Abstract
In this article, we introduce the effect of “constructional contamination”. In constructional contamination, a subset of the instances of a target construction deviate in their realization, due to a superficial resemblance they share with instances of a contaminating construction. We claim that this contaminating effect bears testimony to the hypothesis that language users do not always execute a full parse while interpreting and producing sentences. Instead, they may rely on what has been called “shallow parsing”, i. e., chunking the utterances into large, unanalyzed exemplars that may extend across constituent borders. We propose several measures to quantify constructional contamination in corpus data. To evaluate these measures, the Dutch partitive genitive is taken under scrutiny as a target construction of constructional contamination. In this case study, it is shown that neighboring constructions play a crucial role in determining the presence or absence of the -s suffix among instances of the partitive genitive. The different measures themselves, however, are not construction-specific, and can readily be used to track constructional contamination in other case studies as well.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Hendrik De Smet and Lauren Fonteyn for insightful discussions about the nature of constructional contamination, Kris Heylen, Dirk Speelman and Eline Zenner for methodological assistance, and Judith Cappaert for participating in data coding. Additionally, we owe thanks to two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments and suggestions, and to the editor-in-chief, Hubert Cuyckens, as well as to the guest editors of this issue, Karolina Krawczak, Martin Hilpert and Malgorzata Fabiszak, for editorial assistance and useful advice.
References
Abbot-Smith, Kirsten & Heike Behrens. 2006. How known constructions influence the acquisition of other constructions: The German passive and future constructions. Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal of Artificial Intelligence, Linguistics, Neuroscience, Philosophy, Psychology 30(6). 995–1026.10.1207/s15516709cog0000_61Suche in Google Scholar
Anthony, Laurence. 2011. AntConc (Computer Software, version 3.3.3). Tokyo: Waseda University. http://www.antlab.sci.waseda.ac.jp/.Suche in Google Scholar
Arnon, Inbal & Neal Snider. 2010. More than words: Frequency effects for multi-word phrases. Journal of Memory and Language 62(1). 67–82.10.1016/j.jml.2009.09.005Suche in Google Scholar
Baayen, Rolf Harald. 2008. Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics using R. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511801686Suche in Google Scholar
Balk-Smit Duyzentkunst, Frida. 2000. Grammatica van het Nederlands [Grammar of Dutch]. Den Haag: Sdu.Suche in Google Scholar
Bartol, Thomas, Cailey Bromer, Justin Kinney, Michael Chirillo, Jennifer Bourne, Kristen Harris & Terrence Sejnowski. 2015. Nanoconnectomic upper bound on the variability of synaptic plasticity. eLife 4. e10778.10.7554/eLife.10778Suche in Google Scholar
Bates, Douglas, Martin Maechler, Ben Bolker & Steven Walker. 2013. lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.0-4. http://cran.r-project.org/package=lme4.Suche in Google Scholar
Bauer, Laurie. 1983. English word-formation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781139165846Suche in Google Scholar
Bergs, Alexander & Lena Heine. 2010. Mood and modality in English. In Rolf Thieroff & Björn Rothstein (eds.), Mood systems in the languages of Europe, 103–117. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/slcs.120.06berSuche in Google Scholar
Booij, Geert. 2010. Construction morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1111/j.1749-818X.2010.00213.xSuche in Google Scholar
Broekhuis, Hans. 2013. Syntax of Dutch: Adjectives and adjective phrases. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.10.1515/9789048522255Suche in Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan. 2010. Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511750526Suche in Google Scholar
