Abstract
Objectives
Comorbid with chronic pain are negative emotions, anger being particularly salient. To evaluate specific relationships between pain and anger, the present study deconstructed anger into five parameters and dichotomized pain into sensory vs. affective components. Hypotheses were (i) anger parameters would be significantly and positively correlated with affective pain more so than with sensory pain, and (ii) individual parameters would be differentially related to pain components.
Methods
The Anger Parameters Scale (APS) was used to rate five parameters of anger: frequency, duration, intensity, latency, and threshold. Also rated was the physical sensation of pain and the degree of distress from pain. The volunteer sample comprised n=51 chronic pain patients, varying in ethnicity/race and educational level.
Results
Descriptive statistics revealed: APS total M=71.52, SD=16.68, Sensory pain M=6.27, SD=2.15, Affective pain M=5.76, SD=2.28. Sensory and affective pain were highly correlated, r=0.70. APS total was significantly associated with affective pain (r=+0.28) but hardly with sensory pain (r=0.12). Two anger parameters significantly correlated with affective pain: anger frequency (r=+0.30, p<0.05) and anger threshold (r=+0.33, p<0.05). Secondarily, certain educational levels (but not gender and ethnicity/race) were associated with significantly higher APS total scores.
Conclusions
Scores for all variables were in the mid-range. As hypothesized, anger was more strongly correlated with distress/suffering of pain than with physical sensation of pain, though both pain components were closely coupled. Specific findings regarding frequency and threshold imply that being angry often and being oversensitive to provocation are associated with greater distress in this context. In deconstructing anger and dichotomizing pain, the present study extends previous research by elaborating on what aspects of anger are most related to which components of pain. Moreover, certain educational levels with higher levels of anger may need special attention. Further research could examine if treatment of anger might lead to corresponding changes in chronic pain.
Introduction
Pain definition
Pain definitions have undergone successive refinements over the last century [1], and across these variations there has been a consistent reference to sensation and emotion in pain. Thus, the International Association for the Study of Pain (IASP) recently proposed that pain is: “an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience associated with, or resembling that associated with, actual or potential tissue damage” [2]. The emotional or affective component of pain has been further elaborated in the latest updated International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11), which states that chronic primary pain is “characterized by significant emotional distress (anxiety, anger/frustration or depressed mood) or functional disability (interference in daily life activities and reduced participation in social roles)” [3]. In other words, chronic pain is not just aversive or unpleasant in a general sense but is associated with specific types of distress such as anxiety, depression, and anger or, for that matter, any type of negative affect (e.g., guilt, shame, regret).
Anger in the affective component of pain
Of the many types of negative affect possibly associated with pain, anxiety and depression have attracted much systematic research; anger has been the subject of clinical observations and anecdotes but remains relatively under-researched in this population [4, 5]. One recent striking illustration of anger in pain patients is that of a 45-year-old man in the US. He complained of persistent back pain following surgery and upon failing to get additional care, he blamed and fatally shot the physician who had treated him plus three others at the hospital before committing suicide [6]. While such rage by no means typifies chronic pain patients, it saliently points to the comorbidity of chronic pain and anger as a priority for research and an imperative for treatment.
Association between anger and pain
In a classic study involving the World Health Organization, 2,400 schoolchildren ages 11–12 and 15–16 were randomly sampled in Iceland [7]. They completed a 256-item questionnaire about pain, distress, attitudes, and behaviors. The most common concern turned out to be anger, affecting 76.5% of those with weekly back pain, 76.9% of those with weekly headaches, and 78.3% of those with weekly stomach pain. Moreover, anger increased with pain frequency and the number of pains, and it outstripped anxiety and other aspects of negative affect.
In an adult population of chronic pain patients, 69% of chronic pain patients were found to be angry at someone or another [8]. The intensity of this anger was, on average, in the moderate range of a 0–10 scale.
Numerous studies have gone beyond frequency data to standardized measures of anger in relation to various chronic pain syndromes. A narrative review of these studies has led to the conclusion that those reporting higher levels of anger report higher levels of pain [9].
Aggregation of quantitative data from at least two decades of research on anger and pain has now been accomplished in a meta-analysis [10]. Most of the 20 studies analyzed utilized the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory [11]. As reported, pain was significantly related to state anger, r=0.44, and much less so to trait anger, r=0.18. The correlation coefficient between non-specific anger measures and pain/disability was +0.35.
