Startseite Gradual development of constructional complexity in German spatial language
Artikel
Lizenziert
Nicht lizenziert Erfordert eine Authentifizierung

Gradual development of constructional complexity in German spatial language

  • Karin Madlener ORCID logo EMAIL logo , Katrin Skoruppa und Heike Behrens
Veröffentlicht/Copyright: 17. Oktober 2017

Abstract

In this paper, we assess the developmental trajectories by which children approach adult levels of complexity and informativeness in the linguistically and conceptually challenging domain of spatial language. To this end, we look at three types of spatial relations (localization, spontaneous and caused motion) in spontaneous German child speech (age 2;6 to 2;11 and 4;6 to 4;11), and in elicited Frog Story narratives from German child and adult speakers (3-, 5-, 9-year-olds, and adults. Children are generally sensitive to typological preferences. From early on, their productions reflect target-language-specific lexicalization patterns. Our analyses show that they still approach adult-like levels of information complexity and density only gradually. This concerns the local complexity (structural repertoire for the conceptual slots figure, verb, path/ground), as also established in previous research, but in particular the global complexity, as investigated in this study. Global complexity measures the structural integration of information, or the combinatorial complexity that surfaces at the utterance level. As predicted by usage-based theories, adult-like degrees of informativeness and information density are only reached gradually, although the component parts at the local level are available earlier in development.

Acknowledgements

Part of this research was conducted as part of the project Sprachstandsermittlung bei Kindern mit Migrationshintergrund, funded by the Daimler und Benz Stiftung, Ladenburg, Germany. We thank our students, particularly Sophie Dettwiler and Julia Voegelin, for their help with data coding, and Steve Duman for his thorough comments on the manuscript. Thanks also to two anonymous reviewers and to the Associate Editor of Cognitive Linguistics, as well as to the discussants at the UK Cognitive Linguistics Conference 2016 for their helpful critical feedback. The usual disclaimers apply.

References

Augst, G., A. Bauer & A. Stein. 1977. Grundwortschatz und Ideolekt: Empirische Untersuchungen zur semantischen und lexikalischen Struktur des kindlichen Wortschatzes. Tübingen: Niemeyer.10.1515/9783111630809Suche in Google Scholar

Bamberg, M. 1994. Development of linguistic forms: German. In R. Berman & D. I. Slobin (eds.), Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study, 189–238. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Suche in Google Scholar

Becker, T. 2011. Kinder lernen erzählen. Baltmannsweiler: Schneider Verlag Hohengehren.Suche in Google Scholar

Behrens, H. 2006. The input-output relationship in first language acquisition. Language and Cognitive Processes 21. 2–24.10.1080/01690960400001721Suche in Google Scholar

Behrens, H. 2009. Usage-based and emergentist approaches to language acquisition. Linguistics 47(2). 383–411.10.1515/LING.2009.014Suche in Google Scholar

Berman, R. 2016. Language development and use beyond the sentence. In E. L. Bavin & L. Naigles (eds.), The Cambridge handbook of child language, 2nd edn, 458–480. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781316095829.021Suche in Google Scholar

Berman, R. & D. I. Slobin 1994a. Becoming a native speaker. In R. Berman & D. I. Slobin (eds.), Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study, 611–641. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Suche in Google Scholar

Berman, R. & D. I. Slobin. 1994b. Narrative structure. In R. Berman & D. I. Slobin (eds.), Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study, 39–84. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Suche in Google Scholar

Berman, R. & D. I. Slobin. 1994c. Overview of linguistic forms in the frog stories. In R. Berman & D. I. Slobin (eds.), Relating events in narrative: A crosslinguistic developmental study, 109–126. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Suche in Google Scholar

Bhat, S. & S.-Y. Yoon 2015. Automatic assessment of syntactic complexity for spontaneous speech scoring. Speech Communication 67. 42–57.10.1016/j.specom.2014.09.005Suche in Google Scholar

Bowerman, M. 1982. Reorganizational processes in lexical and syntactic development. In E. Wanner & L. R. Gleitman (eds.), Language acquisition: The state of the art, 319–346. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Bowerman, M. & S. Choi. 2001. Shaping meanings for language: Universal and language-specific in the acquisition of spatial semantic categories. In M. Bowerman & S. C. Levinson (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development, 475–511. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511620669.018Suche in Google Scholar

Brennan, R. L. & D. J. Prediger. 1981. Coefficient κ: Some uses, misuses, and alternatives. Educational and Psychological Measurement 41. 687–699.10.1177/001316448104100307Suche in Google Scholar