Chang, Winston. 2014. extrafont: Tools for using fonts. http://cran.r-project.org/package=extrafont.Suche in Google Scholar
Croft, William. 2001. Radical construction grammar: Syntactic theory in typological perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198299554.001.0001Suche in Google Scholar
Dąbrowska, Ewa. 2012. Different speakers, different grammars: Individual differences in native language attainment. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 2(3). 219–253.10.1075/lab.2.3.01dabSuche in Google Scholar
Dąbrowska, Ewa. 2014. Recycling utterances: A speaker’s guide to sentence processing. Cognitive Linguistics 25(4). 617–653.10.1515/cog-2014-0057Suche in Google Scholar
Dąbrowska, Ewa. 2015. Language in the mind and in the community. In Jocelyne Daems, Eline Zenner, Kris Heylen & Dirk Speelman (eds.), Change of paradigms – new paradoxes: Recontextualizing language andlLinguistics. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.Suche in Google Scholar
De Smet, Hendrik. 2010. Grammatical interference: Subject marker for and the phrasal verb particles out and forth. In Elizabeth Trousdale & Graeme Traugott (eds.), Gradience, gradualness and grammaticalization, 75–104. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/tsl.90.06desSuche in Google Scholar
De Smet, Hendrik, Lobke Ghesquière & Freek Van de Velde (eds.). 2013. On multiple source constructions in language change. [Special issue] Studies in Language 37(3).10.1075/sl.37.3.01intSuche in Google Scholar
De Smet, Hendrik & Freek Van de Velde. 2013. Serving two masters: Form–function friction in syntactic amalgams. Studies in Language 37(3). 534–565.10.1075/bct.79.04desSuche in Google Scholar
De Smet, Hendrik & Freek Van de Velde. 2014. Travelling features: Multiple sources, multiple destinations. Paper presented at The 8th International Conference on Construction Grammar (ICCG8), University of Osnabrück, 2–6 September.Suche in Google Scholar
Diessel, Holger. 2007. Frequency effects in language acquisition, language use, and diachronic change. New Ideas in Psychology 25. 108–127.10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.02.002Suche in Google Scholar
Diessel, Holger. 2015. Usage-based construction grammar. In Ewa Dąbrowska & Dagmar Divjak (eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics, 296–321. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.10.1515/9783110292022-015Suche in Google Scholar
Ferreira, Fernanda & Nikole Patson. 2007. The “good enough” approach to language comprehension. Language and Linguistics Compass 1. 71–83.10.1111/j.1749-818X.2007.00007.xSuche in Google Scholar
Fonteyn, Lauren & Nikki van de Pol. 2016. Divide and conquer: The formation and functional dynamics of the Modern English ing-clause network. English Language and Linguistics 20(2). 185–219.10.1017/S1360674315000258Suche in Google Scholar
Fox, John. 2003. Effect displays in R for generalised linear models. Journal of Statistical Software 8. 1–27.10.18637/jss.v008.i15Suche in Google Scholar
Goldberg, Adele Eva. 1995. Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago press.Suche in Google Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th. 2013a. Statistics for linguistics with R: A practical introduction, 2nd edn. Berlin & New York: De Gruyter.Suche in Google Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th. 2013b. 50-something years of work on collocations: What is or should be next. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 18(1). 137–165.10.1075/bct.74.07griSuche in Google Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th. 2014. Coll.analysis 3.5. A script for R to compute perform collostructional analyses.Suche in Google Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th. & Anatol Stefanowitsch. 2004. Extending collostructional analysis: A corpus-based perspective on “alternations.” International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 9(1). 97–130.10.1075/ijcl.9.1.06griSuche in Google Scholar
Grondelaers, Stefan, Katrien Deygers, Hilde Van Aken, Vicky Van den Heede & Dirk Speelman. 2000. Het CONDIV-corpus geschreven Nederlands [The CONDIV-corpus of written Dutch]. Nederlandse Taalkunde 5(4). 356–363.Suche in Google Scholar