The anger-pain association may depend on the particular measures chosen. The STAXI essentially operationalizes state anger as what is felt “momentarily” and trait anger as how often certain behaviors occur in general [11]. It is one of 16 anger assessment tools that have been critically reviewed from a psychometric standpoint [12]. Some of these alternative assessment tools offer additional perspectives for assessing anger in pain patients [13].
Rationale
The Anger Parameters Scale (APS) is premised on the view that anger is measurable not only according to how often it occurs. Like any emotion, anger is quantifiable in terms of five parameters: frequency (number of occurrences), duration (length of time), intensity (magnitude), latency (immediacy), and threshold (sensitivity) [14]. As further detailed in the Method section, the APS is a self-report questionnaire for measuring each of these parameters. Additionally, as defined earlier, pain can be dichotomized into sensory vs. affective components [2]. As detailed in the Method section, these can be measured using unidimensional rating scales. Regarding how anger parameters might be related to pain components, our approach was to look beyond co-occurrence to the actual strength of association. Although the pain components were expected to be closely coupled, it was hypothesized that anger scores (individually and collectively) would be correlated more strongly with the affective than the sensory component of pain.
Methods
Participants
Participants were volunteers who met criteria for a study approved by the Institutional Review Board of the authors’ institution. The criteria for inclusion were (a) having chronic pain (minimum 6 months), (b) recent/current use of analgesic medication (minimum 3 months) in the past 12 months, (c) having anger related to pain or other circumstances as indicated by mid-range scores on STAXI, (d) speaking English, (e) having reliable access to phone and internet, and (f) being between the ages of 18–65. Furthermore, exclusion criteria were: (a) serious psychiatric illness (e.g., autism, schizophrenia, or dementia), (b) current or recent suicidal thoughts, (c) currently taking anti-psychotic medication, and d) currently breastfeeding (as stipulated by the Institutional Review Board because of the possible pain and non-use of analgesics during the weeks postpartum).
A total of 1,061 participants responded to our advertisements for volunteers, of which 184 met criteria for inclusion while not meeting criteria for exclusion. Of those passing the screening process, 82 participants expressed interest in further participation in a subsequent study on the treatment outcome evaluation of anger and they were sent the study questionnaire with items relating to pain, anger, and demographic variables (gender, race/ethnicity, and educational level). As a precursor, this study was designed to address the question of the strength of relationship between pain components and anger parameters, whereas the subsequent treatment outcome study was for the specific purpose of evaluating a new psychological treatment program that had been developed for regulating anger.
Subsequently, 28 participants were dropped due to non-communication or non-completion, resulting in 54 participants who completed the study requirements. Of these, 3 participants were excluded (with consensus from two members of the research team) because of missing or contradictory responses to questions about pain and anger. This yielded a final sample of n=51 participants with analyzable data for this precursor study on the relationship between anger and pain. This sample size was deemed sufficient for 70% power in detecting a medium-sized correlation. The demographic breakdown of the sample is shown in Table 1.
Demographic characteristics of sample.
# Of observations | % Of total sample | |
Gender | ||
Female | 40 | 78.43% |
Male | 10 | 19.61% |
NA | 1 | 1.96% |
Race/Ethnicity | ||
White | 29 | 56.86% |
Black or African American | 9 | 17.65% |
Asian | 5 | 9.80% |
American Indian or Alaska Native | 1 | 1.96% |
Other | 3 | 5.88% |
NA | 4 | 7.84% |
Educational level | ||
Grad school | 1 | 1.96% |
Master’s degree | 11 | 21.57% |
Bachelor’s degree | 9 | 17.65% |
Associate degree | 5 | 9.80% |
Some college, no degree | 17 | 33.33% |
High school diploma or equivalent | 4 | 7.84% |
NA | 4 | 7.84% |
-
Demographic characteristics of sample in three categories: Gender, Race/Ethnicity, and Education level. NA, not available.
Measures
Anger was measured using the Anger Parameters Scale (APS) as described earlier [14]. This self-report questionnaire is designed to assess five parameters of anger: frequency, duration, intensity, latency, and threshold. Frequency refers to how often an individual gets angry, duration refers to how long an individual stays angry, intensity refers to how strong the individual’s anger is, latency refers to how quickly the individual’s anger occurs in response to provocation, and finally, threshold refers to how sensitive to provocation the individual is.
