Home Temporality of sense-making in narrative interactions
Article
Licensed
Unlicensed Requires Authentication

Temporality of sense-making in narrative interactions

  • Yanna Popova

    Yanna B. Popova (D. Phil. in Language and Literature, University of Oxford, 2001) has taught at the Universities of Oxford and Birmingham. She was a founding member of the Department of Cognitive Science at Case Western Reserve University, where she worked between 2006 and 2014. Her education has been in linguistics, literary studies, and philosophy, and her main areas of research are in the fields of cognitive poetics, cognitive linguistics, metaphor theory, narratology, and the applications of enactive cognitive science to understanding of works or art and aesthetic reception. She has published articles in Frontiers in Psychology, Language and Literature, Modern Drama, Style, and The Wallace Stevens Journal. She has also contributed to edited volumes on image schemas in cognitive linguistics, post-cognitivist psychology, cognitive poetics, cognitive narratology, and creativity, among others. Her book Stories, Meaning, and Experience: Narrativity and Enaction (Routledge, 2015) has been recently re-issued in paperback format (Routledge, 2018).

    EMAIL logo
    and Elena Cuffari

    Elena Cuffari is an assistant professor of philosophy and program area chair in women’s studies at Worcester State University, where she has worked since 2014. From 2012 to 2014, she was a Marie Curie Experienced Researcher Postdoctoral Fellow in the international network TESIS (Towards an Embodied Science of InterSubjectivity); for this project, she was based at the University of the Basque Country in Donostia/San Sebastian and spent time at the Center for Subjectivity Research at the University of Copenhagen. Elena completed her doctoral dissertation in philosophy, “Co-speech Gestures in Communication and Cognition,” in 2011 at the University of Oregon, advised by Mark Johnson. She has published articles on gesture, metaphor, embodiment, social interaction, meaning, and habits, and on the twentieth century French philosophers Simone de Beauvoir and Maurice Merleau-Ponty in peer-reviewed journals including Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, Hypatia, Metaphor and Symbol, and Frontiers in Psychology. Elena has contributed to edited volumes, most recently including a chapter on hand gesture analysis in Intercorporeality: Emerging Socialities in Interaction (Oxford University Press, 2017) and one on the science of monogamy in New Philosophies of Sex and Love: Thinking Through Desire (Rowman & Littlefield, 2016). She serves on editorial boards for Language Sciences and Frontiers in Psychology. Her current project is a monograph co-authored with Ezequiel Di Paolo and Hanne De Jaegher (of the University of the Basque Country) and expected to appear in the fall of 2018 entitled Linguistic Bodies: The Continuity Between Life and Language.

Published/Copyright: May 30, 2018
Become an author with De Gruyter Brill

Abstract

Human experience is inherently temporal and involves placing events, symbols, and actions in a temporal scheme. This article deals with a specific aspect of temporal experience as it relates to reading and experiencing narrative fiction. Within an enactive understanding of human cognition, we propose a view of literary reading as a process of participatory sense-making between a reader and a storyteller. The enactive theory of participatory sense-making maintains that agents, by enacting their own sense-making, directly and partially constitute the sense-making of other agents. Sense-makers in interaction navigate two orders of normativity: their own and that of the interaction itself. Linguistic sense-making (languaging) opens up further possibilities for understanding complex spatially and temporally distributed forms of social interactions such as narrative interactions. Reading a narrative is one such example of mutually constituted navigation between an interaction dynamic and interactors’ sense-making. The reader completes and co-authors emergent textual meaning and a textually emerging storyteller guides and anticipates the multiple temporal displacements, realized linguistically, that a reader has to experience in the process of reading. We explore the participatory structure of a narrative through its temporal unfolding and the specific, non-linear nature of the temporal dynamics of interacting with a storytelling agency. In particular, narrative interactions are seen as modulations in the pacing of a given narrative’s unfolding. It is suggested that the reader’s enactment of such temporally realized pacings constitutes a better description of narrative immersion than its traditional understanding as a simulation of spatial situatedness.