Haeseryn, Walter, Kirsten Romijn, Guido Geerts, Jaap de Rooij & Maarten van den Toorn. 1997. Algemene Nederlandse Spraakkunst [General Dutch Grammar]. Groningen: Nijhoff.Suche in Google Scholar
Halliday, Michael & Christian Matthiessen. 2004. An introduction to functional grammar, 3rd edn. London: London Arnold.Suche in Google Scholar
Harrell, Frank. 2013. rms: Regression modeling strategies. R package version 4.0-0. http://cran.r-project.org/package=rms.Suche in Google Scholar
Heine, Bernd. 2002. On the role of context in grammaticalization. In Ilse Wisher & Gabriele Diewald (eds.), New reflections on grammaticalization, 83–101. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/tsl.49.08heiSuche in Google Scholar
Hoeksema, Jack. 2014. De opkomst van “aan” als verbindend element in maatnomenconstructies [The rise of “aan” as a connecting element in measure noun constructions]. In Freek Van de Velde, Hans Smessaert, Frank Van Eynde & Sara Verbrugge (eds.), Patroon en argument: Een dubbelfeestbundel bij het emeritaat van William Van Belle en Joop van der Horst [Pattern and argument: A double festschrift on the occasion of William Van Belle’s and Joop van der Horst’s retirement], 421–432. Leuven: Leuven University Press.10.2307/j.ctt14jxsr0.31Suche in Google Scholar
Hopper, Paul. 1987. Emergent grammar. Berkeley Linguistic Society 13. 139–157.10.3765/bls.v13i0.1834Suche in Google Scholar
Hopper, Paul. 1998. Emergent grammar. The new psychology of language: Cognitive and functional approaches to language structure, 155–175. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.10.4324/9781315085678-6Suche in Google Scholar
Horst, Joop van der. 2008. Geschiedenis van de Nederlandse syntaxis [History of Dutch syntax]. Leuven: Universitaire Pers Leuven.Suche in Google Scholar
Hothorn, Torsten, Peter Bühlmann, Sandrine Dudoit, Annette Molinaro & Mark Van Der Laan. 2006. Survival ensembles. Biostatistics 7(3). 355–373.10.1093/biostatistics/kxj011Suche in Google Scholar
Huddleston, Rodney. 2002. The verb. In Rodney Huddleston & Geoffrey Pullum (eds.), The Cambridge grammar of the English language, 71–212. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/9781316423530.004Suche in Google Scholar
Hüning, Matthias. 1999. Woordensmederij: De geschiedenis van het suffix -erij [Word forging: The history of the suffix -erij]. The Hague: The Hague Holland Academic Graphics.Suche in Google Scholar
Hunston, Susan & Geoff Thompson (eds.). 2001. Evaluation in text: Authorial stance and the construction of discourse. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oso/9780198238546.001.0001Suche in Google Scholar
Jurafsky, Daniel. 1992. An on-line computational model of human sentence interpretation: A theory of the representation and use of linguistic knowledge. Berkeley, CA: University of California dissertation.10.21236/ADA604298Suche in Google Scholar
Lakoff, George. 1988 [1974]. Syntactic amalgams. In Eric Schiller, Barbara Need, Douglas Varley & William Eilfort (eds.), The best of CLS: A selection of out-of-print papers from 1968 to 1975, 25–45. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Suche in Google Scholar
Lakoff, George. 1987. Women, fire, and dangerous things. What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.10.7208/chicago/9780226471013.001.0001Suche in Google Scholar
Landis, John Richard & Gary Grove Koch. 1977. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics 33(1). 159–174.10.2307/2529310Suche in Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald W. 2008. Cognitive grammar: A basic introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195331967.001.0001Suche in Google Scholar
Markman, Ellen & Gwyn Wachtel. 1988. Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology 20(2). 121–157.10.1016/0010-0285(88)90017-5Suche in Google Scholar
Markman, Ellen, Judith Wason & Mikkel Hansen. 2003. Use of the mutual exclusivity assumption by young word learners. Cognitive Psychology 47(3). 241–275.10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00034-3Suche in Google Scholar