The APS has 30-items organized into six items per parameter. Sample items are as follows: Frequency: “Anger is my least common emotion”; Duration: “My anger is prolonged”; Intensity: “I explode with anger”; Latency: “I am slow to anger”; Threshold: “A single provocation is enough to anger me.” Each statement is meant to be rated on a 0 to 4 scale (fractional ratings permitted), 0 meaning the statement is absolutely false or inapplicable to the participant and 4 meaning that it is absolutely true or totally applicable to the participant. Half of the 30 items of the APS are negatively keyed to control for response sets and therefore they are reverse-scored. For each parameter, scores range from 0 to 24, the total score for the instrument having a maximum value of 120. For individual parameters and for the total scores, higher values indicate greater maladaptiveness [14].
Evidence has confirmed the factorial structure of the APS [15]. Internal consistency as indexed by Cronbach’s alpha=0.85 for frequency, 0.90 for duration, 0.62 for intensity, 0.88 for latency, and 0.74 for threshold [16].
To assess pain, two separate unidimensional scales were employed, one pertaining to the physical sensation of pain and the other pertaining to the degree of distress from pain. The scales were anchored at 0 for absence of the phenomenon being rated, 1 for minimally detectable level of the phenomenon being rated, and 10 for maximum level of the phenomenon being rated. This method of twin unidimensional scales for sensory vs. affective pain has been successfully used in a long line of research [17, 18].
Procedures
Participants were recruited largely through posts on the following social media platforms: Facebook, Reddit, Twitter, and Instagram. Electronic announcements were also emailed to various organizations that expressed willingness to disseminate flyers regarding the study. These included chronic pain support groups, police interest groups, warehouse worker groups/unions, construction/manual labor groups/unions, and veteran support groups including the American Legion Posts.
Those interested were directed to an online Qualtrics survey so that informed consent was electronically obtained before participants proceeded to answer specific questions pertaining to eligibility. To ascertain anger, participants were asked to complete the 10 items making up the trait section of the STAXI-2. To be eligible for inclusion, the participant had to score at least 19 out of a maximum of 40 on this subscale. This would put their score in the mid-range of trait anger.
Research assistants screened participants for inclusion and exclusion criteria. Participants who passed the screening process were then granted access to the main study questionnaire containing the APS and scales for measuring pain. The questionnaires were hosted on Qualtrics and disseminated by email as participants became eligible.
Results
Table 2 first shows the descriptive statistics of each variable. As can be seen, the APS total averaged 71.52 out of a possible maximum of 120. Means for individual parameters of anger were in the range of 11.55–16.13 out of a maximum subscale score of 24. As for pain ratings, both sensory and affective components were rated slightly above the mid-point of the 10-point scale.
Means, standard deviation, and Pearson correlation matrix for study parameters.
Variables | M | SD | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. APS total | 71.52 | 16.68 | 1.000 | |||||||
2. Frequency | 14.13 | 4.22 | 0.919a | 1.000 | ||||||
3. Duration | 13.69 | 5.06 | 0.804a | 0.708a | 1.000 | |||||
4. Intensity | 11.55 | 3.98 | 0.686a | 0.614a | 0.420a | 1.000 | ||||
5. Latency | 16.13 | 3.63 | 0.692a | 0.547a | 0.309b | 0.318b | 1.000 | |||
6. Threshold | 16.03 | 3.81 | 0.867a | 0.758a | 0.637a | 0.386a | 0.698a | 1.000 | ||
7. Sensory pain | 6.27 | 2.15 | 0.124 | 0.186 | 0.070 | 0.012 | 0.146 | 0.083 | 1.000 | |
8. Affective pain | 5.76 | 2.28 | 0.283b | 0.303b | 0.132 | 0.150 | 0.239 | 0.327b | 0.697a | 1.000 |
-
ap<0.01 (two tailed), bp<0.05 (two-tailed).