About the authors

Yanna Popova

Yanna B. Popova (D. Phil. in Language and Literature, University of Oxford, 2001) has taught at the Universities of Oxford and Birmingham. She was a founding member of the Department of Cognitive Science at Case Western Reserve University, where she worked between 2006 and 2014. Her education has been in linguistics, literary studies, and philosophy, and her main areas of research are in the fields of cognitive poetics, cognitive linguistics, metaphor theory, narratology, and the applications of enactive cognitive science to understanding of works or art and aesthetic reception. She has published articles in Frontiers in Psychology, Language and Literature, Modern Drama, Style, and The Wallace Stevens Journal. She has also contributed to edited volumes on image schemas in cognitive linguistics, post-cognitivist psychology, cognitive poetics, cognitive narratology, and creativity, among others. Her book Stories, Meaning, and Experience: Narrativity and Enaction (Routledge, 2015) has been recently re-issued in paperback format (Routledge, 2018).

Elena Cuffari

Elena Cuffari is an assistant professor of philosophy and program area chair in women’s studies at Worcester State University, where she has worked since 2014. From 2012 to 2014, she was a Marie Curie Experienced Researcher Postdoctoral Fellow in the international network TESIS (Towards an Embodied Science of InterSubjectivity); for this project, she was based at the University of the Basque Country in Donostia/San Sebastian and spent time at the Center for Subjectivity Research at the University of Copenhagen. Elena completed her doctoral dissertation in philosophy, “Co-speech Gestures in Communication and Cognition,” in 2011 at the University of Oregon, advised by Mark Johnson. She has published articles on gesture, metaphor, embodiment, social interaction, meaning, and habits, and on the twentieth century French philosophers Simone de Beauvoir and Maurice Merleau-Ponty in peer-reviewed journals including Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, Hypatia, Metaphor and Symbol, and Frontiers in Psychology. Elena has contributed to edited volumes, most recently including a chapter on hand gesture analysis in Intercorporeality: Emerging Socialities in Interaction (Oxford University Press, 2017) and one on the science of monogamy in New Philosophies of Sex and Love: Thinking Through Desire (Rowman & Littlefield, 2016). She serves on editorial boards for Language Sciences and Frontiers in Psychology. Her current project is a monograph co-authored with Ezequiel Di Paolo and Hanne De Jaegher (of the University of the Basque Country) and expected to appear in the fall of 2018 entitled Linguistic Bodies: The Continuity Between Life and Language.

References

Augustine. 1961. The confessions. R. S. Pine-Coffin, trans. London: Penguin.Search in Google Scholar

Auvray, M., C. Lenay & J. Stewart. 2009. Perceptual interactions in a minimalist virtual environment. New Ideas in Psychology 27(1). 32–47.10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.12.002Search in Google Scholar

Auvray, M. & M. Rohde. 2012. Perceptual crossing: The simplest online paradigm. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6. 181.10.3389/fnhum.2012.00181Search in Google Scholar

Barandarian, X., Di Paolo, E. A., and Rohde, M. 2009. Defining Agency: Individuality, normativity, asymmetry, and spatio-temporality in action. Adaptive Behavior 17 (5): 367-386.10.1177/1059712309343819Search in Google Scholar

Bergson, H. 1911/1998. Creative evolution. A. Mitchell, trans. Mineola: Dover Publications.10.5962/bhl.title.166289Search in Google Scholar

Bigelow, J. 2013. The emergence of a new family of theories of time. In H. Dyke & A. Bardon (eds.), A companion to the philosophy of time, 151–166. Oxford: Wiley Blackwell.10.1002/9781118522097.ch10Search in Google Scholar

Butler, S. 1903/1994. The way of all flesh. Hertford: Wordsworth Editions.Search in Google Scholar

Chafe, W. 1994. Discourse, consciousness, and time: The flow and displacement of conscious experience in speaking and writing. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Search in Google Scholar

Conrad, J.. 1907/2002. The secret agent. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1097/00001888-200206000-00010Search in Google Scholar

De Jaegher, H. 2013. Embodiment and sense-making in autism. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 7. 15.10.3389/fnint.2013.00015Search in Google Scholar

