and therefore they can lead to possible posttraumatic growth.
References
Armstrong, D., & Shakespeare-Finch, J. (2011). Relationship to the bereaved and perceptions of severity of trauma differentiate elements of posttraumatic growth. Omega 63(2), 125–140.10.2190/OM.63.2.bSuche in Google Scholar
Büchi, S., Mörgeli, H., Schyder, U., Jenewein, J., Hepp, U., Jina, E., Neuhaus, R., Fauchère, J.C., Bucher, H.U., & Sensky, T. (2007). Grief and post-traumatic growth in parents 2–6 years after the death of their extremely premature baby. Psychother Psychosom 76, 106–114.10.1159/000097969Suche in Google Scholar
Burnett, P., Middleton, W., Raphael, B., & Martinek, N. (1997). Measuring core bereavement phenomena. Psychological Medicine 27, 49–57.10.1017/S0033291796004151Suche in Google Scholar
Calhoun, L.G., Cann, A., Tedeschi, R.G., & McMillan, J. (2000). A correlational test of the relationship between posttraumatic growth, religion and cognitive processing. Journal of Traumatic Stress 13(3), 521–527.10.1023/A:1007745627077Suche in Google Scholar
Calhoun,L.G., & Tedeschi, R.G. (2001). Posttraumatic growth: The positive lessons of loss. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.10.1037/10397-008Suche in Google Scholar
Calhoun, L.G & Tedeschi, R.G. (2006). The Foundations of Posttraumatic Growth: An Expanded Framework. In L.G. Calhoun, & R. G. Tedeschi (Eds.), Handbook of posttraumatic growth: Research and practice (pp. 3–23). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Suche in Google Scholar
Calhoun, L.G, Tedeschi, R.G., Cann A., & Hanks, E.A. (2010). Positive outcomes following bereavement: Paths to posttraumatic growth. Psychologica Belgica 50(1-2), 125–143.10.5334/pb-50-1-2-125Suche in Google Scholar
Cann, A., Calhoun, L.G., Tedeschi, R.G., & Solomon, D.T. (2010a). Posttraumatic growth and depreciation as independent experiences and predictors of well-being Journal of Loss and Trauma 15(3), 151–166.10.1080/15325020903375826Suche in Google Scholar
Cann, A., Calhoun, L.G., Tedeschi, R.G., Kilmer, R.P., Gil-Rivas, V., Vishnevsky, T., & Danhauer, S.C. (2010b). The core beliefs inventory: A brief measure of disruption in the assumptive world. Anxiety, Stress & Coping 23(1), 19–34.10.1080/10615800802573013Suche in Google Scholar
Cann, A., Calhoun, L.G., Tedeschi, R.G., Triplett, K.N., Vishnevsky, T., & Lindstrom, C.M. (2011). Assessing posttraumatic cognitive processes: The event related rumination inventory. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping 24(2), 137–156.10.1080/10615806.2010.529901Suche in Google Scholar
Caserta, M., Lund, D., Utz, R., & Vries, B. (2009). Stress-related growth among the recently bereaved. Aging and Mental Health 13(3), 463–476.10.1080/13607860802534641Suche in Google Scholar
Cofini, V., Cecilia, M.R., Petrarca, F., Bernardi, R., Mazza, M., & DiOrio, F. (2014). Factors associated with post-traumatic growth after the loss of a loved one. Minerva psichiatrica 55(4), 207–214.Suche in Google Scholar
Currier, J.M., Holland, J.M., & Neimeyer, R.A. (2006). Sense-making, grief, and the experience of violent loss: Toward a mediational model. Death studies 30(5), 403–28.10.1080/07481180600614351Suche in Google Scholar
Currier, J.M., Mallot, J., Martinez, T.E., Sandy, C., & Neimeyer, R.A. (2013). Bereavement, religion, and posttraumatic growth: A matched control group investigation. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality 5(2), 69–77.10.1037/a0027708Suche in Google Scholar
Ens, C., & Bond, J.B. (2005). Death anxiety and personal growth in adolescents experiencing the death of a grandparent. Death Studies 29(2), 171–178.10.1080/07481180590906192Suche in Google Scholar
