Abstract
In laboratory studies, speakers often modify their speech (e.g., by hyper-articulating, attempting a “supra-regional” norm, or hypercorrecting) to ‘speak correctly’, especially under the (perceived) expectation of formal speech. A strategy to address this challenge involves comparing performance on one variable against a well-documented pattern on another variable. Previous research in Spanish shows /s/ sensitivity to speech formality, making it a useful comparison for probing other lenition processes. This study uses the acoustics of coda /s/ to predict the acoustics of /n/ and pre-nasal vowels among speakers of Argentine and Dominican Spanish, two dialects in which these have been widely attested. The data analyzed come from 28 speakers of Dominican Spanish and 26 of Argentine Spanish, recorded with a nasometer (a split channel set of microphones that record nose and mouth signals separately). Findings shows pervasive presence of frication for /s/ (whether [s] or [h]), suggesting heightened attention to speech. Additionally, longer /s/ co-occurs with longer nasal consonants. However, when duration of /s/ was compared to the time-course of nasalization, only the Argentine data showed earlier onset of nasalization with shorter /s/. The addition of a well-known sociolinguistic variable can serve as a validity measure for a less understood variable.
1 Introduction
It is not uncommon in laboratory studies for speakers to modify their speech (e.g., by hyper-articulating, attempting a “supra-regional” norm, or hypercorrecting) to ‘speak correctly’. Often, the result of this heightened attention to speech can be facilitated by elicitation techniques or the presence of an out-of-community researcher. However, within laboratory approaches -which offer distinct benefits- researchers face a dilemma: they either settle for modified speech, a clear drawback, or take steps to mitigate this effect. One strategy to address this challenge involves incorporating a well-documented variable to account for stylistically modified speech. This was precisely the approach taken by Willis and Ronquest (2022), who examined the ability of coda sibilant weakening to predict coda liquid production in Dominican Spanish. The present study builds upon this foundation to examine production patterns in /n/ weakening and anticipatory vowel nasalization in Argentine and Dominican Spanish.
Willis and Ronquest (2022) investigated the use of innovative variants of coda liquids (such as vocalization) in the Cibao region in the Dominican Republic. In their study, they used the production of /s/ to better understand stylistic change in participants’ speech due to a combination of linguistic insecurity and interaction with an outsider. Within Dominican speech, linguistic insecurity is prevalent (Alba 2004), often manifesting as hypercorrection (either by overshooting the use of a prestige variant or overgeneralizing a pattern where it is not etymologically warranted) or linguistic devaluation, or both. Thus, the desire to speak in a prestigious manner may prompt participants to adjust their speech patterns. To assess performative speech, the researchers compared the percentage of use of fricated /s/ (retained/aspirated vs. elided) to the use of variants of /l/ and /ɾ/ in a corpus of sociolinguistic interviews with 24 speakers from the Cibao region. The results showed that speakers with a higher tendency to retain or aspirate /s/ also produced canonical variants of liquid phonemes (i.e., [l] and [ɾ] respectively). When the realization of /s/ was included in the statistical models (one per liquid), it predicted vocalization of coda /l/ and of /ɾ/ in Cibao Spanish more so than any other variables in the models (such as social class or sex). Hence, comparing performance on one variable against a well-documented pattern on another variable provides a relevant framework to investigate participants’ potential style shifts due to data elicitation methods, linguistic insecurity, or both.
Building on this research, the present study probes the use of the same well-known sociolinguistic variable in Spanish – coda /s/ weakening – to predict patterns in the production of /n/ weakening and anticipatory vowel nasalization. Previous work comparing Argentine and Dominican Spanish has shown that the latter exhibits earlier onset of nasalization, arguably a reflex of phonologized nasalization (Bongiovanni 2021a, 2021b). Bongiovanni (2021b) also found similar degrees of nasal consonant weakening (measured in terms of duration and consonant-to-vowel ratio) across dialect groups. In fact, neither group exhibited much nasal consonant weakening. Considering the previous literature documenting nasal consonant weakening among several communities in the Hispanic Caribbean, the latter was an unexpected pattern. These findings thus suggest that results may have been the artifact of heightened attention to speech due to the nature of laboratory speech and the subsequent expectation of formal speech. Following Willis and Ronquest (2022), the present study returns to the dataset in Bongiovanni (2021a, 2021b) to examine the production of nasal consonant weakening and anticipatory vowel nasalization as a function of (broadly defined) attention to speech, which is characterized in terms of coda /s/ production. Specifically, I assess the power of the acoustics of coda /s/ to predict the acoustics of /n/ and anticipatory vowel nasalization among speakers of Argentine and Dominican Spanish, two dialects in which these have been widely attested. Findings revealed a pervasive presence of frication (whether [s] or [h]), across dialect groups. The examination of duration, center-of-gravity, and zero-crossing rates of coda /s/ showed that the Buenos Aires data exhibited more signs of lenition than the Santo Domingo data. Findings also showed that weakening of /s/ co-occurs with weakening of /n/, a statistical trend in both dialect groups. However, when weakening of /s/ was compared to the time-course of nasalization, it was only the Argentine data which showed earlier onset of nasalization correlated with lenited /s/.
