Home Why Is nice and Adj So Much More Frequent than Adj and nice?—From the Perspective of Humans’ Social and Limited-Processing-Capacity Attributes
Article
Licensed
Unlicensed Requires Authentication

Why Is nice and Adj So Much More Frequent than Adj and nice?—From the Perspective of Humans’ Social and Limited-Processing-Capacity Attributes

  • Jinfang Peng

    Jinfang PENG earned her MA in Applied Linguistics from Guangdong University of Foreign Studies. She is a self-employed researcher and English teacher. Her research efforts have focused on second language acquisition, corpus linguistics, and their application in English teaching.

    EMAIL logo
Published/Copyright: August 14, 2020
Become an author with De Gruyter Brill

Abstract

This paper investigates the phenomenon of imbalance between the frequencies of the nice and Adj and Adj and nice patterns from the perspective of humans’ social and limited-processing-capacity attributes. Humans’ social attribute requires that language users stay informative with minimal effort in communication, resulting in the from-the-least-to-the-most-informative information organization in discourse. Their limited-processing-capacity attribute requires that they resort to the production biases of “easy first” and “plan reuse” in order to achieve communicative efficiency in real-time production. The analysis of the occurrences of the nice and Adj pattern and native speakers’ judgment of the degree of informativeness of nice in these occurrences suggest that nice is largely delexicalized. Such delexicalization makes nice and Adj consistent with the information organization and allows language users to stay informative with the use of the pattern, thus in line with the social attribute, but not Adj and nice. In the meantime, nice is not only highly frequent but also conceptually salient when it comes to the positive property (Panther & Thornburg, 2009), making nice and Adj easier to produce and more likely to be reused than Adj and nice, thus in line with the limited-processing-capacity attribute. The analysis of the unbalanced frequency of the two patterns suggests that human attributes should be considered when studying language form, and this should offer insights into English learning.

About the author

Jinfang Peng

Jinfang PENG earned her MA in Applied Linguistics from Guangdong University of Foreign Studies. She is a self-employed researcher and English teacher. Her research efforts have focused on second language acquisition, corpus linguistics, and their application in English teaching.

Acknowledgments

The author thanks anonymous reviewers for their helpful suggestions on improvement. Thanks also go to Professor Xiaofei Lu for his help with the survey and his comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript.

References

Acheson, D., & MacDonald, M. (2009). Working memory and language production: Common approaches to the serial ordering of verbal information. Psychology Bulletin, 135, 50-68.10.1037/a0014411Search in Google Scholar

Armstrong, S. L., Gleitman, L. R., & Gleitman, H. (1983). What some concepts might not be. Cognition, 13, 263-308.10.1093/oso/9780199828098.003.0019Search in Google Scholar

Becker, G. (1976). The economic approach to human behavior. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.10.7208/chicago/9780226217062.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Benor, S. B., & Levy, R. (2006). The chicken or the egg? A probabilistic analysis of English binomials. Language, 82, 233-277.10.1353/lan.2006.0077Search in Google Scholar

Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., & Cleland, A. A. (2000). Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue. Cognition, 75, B13-25.10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00081-5Search in Google Scholar

Davies, M. (2008-2012). The Corpus of Contemporary American English: 450 million words, 1990-present. Available from http://corpus.byu.edu/coca/Search in Google Scholar

Giora, R. (1988). On the informativeness requirement. Journal of Pragmatics, 12, 547-565.10.1016/0378-2166(88)90048-3Search in Google Scholar

Goldberg, A. E. (1995). Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Search in Google Scholar

Goldberg, A. E. (2006). Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics (pp. 41-58). New York: Academic Press.Search in Google Scholar

Halliday, M. A. K., & Matthiessen, C. (2004). An Introduction to Functional Grammar (3rd ed.). London, UK: Edward Arnold.Search in Google Scholar

Just, M., & Carpenter, P. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122-149.10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.122Search in Google Scholar

Liu, S. (2012). Study on ordering constraints in Chinese and English binomials. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Shanghai International Studies University, Shanghai, China.Search in Google Scholar

MacDonald, M. (2013). How language production shapes language form and comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology, 4. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00226Search in Google Scholar

Malkiel, Y. (1959). Studies in irreversible binomials. Lingua, 8, 113-160.10.1016/0024-3841(59)90018-XSearch in Google Scholar

McLaughlin, B., Rossman, T., & McLeod, B. (1983). Second language learning: An information-processing perspective. Language Learning, 33, 135-158.10.1111/j.1467-1770.1983.tb00532.xSearch in Google Scholar

Mollin, S. (2012). Revisiting binomial order in English: Ordering constraints and reversibility. English Language and Linguistics, 16, 81-103.10.1017/S1360674311000293Search in Google Scholar

Panther, K., & Thornburg, L. (2009). From syntactic coordination to conceptual modification: The case of the nice and Adj construction. Constructions and Frames, 1, 56-86.10.1075/cf.1.1.04panSearch in Google Scholar

Sinclair, J. (1991). Corpus, concordance, collocation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Sinclair, J. (2003). Reading concordances: An introduction. London, UK: Pearson Education.Search in Google Scholar

Sinclair, J. (2004). Trust the text: Language, corpus and discourse. London, UK: Routledge.10.4324/9780203594070Search in Google Scholar

Smith, M., & Wheeldon, L. (2004). Horizontal information flow in spoken sentence production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 675-686.10.1037/0278-7393.30.3.675Search in Google Scholar

Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance: Communication and cognition (2nd ed.). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers.Search in Google Scholar

Swan, M. (2005). Practical English usage (2nd ed.). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Zipf, G. (1949). Human behavior and the principle of least effort. Cambridge, Mass.: Addison-Wesley Press.Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2020-08-14
Published in Print: 2020-06-25

© 2020 FLTRP, Walter de Gruyter, Cultural and Education Section British Embassy

Downloaded on 10.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/CJAL-2020-0015/html
Scroll to top button