De Jaegher, H. & E. Di Paolo. 2007. Participatory sense-making. Phenomenology and The Cognitive Sciences 6. 485–507.10.1007/s11097-007-9076-9Search in Google Scholar

Di Paolo, E. 2009. Extended life. Topoi 28(1). 9–21.10.1007/s11245-008-9042-3Search in Google Scholar

Di Paolo, E., T. Buhrmann & X. Barandiaran. 2017. Sensorimotor life: An enactive proposal. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198786849.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Di Paolo, E., E. Cuffari & H. De Jaegher. Forthcoming. Linguistic bodies: The continuity between life and language. Cambridge: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Di Paolo, E., M. Rohde & H. Iizuka. 2008. Sensitivity to social contingency or stability of interaction? Modelling the dynamics of perceptual crossing. New Ideas in Psychology 26(2). 278–294.10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.07.006Search in Google Scholar

Di Paolo, E. & E. Thompson. 2014. The enactive approach. In L. Shapiro (ed.), The Routledge handbook of embodied cognition, 68–78. London: Routledge Press.Search in Google Scholar

Doerr, A. 2014. All the light we cannot see. London: Fourth Estate.Search in Google Scholar

Donald, M. 2007. The slow process: A hypothetical cognitive adaptation for distributed cognitive networks. Journal of Physiology-Paris 111. 214–222.10.1057/9780230360792_2Search in Google Scholar

Fleischman, S. 1990. Tense and narrativity. London: Routledge.10.7560/780903Search in Google Scholar

Fraisse, P. 1963. The psychology of time. J. Leith, trans. New York & London: Harper & Row.Search in Google Scholar

Froese, T. 2014. Bio-machine hybrid technology: A theoretical assessment and some suggestions for improved future design. Philosophy and Technology 27(4). 539–560.10.1007/s13347-013-0130-ySearch in Google Scholar

Froese, T. & E. Di Paolo. 2008. Stability of coordination requires mutuality of interaction in a model of embodied agents. In International Conference on Simulation of Adaptive Behavior, 52–61. Berlin: Springer.10.1007/978-3-540-69134-1_6Search in Google Scholar

Froese, T. & E. Di Paolo. 2010. Modelling social interaction as perceptual crossing: An investigation into the dynamics of the interaction process. Connection Science 22(1). 43–68.10.1080/09540090903197928Search in Google Scholar

Froese, T. & E. Di Paolo. 2011. The enactive approach: Theoretical sketches from cell to society. Pragmatics and Cognition 19(1). 1–36.10.1075/pc.19.1.01froSearch in Google Scholar

Genette, G. 1980. Narrative discourse: An essay on method. J. E. Lewin, trans. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Givon, T. 1979. On understanding grammar. New York: Academic Press.Search in Google Scholar

Heidegger, M. 1962. Being and time. J. Macquarrie & E. Robinson, trans. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Search in Google Scholar

Heidegger, M. 2008. History of the concept of time. T. Kisial, trans. Bloomington & Indianapolis: Indiana University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Herman, David. 2008. Narrative theory and the intentional stance. Partial Answers 6. 233–260.10.1353/pan.0.0019Search in Google Scholar

Husserl, E.. 1991. On the phenomenology of the consciousness of internal time (1893–1917). J. B. Brough, transDordrecht: Kluwer Academic.10.1007/978-94-011-3718-8Search in Google Scholar

Ingarden, R. 1973. The cognition of the literary work of art. R. A. Crowley & K. R. Olson, trans. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Iser, W. 1978. The act of reading: A theory of aesthetic response. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.10.56021/9780801821011Search in Google Scholar

James, W. 1890. Principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt.10.1037/10538-000Search in Google Scholar

Jaszczolt, K. M. 2009. Representing time: An essay on temporality as modality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oso/9780199214433.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Klein, W. 1994. Time in language. London: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Langacker, R. W. 2012. Linguistic manifestations of the space-time (dis)analogy. In L. Filipovic & K. Jaszczolt (eds.), Space and time in languages and cultures, 191–215. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/hcp.37.15lanSearch in Google Scholar