Hirooka, K., Fukahori, H., Ozawa, M., & Akita, Y. (2017). Differences in posttraumatic growth and grief reactions among adolescents by relationship with the deceased. Journal of Advanced Nursing 73(4), 955–965.10.1111/jan.13196Suche in Google Scholar
Janoff-Bulman, R. (2006). Schema-change perspectives on posttraumatic growth. In L. G. Calhoun & R. G. Tedeschi (Eds.), Handbook of posttraumatic growth: Research and practice (pp. 81–99). Mahwah, NJ: Routledge.Suche in Google Scholar
Lindstrom, C.M., Cann, A., Calhoun, L.G., Tedeschi, R.G. (2013). The relationship of core belief challenge, rumination, disclosure, and sociocultural elements to posttraumatic growth. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy 5(1), 50–55.10.1037/a0022030Suche in Google Scholar
Linley, P.A., & Joseph, S. (2004). Positive change following trauma and adversity: A review. Journal of Traumatic Stress 17(1), 11–21.10.1023/B:JOTS.0000014671.27856.7eSuche in Google Scholar
Losavio, T.S., Cohen, H.L., Laurenceau, J.P., Dasch, K.B., Parrish, B.P., & Park, C.L. (2011). Reports of stress-related growth from daily negative events. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 30(7), 760–785.10.1521/jscp.2011.30.7.760Suche in Google Scholar
Martinčeková, L., & Klatt, J. (2017). Mothers’ grief, forgiveness, and posttraumatic growth after the loss of a child. Omega – Journal of Death and Dying 75(3), 1–18.10.1177/0030222816652803Suche in Google Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2001). Ruminative coping and adjustment to bereavement. In M.S. Stroebe, R.O. Hansson, W. Stroebe, & H. Schut (Eds.), Handbook of bereavement research: Consequences, coping, and care (pp. 54–562). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.10.1037/10436-023Suche in Google Scholar
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Davis, C.C. (2004). Theoretical and methodological issues in the assessment and interpretation of posttraumatic growth. Psychological Inquiry 15(1), 60–64.Suche in Google Scholar
Ogińska-Bulik, N. (2014). The role of temperament in posttraumatic growth following death of a loved one. Polish Psychological Bulletin 45(3), 357–362.10.2478/ppb-2014-0043Suche in Google Scholar
Ramos, C., & Leal, L. (2013). Posttraumatic growth in the aftermath of trauma: A literature review about related factors and application contexts. Psychology, Community & Health 2(1), 43–54.10.5964/pch.v2i1.39Suche in Google Scholar
Stroebe, M., Schut, H. (1999). The Dual Process Model of Coping With Bereavement: Rationale and Description. Death Studies 23(3), 197-224.10.1080/074811899201046Suche in Google Scholar
Taku, K., Calhoun, L.G., Cann, A., & Tedeschi, R.G. (2008). The role of rumination in the coexistence of distress and posttraumatic growth among bereaved Japanese university students. Death Studies 32(1), 428–444.10.1080/07481180801974745Suche in Google Scholar
Taku, K., & Oshio, A. (2015). An item-level analysis of the posttraumatic growth inventory: relationship with an examination of core beliefs and deliberate rumination. Personality and Individual Differences 86, 156–160.10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.025Suche in Google Scholar
Taku, K., Tedeschi, R.G., & Cann, A. (2015). Relationships of posttraumatic growth and stress responses in bereaved young adults. Journal of Loss and Trauma 20(1), 56–71.10.1080/15325024.2013.824306Suche in Google Scholar
Tedeschi, R.G., & Calhoun, L.G. (1996). Posttraumatic growth inventory: measuring the positive legacy of trauma. Journal of Traumatic Stress 9(3), 455–471.10.1002/jts.2490090305Suche in Google Scholar
Tedeschi, R.G., Park, C.L., & Calhoun, L.G. (1998). Posttraumatic growth: Positive changes in the aftermath of crisis Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Suche in Google Scholar