Martin, James & Peter White. 2007. The language of evaluation: Appraisal in English. Houndmills: Palgrave Macmillan.Suche in Google Scholar
Norde, Muriel. 2014. On parents and peers in constructional networks. Paper presented as CoglingDays 6. University of Ghent, December 12.Suche in Google Scholar
Oostdijk, Nelleke, Wim Goedertier, Frank Van Eynde, Louis Boves, Jean-Pierre Martens, Michael Moortgat & Harald Baayen. 2002. Experiences from the spoken Dutch corpus project. Proceedings of the third International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation, 340–347. Las Palmas. http://www.lrec-conf.org/proceedings/lrec2002/.Suche in Google Scholar
Pijpops, Dirk & Freek Van de Velde. 2014. A multivariate analysis of the partitive genitive in Dutch: Bringing quantitative data into a theoretical discussion. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory. Published online, ahead of print.Suche in Google Scholar
Pijpops, Dirk & Freek Van de Velde. 2016. Ethnolect speakers and Dutch partitive adjectival inflection: A corpus analysis. Taal en Tongval 67(2). 343–371.10.5117/TET2015.2.PIJPSuche in Google Scholar
R Core Team. 2014. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna. http://www.r-project.org/.Suche in Google Scholar
Speelman, Dirk. 2014. Logistic regression: A confirmatory technique for comparisons in corpus linguistics. In Dylan Glynn & Justyna A. Robinson (eds.), Corpus methods for semantics: Quantitative studies in polysemy and synonymy, 487–533. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/hcp.43.18speSuche in Google Scholar
Strobl, Carolin, Anne-Laure Boulesteix, Thomas Kneib, Thomas Augustin & Achim Zeileis. 2008. Conditional variable importance for random forests. BMC Bioinformatics 9(307). Available at http://bmcbioinformatics.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/1471-2105-9-307. DOI: 10.1186/1471-2105-9-307.Suche in Google Scholar
Strobl, Carolin, Anne-Laure Boulesteix, Achim Zeileis & Torsten Hothorn. 2007. Bias in random forest variable importance measures: Illustrations, sources and a solution. BMC Bioinformatics 8(25). Available at http://bmcbioinformatics.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/1471-2105-8-25. DOI: 10.1186/1471-2105-8-25.Suche in Google Scholar
Szmrecsanyi, Benedikt. 2005. Language users as creatures of habit: A corpus-linguistic analysis of persistence in spoken English. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory 1(1). 113–150.10.1515/cllt.2005.1.1.113Suche in Google Scholar
Townsend, David & Thomas Bever. 2001. Sentence comprehension: The integration of habits and rules. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/6184.001.0001Suche in Google Scholar
Van Bart, Peter, Johan Kerstens & Arie Sturm. 1998. Grammatica van het Nederlands: Een inleiding [Grammar of Dutch: An introduction]. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.10.5117/9789053562819Suche in Google Scholar
Van de Velde, Freek. 2001. Iets taalkundig(s): Een functioneel georiënteerde analyse van deflexie en de genitiefontwikkeling in het Nederlands [Something linguistic: A functionally oriented analysis of deflexion and the development of the genitive in Dutch]. Leuven: Univerisity of Leuven MA thesis.Suche in Google Scholar
Van de Velde, Freek. 2009. De nominale constituent: Structuur en geschiedenis [The noun phrase. Structure and history]. Leuven: Leuven University Press.Suche in Google Scholar
Van de Velde, Freek. 2014. Degeneracy: The maintenance of constructional networks. In Ronny Boogaart, Timothy Colleman & Gijsbert Rutten (eds.), Extending the scope of Construction Grammar, 141–179. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.10.1515/9783110366273.141Suche in Google Scholar
Van de Velde, Freek, Hendrik De Smet & Lobke Ghesquière. 2013. On multiple source constructions in language change. Studies in language 37(3). 473–489.10.1075/bct.79.01intSuche in Google Scholar
Van de Velde, Freek & Joop van der Horst. 2013. Homoplasy in diachronic grammar. Language Sciences 36(1). 66–77.10.1016/j.langsci.2012.03.020Suche in Google Scholar
