A comprehensive understanding of pain requires not only articulating its proximate mechanisms but to appreciate how adjustments to its presence regulate ourselves and the world around us through the behavioural changes that it drives. Such changes are the basis upon which most knowledge about pain mechanisms in humans and animals is based. As fundamentally social animals, many of the behaviour changes induced by pain in humans are communicative [1]. Some are communicative indirectly. For example, rubbing an injured forearm, while primarily aimed at downregulating discomfort, can lead an observer to infer something about the sufferer’s experience. Other pain-related behaviours are communicative “by design.” In other words, they appear to have evolved largely, if not primarily, as a means of transmitting something about the sufferer’s experience to others [2]. Among such behaviours are certain characteristic changes in facial expression that have been the subject of considerable scrutiny ever since methodology that enabled their objective empirical description became available [3]. Analysis of such behaviours enables us to describe the link between pain and its proximal causes on one hand and its behavioural expression on the other [4], [5] and, increasingly, to draw inferences about sufferers’ pain that would otherwise be difficult, if not impossible to make. For example, in previous work by the authors of the article by Meister, Horn-Hofmann, Kunz, Krumhuber and Lautenbacher ([6], this volume), by making careful measurements of the facial expressions of patients with dementia, Kunz, Scharmann, Hemmeter, Schepelmann and Lautenbacher [7] were able to demonstrate enhanced pain reactivity in patients who could no longer characterize their pain verbally as a result of dementia. While important, study of the links among bodily injury, pain experience, and pain expression nevertheless provides an incomplete characterization of the pain nexus because it ends at the expression and does not deal with its social consequences. A robust field of inquiry has emerged in recent years that focuses on those social consequences [8].
In fact, the article by Meister et al. [7] contributes both to our understanding of the properties of pain expression and of its social consequences. Early work on pain expression showed that pain arising from many different experimental manipulations or clinical conditions is accompanied by a limited number of changes in facial expression. The most common changes include contractions of the orbicularis oculi muscle, which narrows the eye aperture and raises the cheeks; contraction of the corrugator muscle, lowering and drawing together the eyebrows, contraction of the levator muscle, which raises the upper lip and, sometimes wrinkles the nose, and closure of the eyes [9], [10]. Empirical studies of pain expression using the gold-standard for dissecting facial movements, the Facial Action Coding System [11] (FACS) have occasionally reported other movements to be pain-related, including, curiously, contraction of zygomaticus major, the principal muscle involved in the smile; however, such findings have been inconsistent and occasional. The consistency with which the aforementioned actions have been found to be associated with pain have led to occasional conclusions that they comprise a unitary “pain expression.” However, later work by Kunz and Lautenbacher [12], using cluster analysis, has called this notion into question by identifying several distinct configurations of facial actions that occur during pain, in addition to a “stoic” expression, which accompanies pain among a significant number of people.
All of this work has been based on the FACS method, which is detailed and fine-grained, applied to recordings of people in various painful conditions. But to show that the behavioural features associated with the identified configurations are indeed socially meaningful, it is necessary to show that they are perceived coherently as communicating pain by others, using a conventional judgement study methodology [13]. To do so poses problems for several reasons, among which are that features of the behaviour to be shown other than the topographical configurations at issue–for example, the timing of the actions or the co-occurrence of other movements—could affect or even confound judgements. Technical advances in the form of avatars hold promise to overcome this problem because they allow the construction of realistic models of facial expression in which these other features can be controlled precisely. Moreover, they make it possible to manipulate other features of the face, such as its apparent gender, age, skin colour, etc., so that understanding can be built of their role in social perception of the pain sufferer.
Avatars were programmed using software that generates realistic-looking facial expressions corresponding to FACS Action Units. The apparent gender and age of the avatars were varied systematically. Observers viewed the programmed videos, rating the degree of pain displayed and their confidence in their judgements. Analyses revealed that three of the five clusters were judged to show pain of moderate intensity. The ones that did not included a “stoic” cluster and a cluster involving raised brows.
The judgement study methodology thus helps to sharpen our understanding of the empirical configurations identified through cluster analysis by showing that not all of them are perceived reliably as communicating pain. Further, and interestingly, the findings appear to emphasize the central importance of the action of orbicularis oculi, which is a component of all the configurations perceived as painful. Although the authors largely focus on what the findings may imply in terms of training people and computer vision systems to identify pain in others, the demonstration of these three configurations opens questions about their underlying determinants that further research could profitably pursue.
The other major substantive finding was that female avatars were rated as displaying substantially more pain than avatars who appeared to be male. The difference was greatest within the cluster defined exclusively by furrowed brows and narrowed eyes. Although, as the authors point out, this is a not-unexpected finding in light of previous studies, the use of the avatar methodology, which makes it possible to present precisely the same actions on faces modified to look male or female provides a degree of experimental control heretofore unavailable. Thus, the article by Meister et al. provides a kind of “proof of concept” that can in principle be applied to other socially-relevant physiognomic and behavioural characteristics.