Lawrence, D. H. 1926/1995. The plumed serpent. Hertford: Wordsworth Editions.Search in Google Scholar

Loaiza, J. 2016. Musicking, embodiment and participatory enaction of music: Outline and key points. Connection Science 28(4). 410–422.10.1080/09540091.2016.1236366Search in Google Scholar

Maiese, M. 2015. Transformative learning, enactivism, and affectivity. Studies in Philosophy and Education. doi: 10.1007/s11217-015-9506-z.Search in Google Scholar

Michael, J. 2011. Interactionism and mindreading. Review of Philosophy and Psychology 2. 559–578.10.1007/s13164-011-0066-zSearch in Google Scholar

Michael, J. & S. Overgaard. 2012. Interaction and social cognition: A commentary on Auvray et al.’s perceptual crossing paradigm. New Ideas in Psychology 30. 296–299.10.1016/j.newideapsych.2012.02.001Search in Google Scholar

Ondatjee, M. 1992. The English patient. London: Bloomsbury.Search in Google Scholar

Popova, Y. 2007. On human temporality — Time, language, and a sense of self. In B. Wallace, A. Ross et al. (eds.), The mind, the body, and the world: Psychology after cognitivism, 257–268. Exeter: Imprint Academic.Search in Google Scholar

Popova, Y. 2014. Narrativity and enaction: The social nature of literary narrative understanding. Frontiers in Psychology 5. 895.10.4324/9781315880488-3Search in Google Scholar

Popova, Y. 2015. Stories, meaning, and experience: Narrativity and enaction. London: Routledge.10.4324/9781315880488Search in Google Scholar

Pöppel, E. 1988. Mindworks: time and conscious experience. T. Artin, trans. Boston: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Search in Google Scholar

Pöppel, E. 1989. Taxonomy of the subjective: An evolutionary perspective. In J. W. Brown (ed.), Neuropsychology of visual perception, 219–232. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.10.4324/9781315441849-11Search in Google Scholar

Prince, G. 2003. A dictionary of narratology. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Search in Google Scholar

Ricoeur, P. 1984. Time and narrative, vol. 1. K. Blamey & D. Pellauer, trans. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.10.7208/chicago/9780226713519.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Ricoeur, P. 1988. Time and narrative, vol. 3. K. Blamey & D. Pellauer, trans. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.10.7208/chicago/9780226713533.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Rohde, M. & E. Di Paolo. 2008. Embodiment and perceptual crossing in 2D. In International Conference on Simulation of Adaptive Behavior, 83–92. Berlin: Springer.10.1007/978-3-540-69134-1_9Search in Google Scholar

Sokolowski, R. 2000. Introduction to phenomenology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Stanzel, F. K. 1984. A theory of narrative. C. Goedsche, trans. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Tallis, R. 2017. Of time and lamentation: Reflections on transience. Newcastle: Agenda Publishing.10.2307/j.ctv5cg85xSearch in Google Scholar

Thompson, E. 2007. Mind in life: Biology, phenomenology and the sciences of mind. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Towers, J. & L. C. Martin. 2015. Enactivism and the study of collectivity. ZDM Mathematics Education 47(2). 247–256.10.1007/s11858-014-0643-6Search in Google Scholar

Van Der Schyff, D., A. Schiavio & D. J. Elliott. 2016. Critical ontology for an enactive music pedagogy. Action, Criticism, and Theory for Music Education 15(5). 81–121.10.22176/act15.5.81Search in Google Scholar

Varela, F. J. 1999. Present-time consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 2–3. 111–140.Search in Google Scholar

Wittman, M. 2016. Felt time: The psychology of how we perceive time. Cambridge: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Zwaan, R. A. 2004. The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In B. H. Ross (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 44, 35–62. San Diego & London: Elsevier Academic Press.10.1016/S0079-7421(03)44002-4Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2018-05-30

© 2018 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 10.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/cogsem-2018-0007/html
Scroll to top button