Bryant, D. 2012. Lokalisierungsausdrücke im Erst- und Zweitspracherwerb. Typologische, ontogenetische und kognitionspsychologische Überlegungen zur Sprachförderung in DaZ. Baltmannsweiler: Schneider Verlag Hohengehren.Suche in Google Scholar

Bybee, J. 2006. From usage to grammar: The mind’s response to repetition. Language 82. 711–733.10.1353/lan.2006.0186Suche in Google Scholar

Cadierno, T. 2008. Learning to talk about motion in a foreign language. In P. Robinson & N. C. Ellis (eds.), Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition, 239–275. New York & London: Routledge.Suche in Google Scholar

Childers, J. B. & M. Tomasello. 2001. The role of pronouns in young children’s acquisition of the English transitive construction. Developmental Psychology 37(6). 739–748.10.1037/0012-1649.37.6.739Suche in Google Scholar

Core Team, R 2015. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Version 3.2.2. (2015-08-14). R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/.Suche in Google Scholar

Culicover, P. W. 2014. Constructions, complexity, and word order variation. In F. J. Newmeyer & L. B. Preston (eds.), Measuring linguistic complexity, 148–178. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199685301.003.0008Suche in Google Scholar

Dąbrowska, E. 2012. Different speakers, different grammars: Individual differences in native language attainment. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 2(3). 219–253.10.1075/lab.2.3.01dabSuche in Google Scholar

Fenk-Oczlon, G. & A. Fenk. 2008. Complexity trade-offs between the subsystems of language. In M. Miestamo, K. Sinnemäki & F. Karlsson (eds.), Language complexity: Typology, contact, change, 43–65. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.10.1075/slcs.94.05fenSuche in Google Scholar

Fernández, C. 2011. Mindful storytellers: Emerging pragmatics and theory of mind development. First Language 33. 20–46.10.1177/0142723711422633Suche in Google Scholar

Fleischman, S. 1991. Discourse as space/discourse as time: Reflections on the metalanguage of spoken and written discourse. Journal of Pragmatics 16(4). 291–306.10.1016/0378-2166(91)90083-ASuche in Google Scholar

Gentner, D. 1979. On relational meaning. The acquisition of verb meaning. Journal of Child Development 49. 988–998.10.2307/1128738Suche in Google Scholar

Gentner, D. & M. Bowerman. 2009. Why some spatial semantic categories are harder to learn than others: The typological prevalence hypothesis. In J. Guo, E. V. M. Lieven, N. Budwig, S. E.-T. K. Nakamura & S. Özcaliskan (eds.), Crosslinguistic approaches to the psychology of language: Research in the tradition of Dan Isaac Slobin, 465–480. New York: Taylor & Francis.Suche in Google Scholar

Graesser, A., D. S. McNamara, M. Louwerse & Z. Cai. 2004. Coh-Metrix: Analysis of text on cohesion and language. Behavioral Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers 36(2). 193–202.10.3758/BF03195564Suche in Google Scholar

Haggblade, E. 1994. Die Lexikalisierung von semantischen Komponenten in den Bewegungsverben: Vergleichende Untersuchungen zur Semantik von Partikelverben im Englischen und Deutschen. Berlin: Freie Universität Berlin unpublished dissertation.Suche in Google Scholar

Hofmeister, P. 2011. Representational complexity and memory retrieval in language comprehension. Language and Cognitive Processes 26(3). 376–405.10.1080/01690965.2010.492642Suche in Google Scholar

Hsu, H. C. 2017. The effect of task planning on L2 performance and L2 development in text-based synchronous computer-mediated communication. Applied Linguistics 38(3). 359–385.10.1093/applin/amv032Suche in Google Scholar

Ji, Y., H. Hendriks & M. Hickmann. 2011. How children express caused motion events in Chinese and English: Universal and language-specific influences. Lingua 121. 1796–1819.10.1016/j.lingua.2011.07.001Suche in Google Scholar

Juola, P. 2008. Assessing linguistic complexity. In M. Miestamo, K. Sinnemäki & F. Karlsson (eds.), Language complexity: Typology, contact, change, 89–108. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.10.1075/slcs.94.07juoSuche in Google Scholar

Kim, S. 2015. ppcor: Partial and Semi-Partial (Part) Correlation. R package version 1.1. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ppcor (last accessed October 5, 2017).Suche in Google Scholar

Lange, B. 2007. Machen, haben, gehen, kommen: Einige "Passepartout"-Verben im Primärspracherwerb des Deutschen. Frankfurt/Main: Peter Lang.Suche in Google Scholar

MacWhinney, B. 2000. The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. Volume 2: The database. 3rd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Suche in Google Scholar