The next section presents research on variation in word-final nasal consonant production, anticipatory vowel nasalization and word-final /s/ variation which motivates this research. The third section introduces the present study, and the fourth section details the methodology. The results are presented in the fifth section and further discussed in the sixth section. Section seven offers concluding remarks.
2 Background
2.1 Variation in /s/ production: region and speech style
Weakening of /s/ is undoubtedly the most studied phonological process in Spanish linguistics. Succinctly put, /s/ exhibits tendency towards debuccalization and deletion in the coda environment, both word-internally and across word boundaries, especially when preceding a consonant. The examples in (1) provide a simple sketch of the variation. One crucial distinction is the presence (pe[s]te or pe[h]te) versus absence of frication (pe[ø]te, or other attested variants with no frication such as gemination of the post-fricative consonant, e.g., pe[t̪t̪]e, or lengthening of the preceding vowel, e.g., p[e:]te). Variation in /s/ production, however, is complex, influenced by phonological, regional, social, and stylistic factors. This section does not intend to provide an exhaustive account of these patterns, but rather to zoom in on the relevant facts. For this reason, I concentrate on two aspects of the variation in word-final /s/ production: regional (Argentine vs. Dominican Spanish), and stylistic.
la peste ‘the pest’ | (i) [la.pés.t̪e] |
(ii) [la.pés.t̪e] ∼ [la.péh.t̪e] ∼ [la.pé.t̪e] |
el muslo ‘the thigh’ | (i) [el.múz.lo] |
(ii) [el.múz.lo] ∼ [el.múɦ.lo] ∼ [el.múh.lo] ∼ [el.mú.lo] |
la tos ‘the cough’ | (i) [la.t̪ós] |
(ii) [la.t̪ós] ∼ [la.t̪óh] ∼ [la.t̪ó] |
Both Argentine and Dominican Spanish are s-weakening dialects, albeit to different degrees. In the Dominican Republic, weakening of /s/, whether as aspiration or deletion, is the norm, regardless of educational attainment (Alba 1982, 1990; Henríquez Ureña 1940; Jiménez Sabater 1975; López Morales 1990; Núñez-Cedeño 1980; Terrell 1986). López Morales (1990: 130) reports deletion rates ranging from 98 % among illiterate speakers to 66 % among college-educated participants. He does not report aspiration rates, which he considers it as instances of retention of /s/. Alba (1990: 60), however, reports 90 and 25 % deletion rates for the same populations, respectively. In Argentina, the scope of weakening has been more limited, with low rates reported, at least among educated speakers. Terrell (1978: 44) reports aspiration at a 40 % average rate and deletion at 14 %. Thus, a key difference between these dialects is the rate of /s/ deletion, which becomes even more disparate in educated speech. In fact, when compared to Dominican Spanish, Argentine Spanish remains a somewhat conservative dialect in this respect (Hualde 2014: 159).
Regional variation aside, /s/ production is also susceptible to the effects of (broadly defined) speech style. Following a Labovian sociolinguistic model, weakening is typically more prevalent when speakers pay little attention to speech, as is the case with informal speech in sociolinguistic interviews (Labov 1972). When attention to speech is encouraged (e.g., when style becomes more formal), as is the case with a word-list task, ‘standard’ or prestige variants (i.e., in many speech communities, mostly retained [s], or possibly [h]; crucially not [ø])[1] increase in frequency of use. Of relevance to the present study is Alba (2011), which investigated speech patterns among television announcers and reporters in the Dominican Republic. He reported that even though weakened variants – [ø] and [h] – are prevalent across all social groups, newscasters in Dominican television produce fully retained variants of /s/. The use of cameras, studio lights, and a wide and public audience elicit a more formal and careful speech style from these speakers. To this end, Alba (2011) analyzed ten TV news segments of 10 min each to understand the patterns of variation. A key insight from this study was that people on television (including interviewees, reporters, and news anchors) retained /s/ at a rate of 63 %, five times the rate that is observed in spontaneous learned speech (or habla culta, the targeted audience for news reporting). In fact, when only news reporters were considered (that is, no interviewees), there was no variation, with retained /s/ being the only variant observed (i.e., no [h] nor deletion). Additionally, a qualitative analysis of a few tokens of what some could label as an “exaggerated” /s/ showed an average duration of 163 ms, compared to 93 ms for spontaneous learned speech. Regarding newscaster /s/ in other /s/-weakening dialects (including Argentina), Alba (2011) noted that the most frequent variant was typically [h], and not full [s]. This is confirmed by Colantoni and Hualde (2013: 29) who, writing about the phonetics and phonology of Argentine Spanish, make the observation that Argentine newscasters default to aspiration, unlike what may be found in other areas, such as Southern Spain and the Hispanic Caribbean. Thus, the repertoire of newscaster speech in some [s]-weakening dialects includes lenited variants such as [h]. In the Dominican Republic, this does not appear to be the case. The findings in Alba (2011) suggest that one way in which attention to speech surfaces among Dominicans may be by means of a fully retained /s/, even though it may not be the expected realization elsewhere in Dominican society.