Table 2 also shows the correlation coefficients among all anger and pain variables. Sensory and affective pain components were highly and significantly correlated, r=+0.7, p<0.01. However, the APS total score was significantly associated with affective pain (r=+0.28, p<0.05), but hardly with sensory pain (r=0.12, p=0.40). Two of the five anger parameters were significantly correlated with affective pain. These were anger frequency, r=+0.30, p<0.05 and anger threshold, r=+0.33, p<0.05. A third parameter, latency, was marginally significant, r=0.24, p=0.09. All probabilities are two-tailed.
As part of secondary analyses, a scattergram is provided of affective pain in relation to APS total scores (Figure 1). The regression equation is shown on the regression line. Finally, multiple regression of demographic variables (gender, ethnicity/race, and educational level) onto APS total scores revealed no significance except for educational level (Table 3). With “Associate degree” designated as the reference subgroup, participants with high school diploma or equivalent had the highest anger scores whereas those with college level or higher degrees had relatively lower anger scores (Note that with high school designated as reference group, none of the coefficients in Table 3 reached significance).

Scatterplot and line regression of affective pain to APS total score. APS is an abbreviation for anger parameter Scale.
Regression results for APS total scores.
Variables | Coefficients | Standard error | t-Statistic | Prob. |
---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 84.341 | 21.896 | 3.852 | 0.000463a |
Gender: Male | −2.490 | 6.873 | −0.362 | 0.719294 |
Ethnicity and race: Asian | −16.178 | 21.704 | −0.745 | 0.460863 |
Ethnicity and race: Black or African American | −28.337 | 19.267 | −1.471 | 0.150051 |
Ethnicity and race: White | −28.134 | 20.760 | −1.355 | 0.183791 |
Ethnicity and race: Other | −29.633 | 22.519 | −1.316 | 0.196526 |
Education level: High school diploma or equivalent | 21.659 | 14.316 | 1.513 | 0.139027 |
Education level: Some college, no degree | 10.640 | 8.801 | 1.209 | 0.234594 |
Education level: Bachelor’s degree | 19.597 | 9.399 | 2.085 | 0.044222b |
Education level: Master’s degree | 20.311 | 9.135 | 2.223 | 0.032553b |
Education level: Graduate school | 11.283 | 18.950 | 0.595 | 0.555304 |
-
ap<0.01 (two tailed), bp<0.05 (two-tailed).
Discussion
The association between sensory and affective components of pain was highly positive and significant, r=0.70. This is consistent with a long line of research indicating that these phenomena are closely coupled while also separable under intervention [19, 20]. However, as hypothesized in this study, the two components turn out to be differentially related to anger, affective pain being significant in its relationship with anger whereas sensory pain being almost uncorrelated with anger. The observed r=0.28 between anger and pain approaches the finding of r=0.35 between non-specific anger and pain in the Adachi et al. meta-analysis [10].
By deconstructing anger according to the five subscales of the APS, it is further possible to see how individual parameters of anger relate differently to components of pain [14]. As with the overall APS score, the five individual parameters of anger were all stronger in their association with affective pain than sensory pain. The significant relationships observed between affective pain and anger frequency and anger threshold, suggest that being angry more often and being oversensitive to provocation are associated with greater distress or suffering related to pain. To our knowledge, this is the first demonstration of the links between specific parameters of the anger experience and the affective component of pain.
The above associations between pain and anger are open to multiple explanatory models, both biological and behavioral. As succinctly reviewed by Greenwood and colleagues, one possibility is that anger increases muscle reactivity thus contributing to pain in certain sites [9]. This is supported by the research of Burns and colleagues [21]. Another pathway may be through the reduced effects of endogenous opioids [22]; a third possibility emerging from the findings of Kiecolt-Glaser and colleagues is that emotions like anger may alter immune and endocrine responses to pain [23]. In the behavioral domain, anger may increase the probability of maladaptive behaviors that are further reinforced by significant others and hinder the development of therapeutic alliance with healthcare providers [4, 9].
Secondary analyses using multiple regression indicates that while gender and ethnicity/race did not have any significant relationship with anger scores, one demographic variable (educational level) was noteworthy. Participants with no more than high school qualifications had the highest anger scores compared to those with college or graduate education who had relatively lower anger scores. This cannot be taken to mean a linear association between educational level and anger. Moreover, it is possible that educational attainment may itself be correlated with developmental stage and life experiences. Together with the above primary findings, the clinical implication is that treating anger in chronic pain patients may be enhanced by addressing the frequency and (hyper)sensitivity of the anger response. as well as any educational/developmental aspects that may govern how receptive and responsive the individual may be towards the cognitive, behavioral, and experiential therapies typically used for anger regulation.