Increasingly in recent years, we have become aware of the downside of the explosive developments that have occurred with information technology. Nevertheless, some of the capabilities it has given us have allowed for advances that it is hard to imagine having taken place in other ways. Indeed, as other recent work involving the use of avatars and other complex features enabled by information technology has shown [14], it is becoming increasingly possible to model real-life social interactions in the clinical setting and, in so doing, both deepen our understanding of the social nexus of pain and to identify ways of improving clinical practice. The paper by Meister et al. adds to this emergent field. As a proof-of-concept, it is likely to stimulate numerous novel lines of research.
-
Research funding: None declared.
-
Author contributions: All authors have accepted responsibility for the entire content of this manuscript and approved its submission.
-
Competing interests: The authors state no conflict of interest.
References
1. Martel, MO, Sullivan, MJL. Pain behavior: unitary or multidimensional phenomenon?. In: Vervoort, T, Kairos, K, Trost, Z, Prkachin, KM, editors. Social and interpersonal dimensions of pain: we don’t suffer alone. Cham, Switzerland: Springer international Publishing AG; 2018:79–99 pp.10.1007/978-3-319-78340-6_5Search in Google Scholar
2. Prkachin, KM. Pain behaviour is not unitary. Behav Brain Sci 1986;9:754–5. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00052316.Search in Google Scholar
3. Craig, KD, Prkachin, KM, Grunau, RVE. The facial expression of pain. In: Turk, DC, Melzack, R, editors. Handbook of pain assessment, 3rd ed. New York: Guilford; 2011:117–33 pp.Search in Google Scholar
4. Prkachin, KM, Craig, KD. Expressing pain: the communication and interpretation of facial pain signals. J Nonverbal Behav 1995;19:191–205. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02173080.Search in Google Scholar
5. Prkachin, KM, Kaseweter, KA, Browne, ME. Understanding the suffering of others: the sources and consequences of third-person pain. In: Pickering, G, Gibson, S, editors. Pain, emotion and cognition: a complex nexus. New York: Springer; 2015:53–72 pp.10.1007/978-3-319-12033-1_4Search in Google Scholar
6. Meister, E, Horn-Hofmann, C, Kunz, M, Krumhuber, E, Lautenbacher, S. Decoding of facial expressions of pain in avatars: does sex matter? Scand J Pain. https://doi.org/10.1515/sjpain-2020-0078 [Epub ahead of print].Search in Google Scholar PubMed
7. Kunz, M, Scharmann, S, Hemmeter, U, Schepelmann, K, Lautenbacher, S. The facial expression of pain in patients with dementia. Pain 2007;133:221–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2007.09.007.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
8. Vervoort, T, Karos, K, Trost, Z, Prkachin, KM. Social and interpersonal dynamics in pain: we don’t suffer alone. Cham, Switzerland: Springer; 2018.10.1007/978-3-319-78340-6Search in Google Scholar
9. Kunz, MM, Kairos, K, Vervoort, T. When, how, and why do we express pain?. In: Vervoort, T, Kairos, K, Trost, Z, Prkachin, KM, editors. Social and interpersonal dimensions of pain: we don’t suffer alone. Cham, Switzerland: Springer international Publishing AG; 2018:101–20 pp.10.1007/978-3-319-78340-6_6Search in Google Scholar
10. Prkachin, KM. The consistency of facial expressions of pain: a comparison across modalities. Pain 1992;51:297–306. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(92)90213-u.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
11. Ekman, P, Hager, J, Friesen, WV. Facial action coding system. The manual on CD-ROM. Salt Lake City, UT: A Human Face; 2002.Search in Google Scholar
12. Kunz, M, Lautenbacher, S. The faces of pain: a cluster analysis of individual differences in facial activity patterns of pain. Eur J Pain 2014;18:813–23. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1532-2149.2013.00421.x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
13. Rosenthal, R. Conducting judgment studies: some methodological issues. In: Harrigan, JA, Rosenthal, R, Scherer, K, editors. The new handbook of methods in nonverbal behavior research. New York: Oxford University Press; 2008:199–236 pp.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198529620.003.0005Search in Google Scholar
14. Hirsh, AT, Miller, MM, Hollingshead, NA, Anastas, T, Carnell, ST, Lok, BC, et al. A randomized controlled trial testing a virtual perspective-taking intervention to reduce race and socioeconomic status disparities in pain care. Pain 2019;160:2229–40. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001634.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
© 2020 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston
Articles in the same Issue
- Frontmatter
- Editorial Comments
- Patients with shoulder pain referred to specialist care; treatment, predictors of pain and disability, emotional distress, main symptoms and sick-leave: a cohort study with a 6-months follow-up
- Inferring pain from avatars
- Systematic Review
- Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the primary motor cortex in management of chronic neuropathic pain: a systematic review
- Topical Reviews
- Exploring the underlying mechanism of pain-related disability in hypermobile adolescents with chronic musculoskeletal pain
- Pain management programmes via video conferencing: a rapid review
- Clinical Pain Research
- Prevalence of temporomandibular disorder in adult patients with chronic pain
- A cost-utility analysis of multimodal pain rehabilitation in primary healthcare
- Psychosocial subgroups in high-performance athletes with low back pain: eustress-endurance is most frequent, distress-endurance most problematic!