2.2 Regional variation in nasal consonant weakening and anticipatory vowel nasalization
The nasal system in Spanish includes three consonant phonemes that contrast in place of articulation. As shown in (2), bilabial, alveolar and palatal nasal consonants freely occur in the syllable onset.
cama [ká.ma] ‘bed’ | mata [má.t̪a] ‘s/he kills’ |
cana [ká.na] ‘fishing rod, reed’ | nata [ná.t̪a] ‘cream’ |
caña [ká.ɲa] ‘grey hair’ | ñata [ɲá.t̪a] ‘nose’ (colloquial) |
In the coda, only one nasal is permitted, /n/, which typically exhibits contextual and regional variation. As is common cross-linguistically, Spanish coda /n/ assimilates to the place of articulation of the consonant that follows (whether word-internally or across a word-boundary), such that six allophones are typically described in this domain, as shown in (3).
a. | un beso [um.bé.so] |
b. | informe [iɱ.fóɾ.me] ‘report’ |
c. | canto [kán̪.t̪o] ‘I sing’ |
d. | ansia [án.sja] ‘anxiety’ |
e. | ancho [ánj.tʃo] ‘wide’ |
f. | ángel [áŋ.xel] ‘angel’ |
Additionally, coda /n/ exhibits considerable regional variation, as shown in (4). In broad terms, dialects of Spanish belong to one of two groups with regard to this variable (Canfield 1981; Lipski 1994; Resnick 1975). A first group, which I label ‘[n]-dialects’, exhibit a tendency towards alveolar variants of /n/ in word-final position, as shown in examples (i) in (4). The exception is the case preceding a velar consonant, as in 4(c)i (following the assimilation process explained above). There is a second group, ‘[ŋ]-dialects’, which opt for the velar variant in all contexts, as seen in examples (ii) in (4), even when not preceding a velar consonant, as is the case with 4(a)ii, 4(b)ii and 4(d)ii (Bakovic 2001; Cedergren and Sankoff 1975; D’Introno and Sosa 1988; Haché de Yunén 1981; Hyman 1956; López Morales 1980; Muñiz-Cachón 2003; Salvador 1987; Terrell 1975; Trigo Ferre 1988).
pan ‘bread’ | (i) [pan] |
(ii) [pan] ∼ [paŋ] ∼ [pã] |
pan agrio ‘bitter bread’ | (i) [pá.ná.ɣɾjo] |
(ii) [pá.ná.ɣɾjo] ∼ [pa.ŋá.ɣɾjo] ∼ [pã.ɣɾjo] |
pan casero ‘homemade bread’ | (i) [páŋ.ká.se.ɾo] |
(ii) [páŋ.ká.se.ɾo] ∼ [pã́.ka.sé.ɾo] |
pan turco ‘Turkish bread’ | (i) [pán̪.t̪úɾ.ko] |
(ii) [pán̪.t̪úɾ.ko] ∼ [páŋ.t̪úɾ.ko] ∼ [pã́.t̪úɾ.ko] |
Velarization of /n/ (examples [ii]) is traditionally viewed as part of a process of nasal consonant attenuation, that by way of consonant deletion, leads to nasalization of the pre-nasal vowel. As such, [ŋ]-dialects exhibit greater anticipatory vowel nasalization (though it is not clear whether “greater” means longer time-course or larger velar port opening) compared to [n]-dialects. Given these facts, most research on word-final nasal consonant weakening and anticipatory vowel nasalization has understandably focused on [ŋ]-dialects in the Caribbean region. Whether from a formal or a sociolinguistic perspective, empirical (albeit impressionistic) research has confirmed several trends. Firstly, nasal consonants are prone to loosening of the oral constriction, particularly in the word-final domain. Backing of /n/ (i.e., [ŋ]), on the other hand, is the most common variant. Anticipatory vowel nasalization surfaces with nasal consonant deletion. Finally, given the high frequency of nasal consonant deletion for some [ŋ]-dialects, it is generally accepted that nasalization in the pre-nasal vowel may have conventionalized and putatively phonologized. That is, nasalization has become a feature of the vowel and no longer a marker of nasal consonant weakening.
In the last two decades, instrumental work has also enriched the research landscape by providing detailed phonetic characterizations of variation patterns, as well as adding comparisons to [n]-dialects. For example, electropalatographic studies have confirmed more back articulations of the nasal consonants in [ŋ]-dialects, in addition to lesser degrees of constriction, when compared to [n]-dialects (Colantoni and Kochetov 2012; Ramsammy 2013). Using a nasograph,[2] Lederer (2000, cited in 2003) probed regional differences by comparing the time-course of nasalization in an [n]-dialect (Peninsular Spanish) and an [ŋ]-dialect (Cuban Spanish). Following Solé (1992), who showed that the onset of nasalization of phonetic/coarticulatory (Peninsular Spanish) and phonologized (American English) nasalization are different, Lederer found that the patterns of anticipatory nasalization in the [ŋ]-dialect were more similar to those of English (which exhibits fully nasalized pre-nasal vowel allophones), than to those of the [n]-dialect.