Tedeschi, R.G., & Calhoun, L.G. (2004). Posttraumatic growth: Conceptual foundations and empirical evidence. Psychological Inquiry 15(1), 1–18.10.1207/s15327965pli1501_01Suche in Google Scholar
Tedeschi, R.G., & Calhoun, L.G. (2006). Time of change? The spiritual challenges of bereavement and loss. Omega 53, 105–116.10.2190/7MBU-UFV9-6TJ6-DP83Suche in Google Scholar
Triplett, K.N., Tedeschi, R.G., Cann, A., Calhoun, L.G., & Reeve, C.L. (2012). Posttraumatic growth, meaning in life, and life satisfaction in response to trauma. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy 4, 400–410.10.1037/a0024204Suche in Google Scholar
Yilmaz, M., & Zara, A. (2016). Traumatic loss and posttraumatic growth: The effect of traumatic loss related factors on posttraumatic growth. Anatolian Journal of psychiatry 17(1), 5–11.10.5455/apd.188311Suche in Google Scholar
Zhou, X., Wu, X., Fu, F., & An, Y. (2015). Core belief challenge and rumination as predictors of PTSD and PTG among adolescent survivors of the Wenchuan earthquake. Psychology of Trauma 7(4), 391–397.10.1037/tra0000031Suche in Google Scholar
© 2020 Institute for Research in Social Communication, Slovak Academy of Sciences
Artikel in diesem Heft
- Introduction: Self-esteem and social esteem: Normative issues
- Self-esteem and social esteem: Is Adam Smith right?
- The implications of the loss of self-respect for the recovery model in mental healthcare
- Epistemic self-esteem of philosophers in the face of philosophical disagreement
- Georg Misch’s A History of Autobiography and the problem of self-esteem
- Esteem and self-esteem as an interweaving polarity. Max Weber´s analysis from the Protestant ethic to the ideal-type of politician
- Consensual Qualitative Research on Free Associations for Criticism and Self-Criticism
- More than a medical condition: Qualitative analysis of media representations of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease
- Posttraumatic growth after the loss of a loved one in relation to ruminations and core beliefs
- Approaches to subjective poverty in economic and sociological research
- Phenomenology in Central Europe: Philosophy from the margins
- The anthropological foundations of Buber’s cosmic vision of dialogical life
- Discontent as motivation: Why people engage with the democratic process
- “Murdered Mozarts.” narrative of a previous malian student generation in the era of the crumbling state
- Philosophy, politics and religion: Origins of environmental ethics
Artikel in diesem Heft
- Introduction: Self-esteem and social esteem: Normative issues
- Self-esteem and social esteem: Is Adam Smith right?
- The implications of the loss of self-respect for the recovery model in mental healthcare
- Epistemic self-esteem of philosophers in the face of philosophical disagreement
- Georg Misch’s A History of Autobiography and the problem of self-esteem
- Esteem and self-esteem as an interweaving polarity. Max Weber´s analysis from the Protestant ethic to the ideal-type of politician
- Consensual Qualitative Research on Free Associations for Criticism and Self-Criticism
- More than a medical condition: Qualitative analysis of media representations of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease
- Posttraumatic growth after the loss of a loved one in relation to ruminations and core beliefs
- Approaches to subjective poverty in economic and sociological research
- Phenomenology in Central Europe: Philosophy from the margins
- The anthropological foundations of Buber’s cosmic vision of dialogical life
- Discontent as motivation: Why people engage with the democratic process
- “Murdered Mozarts.” narrative of a previous malian student generation in the era of the crumbling state
- Philosophy, politics and religion: Origins of environmental ethics