Mayer, M. 2003 [1969]. Frog, where are you? (A boy, a dog, and a frog). New York: Dial Books.Suche in Google Scholar

Nachtigäller, K., K. J. Rohlfing & K. McGregor. 2013. A story about a word: Does narrative presentation promote learning of a spatial preposition in German two-year-olds? Journal of Child Language 40. 900–917.10.1017/S0305000912000311Suche in Google Scholar

Ninio, A. 1999. Pathbreaking verbs in syntactic development and the question of prototypical transitivity. Journal of Child Language 26(3). 619–653.10.1017/S0305000999003931Suche in Google Scholar

Ochsenbauer, A.-K. & H. Engemann. 2011. The impact of typological factors in monolingual and bilingual first language acquisition. Caused motion expressions in English and French. Language, Interaction and Acquisition 2(1). 101–128.10.1075/lia.2.1.05ochSuche in Google Scholar

Ochsenbauer, A.-K. & M. Hickmann. 2010. Children’s verbalization of motion events in German. Cognitive Linguistics 21(2). 217–238.10.1515/COGL.2010.008Suche in Google Scholar

Ochsenbauer, A.-K. & M. Hickmann. 2016. Static and dynamic location in French and German child language. In P. Guijarro-Fuentes, K. Schmitz & N. Müller (eds.), The acquisition of French in multilingual contexts, 118–144. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.10.21832/9781783094530-008Suche in Google Scholar

Olsen, S. 1996. Pleonastische Direktionale. In G. Harras & M. Bierwisch (eds.), Wenn die Semantik arbeitet: Klaus Baumgärtner zum 65. Geburtstag, 303–329. Tübingen: Niemeyer.Suche in Google Scholar

Pallotti, G. 2015. A simple view of linguistic complexity. Second Language Research 31(1). 117–134.10.1177/0267658314536435Suche in Google Scholar

Rescher, N. 1998. Complexity. A philosophical overview. New Brunswick & London: Transaction Publishers.Suche in Google Scholar

Sinnemäki, K. 2008. Complexity trade-offs in core argument marking. In M. Miestamo, K. Sinnemäki & F. Karlsson (eds.), Language complexity: Typology, contact, change, 67–88. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.10.1075/slcs.94.06sinSuche in Google Scholar

Slobin, D. I. 1996. From “thought and language” to “thinking for speaking”. In J. J. Gumperz & S. Levinson (eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity, 70–96. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Slobin, D. I. 2001. Form – function relations: How do children find out what they are? In M. Bowerman & S. Levinson (eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development, 406–449. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511620669.016Suche in Google Scholar

Slobin, D. I. 2003. Language and thought online: Cognitive consequences of linguistic relativity. In D. Gentler & S. Goldin-Meadow (eds.), Language in mind. Advances in the study of language and thought, 157–191. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Slobin, D. I. 2004. The many ways to search for a frog. Linguistic typology and the expression of motion events. In S. Strömqvist & L. Verhoeven (eds.), Relating events in narrative. Volume 2: Typological and contextual perspectives, 219–257. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Suche in Google Scholar

Slobin, D. I. 2006. What makes manner of motion salient? Explorations in linguistic typology, discourse, and cognition. In Maya Hickman & Stephane Robert (eds.), Space in languages: Linguistic systems and cognitive categories, 83–101. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.10.1075/tsl.66.05sloSuche in Google Scholar

Strömqvist, S. & L. Verhoeven. 2004. Typological and contextual perspectives on narrative development. In S. Strömqvist & L. Verhoeven (eds.), Relating events in narrative. Volume 2: Typological and contextual perspectives, 3–14. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.10.4324/9781410609694Suche in Google Scholar

Talmy, L. 1985. Lexicalization patterns. Semantic structure in lexical forms. In T. Shopen (ed.), Language typology and syntactic description: Grammatical categories and the lexicon. Vol. 3, 57–149. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. 1992. First verbs: A case study of early grammatical development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511527678Suche in Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. 2003. Constructing a language. Boston: Harvard University Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Verspoor, M., W. Lowie & M. Van Dijk. 2008. Variability in second language development from a dynamic systems perspective. Modern Language Journal 92(2). 214–231.10.1111/j.1540-4781.2008.00715.xSuche in Google Scholar

Received: 2016-8-10
Revised: 2017-3-22
Accepted: 2017-4-17
Published Online: 2017-10-17
Published in Print: 2017-11-27

© 2017 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Heruntergeladen am 1.10.2025 von https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/cog-2016-0089/html
Button zum nach oben scrollen