The crucial link between nasal weakening and anticipatory nasalization is the potential for realignment of oral and nasal gestures, a well-documented historical and synchronic pattern (e.g., Beddor 2009; Hajek 1997; Zhang 2000): as weakening of the nasal consonant increases in magnitude, so does the temporal extent of nasalization in the pre-nasal vowel. However, investigating this relationship among dialects of Spanish, whether [n]- or [ŋ]-, has not been possible so far due to the nature of electropalatography and nasography. Electropalatography is unable to capture velum lowering, while nasography is unable to characterize the degree of constriction. Recent work employing nasometry has permitted such an analysis, to which we turn next.
Building on the cross-dialectal findings in the extant literature, Bongiovanni (2021a, 2021b) examined nasal consonant weakening and anticipatory vowel nasalization in Santo Domingo Spanish (Dominican Republic, an [ŋ]-dialect) and Buenos Aires Spanish (Argentina, an [n]-dialect). Results in Bongiovanni (2021a) showed earlier onset of nasalization for the Santo Domingo data compared to Buenos Aires Spanish. Bongiovanni (2021b), on the other hand, correlated the nasalization findings with weakening of the word-final nasal consonant. A crucial – and unexpected – finding was that earlier onset of nasalization obtained with little – and arguably without – weakening of the nasal. Specifically, findings revealed similar degrees of nasal consonant weakening (measured in terms of duration and consonant-to-vowel ratio) across dialect groups, unlike what would have been predicted based on previous research. These findings suggested that differences in nasalization cannot solely be attributed to weakening of the nasal consonant. Thus, this instrumental work provided experimental support to the hypothesis of phonological differences across dialects: for the Santo Domingo data, nasalization may no longer be a marker of nasal consonant weakening, but a feature of the pre-nasal vowel allophone.
The lack of nasal consonant weakening, particularly in the Santo Domingo corpus, requires further explanation. Bongiovanni (2021b) hypothesized that the findings were the result of heightened attention to speech, due to a combination of factors, including the use of specialized equipment, a reading task, well-known differences between dialect groups regarding the evaluation of their own variety (Büdenbender 2010; Bugel 2012; Llull and Pinardi 2014; Toribio 2000), and an out-of-community researcher for the Dominican group. There were also more reading errors in the Dominican than in the Argentine group. Additionally, some Dominican participants exhibited dialect “inappropriate” variants, such as the use of the interdental fricative, [θ], for the graphemes <c, z> (a total of 13 tokens, or 0.1 % of the corpus, produced by four speakers: three women and one man), which suggests a perceived “supra-regional” norm.[3] Far from (negatively) evaluating participants’ performance, I take these pieces as anecdotal evidence of participants attempting a ‘more formal’ speech style or, at the very least, heightened attention to speech.
3 The present study
The present study examines the production of nasal consonant weakening and anticipatory vowel nasalization as a function of (broadly defined) attention to speech, operationalized in terms of the production of coda /s/. Willis and Ronquest (2022), which provides a relevant framework to study participants’ potential style shift due to data elicitations methods, serves as the inspiration for this study. The hypothesis is that more weakened /s/ will co-occur with weakened /n/ and earlier onset of anticipatory vowel nasalization. However, given that there is some evidence that anticipatory vowel nasalization is not dependent on /n/ weakening for Dominican Spanish (Bongiovanni 2021b), it is entirely possible that for this dialect, weakening of /s/ does not correlate to anticipatory vowel nasalization.
Expanding Willis and Ronquest (2022) promises to shed light on whether a well-known sociolinguistic variable can be a viable heuristic to better understand speech patterns in laboratory-oriented studies. Additionally, the present study extends the analysis to variables that operate below conscious awareness. Sibilant weakening may elicit negative speaker attitudes, which can range from informal or uneducated to even unauthentic, depending on the community (e.g., Bullock et al. 2014; Chappell 2019, 2021; Lafford 1986; Waltermire 2021). Variation regarding /l/ production is also heavily evaluated in the Cibao region (Alba 2004: 90–93). With /n/, to the best of my knowledge, no such evaluations have been documented. That is, for the most part, speakers are aware of /s/ weakening, whereas neither /n/ variation nor anticipatory vowel nasalization elicit the same evaluations. Rather than undermining the research undertaken here, these differences bring to the forefront the need to extend Willis and Ronquest (2022) to a variable that remains below the level of consciousness, distinct from /l/ in the Cibao region and /s/ cross-dialectally.
4 Methods
4.1 Participants and equipment
This study compares Buenos Aires (Argentina) and Santo Domingo (Dominican Republic) Spanish. Data come from 54 native speakers from these metropolises (Santo Domingo, n = 28, aged 18–27 years old, 18 women and 10 men; Buenos Aires, n = 26, aged 19–28 years old, 18 women and 8 men). Except for two who did not answer, all Dominicans lived in Santo Domingo. Four participants reported having also lived in Jarabocoa (n = 1, until the age of 10), Monseñor Nouel (n = 1, until the age of 16) and Monte Plata (n = 2, both until early adolescence). All Argentine participants, with the exception of two who did not provide this information, reported living in Buenos Aires (or the greater metropolitan area) for their entire lives.