Limitations
The present study is limited by the size and composition of the sample. Of n=51, 78% were female, and 57% identified as “White”. Although these are representative of the gender and ethnicity/race of the population from which participants were drawn, our findings would need to be replicated in a much larger and demographically heterogenous sample before they could be deemed generalizable.
The methodology of the study is essentially correlational. Causal inferences are not justifiable at this stage. At least five major interactions between negative affect and pain might be entertained [4, 24]:- firstly, anger may precede pain as a predisposing factor that exerts a long-term influence on the latter; secondly, anger may be a precipitating factor that suddenly triggers pain; thirdly, anger may not be an antecedent but a consequence of pain; fourthly anger may be an exacerbating factor that augments pain intensity; and lastly, anger may be a perpetuating factor that prolongs the duration of pain. In order to tease apart which of these interactions holds, other methods including experimental manipulation would be applicable. As it stands, the present findings do build on the existing literature on co-prevalence of anger and pain by measuring the strength of association between the two, but the findings do not explicate the more intriguing question of how each dynamically interacts with the other. Tackling that would entail other methodological designs such as prospective studies, longitudinal observations, and time series analyses.
Funding source: Grant for Research Advancement and Transformation awarded to Dr. Fernandez from the Office of the Vice President for Research, Economic Development, and Knowledge Enterprise at the University of Texas at San Antonio
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank Rudy Garza, Carolina Hernandez, Ruby Munoz, Alberto Jimenez, Lucy Lumumba, and Victoria Rodriguez especially for their assistance in participant recruitment and data collection.
-
Research funding: This study was supported in part by a Grant for Research Advancement and Transformation awarded to Dr. Fernandez from the Office of the Vice President for Research, Economic Development, and Knowledge Enterprise at the University of Texas at San Antonio.
-
Author contributions: All authors have accepted responsibility for the entire content of this manuscript and approved its submission.
-
Competing interests: Authors state no conflict of interest.
-
Informed consent: Informed consent has been obtained from all individuals included in this study.
-
Ethical approval: Research involving human subjects complied with all relevant national regulations, institutional policies and is in accordance with the tenets of the Helsinki Declaration (as amended in 2013) and has been approved by the authors’ Institutional Review Board (IRB #20-002) at the University of Texas at San Antonio.
References
1. Mailis, A, Tepperman, PS, Hapidou, EG. Chronic pain: evolution of clinical definitions and implications for practice. Psychol Inj Law 2020;13:412–26. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12207-020-09391-w.Search in Google Scholar
2. Raja, SN, Carr, DB, Cohen, M, Finnerup, NB, Flor, H, Gibson, S, et al.. The revised International Association for the Study of Pain definition of pain: concepts, challenges, and compromises. Pain 2020;161:1976–82. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001939.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
3. World Health Organization. MG30.0 chronic primary pain [Internet]; 2020. Available from: https://icd.who.int/browse11/l-m/en#/http://id.who.int/icd/entity/1326332835.Search in Google Scholar
4. Fernandez, E. Anxiety, depression, and anger in pain: research findings and clinical options. Dallas, TX: Advanced Psychological Resources; 2002.Search in Google Scholar
5. Fernandez, E, Turk, DC. The scope and significance of anger in the experience of chronic pain. Pain 1995;61:165–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(95)00192-U.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
6. HannaWatts, JA. Gunman who killed 4 at Oklahoma medical building had been a patient of a victim, police chief says [Internet] CNN. Cable News Network; 2022. Available from: https://www.cnn.com/2022/06/02/us/tulsa-hospital-shooting-thursday/index.html [Accessed 6 Jun 2022].Search in Google Scholar
7. Kristjánsdóttir, G. The relationship between pains and various discomforts in schoolchildren. Childhood 1997;4:491–504. https://doi.org/10.1177/0907568297004004008.Search in Google Scholar
8. Okifuji, A, Turk, DC, Curran, SL. Anger in chronic pain: investigations of anger targets and intensity. J Psychosom Res 1999;47:1–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-3999(99)00006-9.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
9. Greenwood, KA, Thurston, R, Rumble, M, Waters, SJ, Keefe, FJ. Anger and persistent pain: current status and future directions. Pain 2003;103:1–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0304-3959(03)00132-5.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
10. Adachi, T, Yamada, K, Fujino, H, Enomoto, K, Shibata, M. Associations between anger and chronic primary pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Scand J Pain 2021;22:1–13. https://doi.org/10.1515/sjpain-2021-0154.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