Van de Velde, Freek & Fred Weerman. 2014. The resilient nature of adjectival inflection in Dutch. In Petra Sleeman, Freek Van de Velde & Harry Perridon (eds.), Adjectives in Germanic and Romance, 113–145. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/la.212.05velSuche in Google Scholar
Venables, William & Brian Ripley. 2002. Modern applied statistics with S, 4th edn. New York: Springer.10.1007/978-0-387-21706-2Suche in Google Scholar
Verhagen Arie. 2013. Darwin en de ideale taalgebruiker [Darwin and the ideal language user]. In Theo A.J.M. Janssen & Jan Noordegraaf (eds.), Honderd jaar taalwetenschap. Artikelen aangeboden aan Saskia Daalder bij haar afscheid van de Vrije Universiteit [A hundred years of linguistics. Articles presented to Saskia Daalder on the occasion of her retirement from the Free University], 151–162. Amsterdam/Münster: Stichting Neerlandistiek VU/Nodus Publikationen.Suche in Google Scholar
Wickham, Hadley & Romain Francois. 2015. dplyr: A grammar of data manipulation. http://cran.r-project.org/package=dplyr.10.32614/CRAN.package.dplyrSuche in Google Scholar
Zipf, George Kingsley. 1932. Selected studies of the principle of relative frequency in language. Harvard: Harvard University Press.10.4159/harvard.9780674434929Suche in Google Scholar
©2016 by De Gruyter Mouton
Artikel in diesem Heft
- Frontmatter
- Usage-based cognitive-functional linguistics: From theory to method and back again
- The cognitive plausibility of statistical classification models: Comparing textual and behavioral evidence
- The usage and spread of sentence-internal capitalization in Early New High German: A multifactorial approach
- Quantifying polysemy: Corpus methodology for prototype theory
- A cognitive-constructionist approach to Spanish creo Ø and creo yo ‘[I] think’
- A corpus-based, cross-linguistic approach to mental predicates and their complementation: Performativity and descriptivity vis-à-vis boundedness and picturability
- Why we need a token-based typology: A case study of analytic and lexical causatives in fifteen European languages
- Constructional contamination: How does it work and how do we measure it?
- Regular FoL papers
- Mutual intelligibility of spoken Maltese, Libyan Arabic, and Tunisian Arabic functionally tested: A pilot study
- Intermediate information status for non-nominal constituents: Evidence from Spanish secondary predicates in adversatives
- Lower domain language shift in Taiwan: The case of Southern Min
- Book Reviews
- Crespo-Fernández, Eliecer: Sex in language: Euphemistic and dysphemistic metaphors in internet forums
- Sonnenhauser, Barbara and Patrizia Noel Aziz Hanna: Vocative! Addressing between system and performance
- Erratum
- Erratum
Artikel in diesem Heft
- Frontmatter
- Usage-based cognitive-functional linguistics: From theory to method and back again
- The cognitive plausibility of statistical classification models: Comparing textual and behavioral evidence
- The usage and spread of sentence-internal capitalization in Early New High German: A multifactorial approach
- Quantifying polysemy: Corpus methodology for prototype theory
- A cognitive-constructionist approach to Spanish creo Ø and creo yo ‘[I] think’
- A corpus-based, cross-linguistic approach to mental predicates and their complementation: Performativity and descriptivity vis-à-vis boundedness and picturability
- Why we need a token-based typology: A case study of analytic and lexical causatives in fifteen European languages
- Constructional contamination: How does it work and how do we measure it?
- Regular FoL papers
- Mutual intelligibility of spoken Maltese, Libyan Arabic, and Tunisian Arabic functionally tested: A pilot study
- Intermediate information status for non-nominal constituents: Evidence from Spanish secondary predicates in adversatives
- Lower domain language shift in Taiwan: The case of Southern Min
- Book Reviews
- Crespo-Fernández, Eliecer: Sex in language: Euphemistic and dysphemistic metaphors in internet forums
- Sonnenhauser, Barbara and Patrizia Noel Aziz Hanna: Vocative! Addressing between system and performance
- Erratum
- Erratum