All participants were university students at the time of data collection. While working with young, educated participants raises questions about the representativeness of the sample and, thus, the generalizability of the results to other populations, this choice was made to ensure comparability across dialect groups: even as Santo Domingo and Buenos Aires differ in territorial and population size, each serves as the political, economic, and cultural center of its respective country.
Recordings were made with a Glottal Enterprises Nasometer, a split set of microphones divided by a plate that rests above the speaker’s lip. This tool allows for stereo recordings, with separate tracks for oral and nasal cavities respectively, and is ideal for analyses of nasalization (see spectrogram in the next subsection). Given its lightweight, portable nature and lack of calibration requirements, this device is particularly well-suited for field data collection, as employed in this study. Recordings were done in Praat (Boersma and Weenink 2016), in stereo, and sampled at 44.1 kHz.
4.2 Materials
Target stimuli were real words, chosen according to a number of linguistic variables: height of the pre-nasal vowel (/a e o/), stress (stressed, unstressed), syllable type (CVC, CVN, NVN, NV) and whether or not the nasal consonant was in an assimilatory context (pre-consonantal vs. pre-vocalic/pre-pausal). Note that these variables will not be analyzed here, as they were the object of study in Bongiovanni (2021a, 2021b). The focus of the present paper is the comparison of CVC and CVN. In order to keep the task from feeling too repetitive, target words were embedded in noun and verb phrases (e.g., el capitán tacaño ‘the stingy captain’) rather than reusing the same carrier sentence (see Appendix for a full list of elicitation materials).
Elicitation materials were displayed in Microsoft PowerPoint on the same laptop used in recording (a MacbookPro with a 13″ screen). Slides were randomized and set up to advance automatically in order to standardize speech rate across repetitions (to the extent possible).
4.3 Annotation and instrumental analysis
A total of 7,639 tokens were extracted, of which 3,812 were CVN and 3,827 were CVC. Two hundred and forty-two (242) were eliminated from analysis primarily due to misreading and disfluencies, thus 7,397 were analyzed in total. Praat was used for segmentation and analysis. Given that recordings were in stereo, with each channel recording material from different cavities in the vocal tract, the annotation and segmentation of target sounds utilized cues in each channel, as well as the spectrogram. Figure 1 presents spectrogram and waveform samples illustrating the target segments. The onset of the vowel was marked at the beginning of periodicity in the waveform and consistent formant structure in the spectrogram. With regard to the nasal consonant, a sudden absence of audio in the oral channel (Channel 1) and presence of intensity in the nasal channel (Channel 2) were taken as indicators of onset of the nasal consonant. To segment /s/, the converse criteria were employed (i.e., presence of intensity in the oral channel and a flat waveform in the nasal channel), as well as an aperiodic waveform typical of fricative sounds. See below procedure for when no oral constriction was identified (that is, deleted tokens), whether /s/ or /n/.

Waveform and spectrogram of the words sartén ‘skillet’ (top) and veintidos ‘twenty-two’ (bottom), produced by a Santo Domingo speaker.
Weakening of the coda consonant, oral and nasal, was analyzed in terms of several acoustic cues. Following more recent research on Spanish fricatives (e.g., Brogan and Bolyanatz 2018; Erker 2010, 2012; File-Muriel and Brown 2011), acoustic cues were preferred over impressionist coding as they allow the researcher to capture production of /s/ without over imposing categories. Weakening of /s/ was examined in terms of three cues: duration, center of gravity (COG) and zero-crossings. Center of gravity characterizes the frequency at which sonic energy is maximally concentrated; it is the mean frequency of the spectra over a duration of time. Lowering of COG is interpreted as a weakening tendency (File-Muriel and Brown 2011: 228). In a similar vein, the rate of zero-crossings characterizes acoustic differences between productions of /s/. Compared to [h], [s] tends to have energy concentrated in a high frequency range, resulting in a higher zero-crossing density (Ruch and Harrington 2014). For this reason, the number of zero-crossings was divided by the duration of the frication interval. Extraction of sibilant acoustic parameters were automated with a Praat script (Elvira-García 2014). For deleted observations, duration and rate of zero-crossings were set at zero. A zero value for COG is not interpretable, and for this reason, deleted tokens were removed from the COG analysis.