- Trajectories in severe persistent pain after groin hernia repair: a retrospective analysis
- Involvement of relatives in chronic non-malignant pain rehabilitation at multidisciplinary pain centres: part one – the patient perspective
- Observational Studies
- Recurrent abdominal pain among adolescents: trends and social inequality 1991–2018
- Cross-cultural adaptation and psychometric validation of the Hausa version of Örebro Musculoskeletal Pain Screening Questionnaire in patients with non-specific low back pain
- A proof-of-concept study on the impact of a chronic pain and physical activity training workshop for exercise professionals
- Intravenous patient-controlled analgesia vs nurse administered oral oxycodone after total knee arthroplasty: a retrospective cohort study
- Everyday living with pain – reported by patients with multiple myeloma
- Original Experimental
- The CA1 hippocampal serotonin alterations involved in anxiety-like behavior induced by sciatic nerve injury in rats
- A single bout of coordination training does not lead to EIH in young healthy men – a RCT
- Think twice before starting a new trial; what is the impact of recommendations to stop doing new trials?
- The association between selected genetic variants and individual differences in experimental pain
- Decoding of facial expressions of pain in avatars: does sex matter?
- Differences in personality, perceived stress and physical activity in women with burning mouth syndrome compared to controls
- Educational Case Reports
- Leiomyosarcoma of the small intestine presenting as abdominal myofascial pain syndrome (AMPS): case report
- Duloxetine for the management of sensory and taste alterations, following iatrogenic damage of the lingual and chorda tympani nerve
- Lead extrusion ten months after spinal cord stimulator implantation: a case report
- Short Communication
- Postoperative opioids and risk of respiratory depression – A cross-sectional evaluation of routines for administration and monitoring in a tertiary hospital
Articles in the same Issue
- Frontmatter
- Editorial Comments
- Patients with shoulder pain referred to specialist care; treatment, predictors of pain and disability, emotional distress, main symptoms and sick-leave: a cohort study with a 6-months follow-up
- Inferring pain from avatars
- Systematic Review
- Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the primary motor cortex in management of chronic neuropathic pain: a systematic review
- Topical Reviews
- Exploring the underlying mechanism of pain-related disability in hypermobile adolescents with chronic musculoskeletal pain
- Pain management programmes via video conferencing: a rapid review
- Clinical Pain Research
- Prevalence of temporomandibular disorder in adult patients with chronic pain
- A cost-utility analysis of multimodal pain rehabilitation in primary healthcare
- Psychosocial subgroups in high-performance athletes with low back pain: eustress-endurance is most frequent, distress-endurance most problematic!
- Trajectories in severe persistent pain after groin hernia repair: a retrospective analysis
- Involvement of relatives in chronic non-malignant pain rehabilitation at multidisciplinary pain centres: part one – the patient perspective
- Observational Studies
- Recurrent abdominal pain among adolescents: trends and social inequality 1991–2018
- Cross-cultural adaptation and psychometric validation of the Hausa version of Örebro Musculoskeletal Pain Screening Questionnaire in patients with non-specific low back pain
- A proof-of-concept study on the impact of a chronic pain and physical activity training workshop for exercise professionals
- Intravenous patient-controlled analgesia vs nurse administered oral oxycodone after total knee arthroplasty: a retrospective cohort study
- Everyday living with pain – reported by patients with multiple myeloma
- Original Experimental
- The CA1 hippocampal serotonin alterations involved in anxiety-like behavior induced by sciatic nerve injury in rats
- A single bout of coordination training does not lead to EIH in young healthy men – a RCT
- Think twice before starting a new trial; what is the impact of recommendations to stop doing new trials?
- The association between selected genetic variants and individual differences in experimental pain
- Decoding of facial expressions of pain in avatars: does sex matter?
- Differences in personality, perceived stress and physical activity in women with burning mouth syndrome compared to controls
- Educational Case Reports
- Leiomyosarcoma of the small intestine presenting as abdominal myofascial pain syndrome (AMPS): case report
- Duloxetine for the management of sensory and taste alterations, following iatrogenic damage of the lingual and chorda tympani nerve
- Lead extrusion ten months after spinal cord stimulator implantation: a case report
- Short Communication
- Postoperative opioids and risk of respiratory depression – A cross-sectional evaluation of routines for administration and monitoring in a tertiary hospital