11. Spielberger, CD. STAXI-2: state-trait anger expression inventory-2. Odessa, Flor: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1999.Search in Google Scholar
12. Fernandez, E, Day, A, Boyle, GJ. Measures of anger and hostility in adults. In: Boyle, GJ, Saklofske, D, Matthews, G, editors. Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Constructs. London, UK: Academic Press; 2015:74–100 pp.10.1016/B978-0-12-386915-9.00004-8Search in Google Scholar
13. Sommer, I, Lukic, N, Rössler, W, Ettlin, DA. Measuring anger in patients experiencing chronic pain - a systematic review. J Psychosom Res 2019;125:109778. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2019.109778.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
14. Fernandez, E, Arevalo, I, Vargas, R, Torralba, A. Norms for five parameters of anger: how do incarcerated adults differ from the community? Int J Forensic Ment Health 2014;13:18–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/14999013.2014.889256.Search in Google Scholar
15. Fernandez, E, Woldgabreal, Y, Guharajan, D, Day, A, Kiageri, V, Ramtahal, N. Social desirability bias against admitting anger: bias in the test-taker or bias in the test? J Pers Assess 2019;101:644–52. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2018.1464017.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
16. Henderson, MM. The anger parameters scale and the anger expressions scale: a psychometric study (Order No. 10108503). San Antonio, TX: The University of Texas at San Antonio ProQuest Dissertations Publishing; 2016.Search in Google Scholar
17. Gracely, RH. Affective dimensions of pain how many and how measured? APS J 1992;1:243–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/1058-9139(92)90056-I.Search in Google Scholar
18. Price, DD, Harkins, SW, Baker, C. Sensory-affective relationships among different types of clinical and experimental pain. Pain 1987;28:297–307. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(87)90065-0.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
19. Melzack, R. Discriminative capacity of the McGill pain questionnaire: letter to the editor. Pain 1985;23:201–3. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(85)90060-0.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
20. Beattie, PF, Dowda, M, Feuerstein, M. Differentiating sensory and affective-sensory pain descriptions in patients undergoing magnetic resonance imaging for persistent low back pain. Pain 2004;110:189–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2004.03.026.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
21. Burns, JW, Bruehl, S, Quartana, PJ. Anger management style and hostility among patients with chronic pain: effects on symptom-specific physiological reactivity during anger- and sadness- recall interviews. Psychosom Med 2006;68:786–93. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.psy.0000238211.89198.e4.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
22. Bruehl, S, Burns, JW, Chung, OY, Ward, P, Johnson, B. Anger and pain sensitivity in chronic low back pain patients and pain-free controls: the role of endogenous opioids. Pain 2002;99:223–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0304-3959(02)00104-5.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
23. Kiecolt-Glaser, JK, McGuire, L, Robles, TF, Glaser, R. Psychoneuroimmunology: psychological influences on immune function and health. J Consult Clin Psychol 2002;70:537–47. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-006x.70.3.537.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
24. Fernandez, E, Kerns, RD. Pain and affective disorders: looking beyond the “chicken and egg” conundrum. In: Giamberardino, MA, Jensen, T, editors. Pain comorbidities: understanding and treating the complex patient. Seattle, WA: IASP; 2012:279–96 pp.Search in Google Scholar
© 2022 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston
Articles in the same Issue
- Frontmatter
- Editorial Comment
- Chronic pain and health inequalities: why we need to act
- Systematic Reviews
- Resilience as a protective factor in face of pain symptomatology, disability and psychological outcomes in adult chronic pain populations: a scoping review
- Is intravenous magnesium sulphate a suitable adjuvant in postoperative pain management? – A critical and systematic review of methodology in randomized controlled trials
- Topical Review
- Pain assessment 3 × 3: a clinical reasoning framework for healthcare professionals
- Clinical Pain Researches
- The treatment lottery of chronic back pain? A case series at a multidisciplinary pain centre
- Parameters of anger as related to sensory-affective components of pain
- Loneliness in patients with somatic symptom disorder
- The development and measurement properties of the Dutch version of the fear-avoidance components scale (FACS-D) in persons with chronic musculoskeletal pain
- Observational Studies
- Can interoceptive sensitivity provide information on the difference in the perceptual mechanisms of recurrent and chronic pain? Part I. A retrospective clinical study related to multidimensional pain assessment
- Distress intolerance and pain catastrophizing as mediating variables in PTSD and chronic noncancer pain comorbidity
- Stress-induced headache in the general working population is moderated by the NRCAM rs2300043 genotype
- Does poor sleep quality lead to increased low back pain the following day?