With regard to /n/, for simplicity, I only examine weakening in terms of temporal reduction (i.e., duration).[4] When no oral constriction was identified, duration was established at zero. In the case of nasalization, the nasal track was used as a proxy for velum lowering: when the velum is lowered, the acoustic signal increases in the nasal track. Thus, energy measurements (in pascals) were taken at 20 equidistant time points during the course of the pre-nasal vowel and another 20 during the nasal consonant, automated with Praat scripts written by the author for the purpose of this research. Energy measurements were then smoothed using a 5-point window. For each individual token, the difference between the highest and the lowest energy reading in the vowel-nasal sequence was calculated. A 15 % of this difference was established as noise band. Onset of nasalization was operationalized as the first time point at which the nasal energy surpassed the 15 % noise band (Bongiovanni 2021a, 2021b; Delvaux et al. 2008; Solé 1992). Given that consonantal duration and vowel nasalization values vary as a function of vowel type, stress, and environment (Bongiovanni 2021b; Clumeck 1976; Krakow 1999), prior to running any statistical analyses, averages of duration and onset of nasalization were calculated per speaker, vowel type, environment, and stress conditions. That is, averages were calculated within speaker and within conditions.
All visualizations in this article were done in R (R Core Team 2019), using the tidyverse suite of packages (Wickham et al. 2019) and the lmer() function (Bates et al. 2015). Statistical modelling was also done in R (details are provided in the next section). Prior to statistical analysis, duration and COG measurements were normalized using the scale() function (Winter 2019).
5 Results
5.1 Acoustic characterization of /s/
Given the documented regional and stylistic variation in /s/ production, I first present the acoustic characterization of /s/ production in both dialects. Prior to examining the three acoustic correlates (duration, COG, and rate of zero-crossings), the data was explored for instances of deletion. The Buenos Aires data showed 37 observations of elided /s/, which represent a 2 % application rate. The Santo Domingo data, on the other hand, presented 102 observations of deleted /s/, a frequency of occurrence of 5.6 %. The rate of deletion is in keeping with the previous literature documenting that Santo Domingo Spanish would exhibit a higher frequency of deletion. Nevertheless, the percentage of use of deletion was well below the numbers reported in the literature for either dialect (c.f. Alba 1990; Terrell 1978).
Regarding the acoustic parameters of /s/ production, Figures 2 through 4 illustrate the distribution of the data, per dialect group, for duration, COG and rate of zero-crossings, respectively.

Boxplot and violin plot of (raw) duration of /s/ in ms, per dialect group. Diamonds represent the mean.

Boxplot and violin plot of (raw) COG measurements, per dialect group. Diamonds represent the mean.

Density plot of zero-crossing rate, per dialect group. Diamonds represent the mean.
Starting with duration, the dialects exhibited great acoustic overlap in the temporal dimension (Figure 2). Contrary to what the previous literature would predict, Santo Domingo Spanish exhibited considerably longer productions of /s/ (Santo Domingo, M = 87.3, SD = 74.4; Buenos Aires, M = 79.6, SD = 95.4). Figure 3 showed that the distribution of COG across dialect groups was skewed in opposite directions, with higher values for Santo Domingo compared to Buenos Aires (Santo Domingo, M = 4,701, SD = 1,151; Buenos Aires, M = 3,411, SD = 1,413). Zero-crossing rates in Figure 4, on the other hand, showed much a wider distribution (i.e., more variability) for the Santo Domingo data in comparison to Buenos Aires, which presented data tightly concentrated towards the lower end of the range (Santo Domingo, M = 42, SD = 27.8; Buenos Aires, M = 24.5, SD = 20.4). Welch two-sample t-tests, one per acoustic parameter, showed that group differences were statistically significant (duration: t (3,490.2) = −2.7, p < 0.01; COG: t (3,455.5) = −29.8, p < 0.001; zero-crossing rates: t (3,232.3) = −24.3, p ≤ 0.001).
An important observation is that the three acoustic parameters represent different acoustic manifestations of /s/ weakening. Therefore, we would expect to see evidence of collinearity. To this end, a Pearson’s correlation test between the acoustic measurements was run. Duration of /s/ and COG were weakly correlated, r (3,675) = 0.17, p < 0.001. Duration of /s/ and rate of zero-crossings were moderately correlated, r (3675) = 0.32, p < 0.001. COG and rate of zero-crossings were strongly correlated, r (3536) = 0.6, p < 0.001.
In closing this section, there are two main take-aways from these data. First, findings suggest that productions of /s/ were significantly more lenited for Buenos Aires than for Santo Domingo (despite the latter dialect exhibiting more instances of deletion). This finding offers some evidence that Santo Domingo speakers’ attention to speech was heightened compared to that of Buenos Aires. To fully diagnose this scenario a comparison with these participants’ speech in other communicative situations (such as sociolinguistic interviews) would be necessary. The lack of such comparison does not hinder the analysis. For the Santo Domingo data, these findings are in stark contrast with the previous literature of “rampant” weakening. Second, the fact that COG and rate of zero-crossings showed the same patterns as observed in the temporal domain indicates that results cannot solely be due to speech rate differences.
5.2 Vowel nasalization, nasal consonant weakening and /s/ production
Prior to presenting the analyses, two reminders are in order. First, averages were calculated per speaker and per phonological context (vowel type, following segment, stress). That is, each speakers’ productions of the word-final consonant in, for example, C-án#t (/a/, stressed and in pre-coronal position) were averaged together. Each point in the scatterplot represented values for the exact same phonological context. Thus, average values of duration of /n/ in C-án#t were compared to duration of /s/ in the exact same context (i.e., C-ás#t) for that particular speaker. In other words, the measurements indexed by each point were taken in equivalent contexts. Second, for deleted consonants, duration was set at zero.