- “I had already tried that before going to the doctor” – exploring adolescents’ with knee pain perspectives on ‘wait and see’ as a management strategy in primary care; a study with brief semi-structured qualitative interviews
- Problematic opioid use among osteoarthritis patients with chronic post-operative pain after joint replacement: analyses from the BISCUITS study
- Worst pain intensity and opioid intake during the early postoperative period were not associated with moderate-severe pain 12 months after total knee arthroplasty – a longitudinal study
- Original Experimentals
- How gender affects the decoding of facial expressions of pain
- A simple, bed-side tool to assess evoked pressure pain intensity
- Effects of psychosocial stress and performance feedback on pain processing and its correlation with subjective and neuroendocrine parameters
- Participatory research: a Priority Setting Partnership for chronic musculoskeletal pain in Denmark
- Educational Case Report
- Hypophosphatasia as a plausible cause of vitamin B6 associated mouth pain: a case-report
- Short Communications
- Pain “chronification”: what is the problem with this model?
- Korsakoff syndrome and altered pain perception: a search of underlying neural mechanisms
Articles in the same Issue
- Frontmatter
- Editorial Comment
- Chronic pain and health inequalities: why we need to act
- Systematic Reviews
- Resilience as a protective factor in face of pain symptomatology, disability and psychological outcomes in adult chronic pain populations: a scoping review
- Is intravenous magnesium sulphate a suitable adjuvant in postoperative pain management? – A critical and systematic review of methodology in randomized controlled trials
- Topical Review
- Pain assessment 3 × 3: a clinical reasoning framework for healthcare professionals
- Clinical Pain Researches
- The treatment lottery of chronic back pain? A case series at a multidisciplinary pain centre
- Parameters of anger as related to sensory-affective components of pain
- Loneliness in patients with somatic symptom disorder
- The development and measurement properties of the Dutch version of the fear-avoidance components scale (FACS-D) in persons with chronic musculoskeletal pain
- Observational Studies
- Can interoceptive sensitivity provide information on the difference in the perceptual mechanisms of recurrent and chronic pain? Part I. A retrospective clinical study related to multidimensional pain assessment
- Distress intolerance and pain catastrophizing as mediating variables in PTSD and chronic noncancer pain comorbidity
- Stress-induced headache in the general working population is moderated by the NRCAM rs2300043 genotype
- Does poor sleep quality lead to increased low back pain the following day?
- “I had already tried that before going to the doctor” – exploring adolescents’ with knee pain perspectives on ‘wait and see’ as a management strategy in primary care; a study with brief semi-structured qualitative interviews
- Problematic opioid use among osteoarthritis patients with chronic post-operative pain after joint replacement: analyses from the BISCUITS study
- Worst pain intensity and opioid intake during the early postoperative period were not associated with moderate-severe pain 12 months after total knee arthroplasty – a longitudinal study
- Original Experimentals
- How gender affects the decoding of facial expressions of pain
- A simple, bed-side tool to assess evoked pressure pain intensity
- Effects of psychosocial stress and performance feedback on pain processing and its correlation with subjective and neuroendocrine parameters
- Participatory research: a Priority Setting Partnership for chronic musculoskeletal pain in Denmark
- Educational Case Report
- Hypophosphatasia as a plausible cause of vitamin B6 associated mouth pain: a case-report
- Short Communications
- Pain “chronification”: what is the problem with this model?
- Korsakoff syndrome and altered pain perception: a search of underlying neural mechanisms