Due to the present of collinearity between acoustic parameterse of /s/, I will focus on duration of /s/ in relation to nasal consonant weakening and onset of nasalization. The correlation between duration of /s/, COG and ratio of zero-crossings renders the information redundant, potentially complicating the interpretation of results. Additionally, the choice to focus on duration in the analysis aims to maintain a temporal comparison with /n/ and nasalization. By homing in on the duration of /s/, I aim to streamline the analysis and avoid the challenges associated with disentangling the unique contributions of each parameter in the context of the study.
Figure 5 presents scatterplots of average duration of /s/, as a function of the average durations of the nasal consonant (top) and onset of nasalization (bottom). Starting with the relationship between duration of /n/ and duration of /s/, visual inspection of the data showed fewer tokens towards the left of the scatterplot (the side that would reflect shorter sibilants) than expected, considering that both dialects are /s/-weakening. Another readily visible trend is the positive relationship between weakening of /s/ and weakening of /n/. In other words, longer productions of /s/, arguably signaling more speech monitoring, co-occurred with longer nasal consonants.

Duration of /n/ (top) and onset of nasalization (bottom) as a function of duration of /s/.
To test whether the relationship between duration of /n/ and duration of /s/ was statistically significant, a mixed effects linear regression model was run, with duration of the nasal consonant as the dependent variable, duration of /s/ and dialect group as the fixed effects, duration of /s/ by dialect group as interaction term, with a by-speaker random intercept. Buenos Aires Spanish was the reference level. The results are summarized in Table 1. Results indicated a fixed effect for duration of /s/ in both dialects. Dialect group and the interaction term with duration of /s/ were not statistically significant. These results indicated that longer durations of /s/ co-occurred with longer durations of /s/. As a reminder, given that other acoustic parameters of /s/ were collinear with duration, this is a result that cannot be exclusively attributed to speech rate differences.
Mixed effects linear regression model for Buenos Aires predicting duration of the nasal consonant.
Estimate | Std. error | df | t-value | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 0.19 | 0.06 | 51.44 | 3.28 | <0.01 |
Mean duration /s/ | 0.87 | 0.04 | 1,226.19 | 21.84 | <0.001 |
Dialect group: SDS | −0.13 | 0.08 | 51.88 | −1.60 | Not significant |
Mean duration of /s/ × dialect group | 0.05 | 0.06 | 1,248.74 | 0.93 | Not significant |
Moving on to the relationship between onset of nasalization and duration of /s/, visual inspection of the data suggests a positive relationship for Buenos Aires, but not for Santo Domingo. That is, for Buenos Aires Spanish, longer word-final /s/ co-occurred with earlier onset of nasalization in pre-nasal vowels. For Santo Domingo Spanish, on the other hand, there appeared to be no relationship, whether positive (both measurements move in the same direction) or negative (as one decreases, the other increases). A second mixed-effects linear regression model was run. The onset of nasalization was entered into the model as dependent variable; fixed effects included duration of /s/, dialect group, and the interaction term between duration of /s/ and dialect group. Speaker was entered as random effect. Buenos Aires Spanish was the reference level here as well. Table 2 summarizes the results.
Mixed effects linear regression model for Buenos Aires predicting onset of nasalization.
Estimate | Std. error | df | t value | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | −6.48 | 0.40 | 51.58 | −16.07 | <0.001 |
Mean duration /s/ | 1.07 | 0.18 | 1,215.71 | 5.8 | <0.001 |
Dialect group: SDS | −2.25 | 0.56 | 51.82 | −4.01 | <0.001 |
Duration of /s/ × dialect group SDS | −1.07 | 0.27 | 1,229.93 | −3.96 | <0.001 |
Findings indicated that mean duration of /s/ and dialect group were significant main effects, as was the interaction term between the two. The pattern of earlier onset of nasalization co-occurred with longer durations of /s/ for Buenos Aires Spanish, but the same effect was not true for Santo Domingo Spanish.
6 Discussion and conclusions
Can a well-known sociolinguistic variable be used to probe speech patterns in laboratory-oriented studies? The present study employed the acoustics of coda /s/ as a proxy for (broadly defined) attention to speech. To this end, it examined weakening of the word-final consonant and the time-course of nasalization in the pre-nasal vowel as a function of weakening of /s/. If /s/ production is a reflex of attention to speech, with retained /s/ signaling more speech monitoring, would longer /s/ co-occur with less weakened nasal consonants and shorter extent of anticipatory nasalization? Briefly summarized, the findings showed a relationship between longer realizations of the sibilant and longer nasal consonants, a trend for both dialect groups. When the time-course of nasalization was examined, shorter durations of /s/ co-occurred with earlier onset of nasalization for Buenos Aires but not Santo Domingo Spanish. These findings indicate that both weakening of /n/ and earlier onset of nasalization may be features of monitored speech for Buenos Aires. For Santo Domingo Spanish, on the other hand, the realization of /n/ and that of /s/ may be susceptible to the pressures of attention to speech, but it may not be the case for onset of nasalization. I return to this issue later when I address phonological differences between dialects. The remainder of this section addresses five points of discussion.
Considering the documented tendency towards /s/ weakening for both dialects, the pervasive presence of frication (whether [s] or [h]) offers evidence of heightened attention to speech. While exhibiting more occurrences of deletion, when all three acoustic parameters are taken together, Santo Domingo /s/ production exhibited less weakening than Buenos Aires. In fact, unlike what is the general trend for Dominican Spanish, these data showed frication akin to Alba’s (2011) newscaster speech patterns: under the pressure of a perceived expectation of formal speech, retained /s/ far exceeded the norms documented in this community. Alba (2011) only includes duration measurements, but the addition of COG and zero-crossings in this study provided additional support. Taken together, the comparison against /s/ suggests that production of /n/ is sensitive to monitored speech cross-dialectally, whereas dialects differ with respect to anticipatory vowel nasalization. The lack of expected /n/ weakening in Bongiovanni (2021b) can now be better understood.
The expectation of formal speech was likely a source of the heightened attention to speech among Dominican speakers. I also hypothesize that the presence of an out-of-community researcher and the bulky piece of equipment (i.e., the nasometer) may have enhanced linguistic awareness. Linguistic insecurity may also have played a role (see Alba 2009; Bongiovanni 2021b). There is no doubt of dialectal differences regarding participants’ attitude towards their own Spanish variety. It is well-documented that Dominican speakers typically exhibit insecurity regarding their dialect (Alba 2004; Büdenbender 2010; Bullock and Toribio 2014; Haché de Yunén 2008; Toribio 2000). With Argentines, research has shown great linguistic pride, even though they may judge North-Central Peninsular Spanish as ‘more correct’ (Bugel 2012; Llull and Pinardi 2014). At the same time, however, and particularly for the Santo Domingo group, if the register calls for formal speech, and a retained (and arguably, ‘exaggerated’) /s/ is part of the formal speech repertoire, linguistic insecurity as source becomes troublesome. Yet, I cannot fully discard the role of linguistic insecurity in shaping variation patterns. Taken together, these all are well-known causes for heightened attention to speech; I leave it to future research to probe this question further.
This study adds an interpretative layer to better understand the patterns of variation. The findings in the present study underscore the phonological interpretation in Bongiovanni (2021b). The fact that earlier anticipatory vowel nasalization co-occurred with constricted nasal consonants was viewed as evidence that, for Dominican Spanish, nasalization in the pre-nasal vowel may be a feature of the vowel and was not merely a marker of nasal consonant weakening. The findings in this study are compatible with this scenario. For Dominicans, even when more carefully enunciating as is the case of read speech, anticipatory vowel nasalization may arguably be a feature of the vowel, and therefore not dependent on patterns of consonantal weakening. In other words, once nasalization has conventionalized as a feature of the vowel, it no longer surfaces exclusively with weakening of /n/. As a result, earlier nasalization may be available when speech monitoring is heightened as much as in all other speech situations. Triangulating the analysis to include /s/ production offered a more nuanced understanding of the variation in the data set.
There are also methodological implications. The findings herein point to the need to consider these effects in the research design and data collection practices in laboratory-oriented studies. For example, while not imperative, researchers investigating variables well known for sociolinguistic variation may find value in involving community members in data collection, acknowledging the need for researchers to exercise their best judgment in making such decisions. Additionally, if the task resembles an activity often performed in school (as is the case of reading), avoiding repetitive carrier phrases, or creating a story line around the stimuli, may lessen some of these effects. In this study, I explicitly avoided the use of ‘traditional’ carrier phrases and created a story line with an eccentric poet to account for the unusual sentences to address these effects. Finally, by incorporating variables whose patterns of variation are well-documented as control variables, or even distractors, we gain additional data points from which to better interpret laboratory data.
More importantly, this study also contributes to conversations regarding the relationship between sociolinguistic and laboratories techniques for eliciting and analyzing data. For example, with naturalistic data, examining certain variables (such as the effect of stress, or specific phonetic and phonological environments) may be challenging due to data sparsity. Laboratory studies often sidestep this issue due to their experimental nature, which allows more control. While more balanced and/or nuanced characterizations may be achievable, it is often at the expense of naturalness of speech. Sociolinguistic methods, on the other hand, ought to have an impact on the way experimental studies are designed and findings considered. Enabling a detailed examination of phonetic variation within broader regional and social contexts, the interplay between laboratory and sociolinguistic methods deepens our understanding regarding underlying mechanisms of human language, and how they interact with variation and change. This study presents a good example of how laboratory and sociolinguistic methods come together to unearth patterns of variation that would otherwise go unnoticed.
Acknowledgments
I would like to thank the Editor and the Assoc. Editor of Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics for their input and guidance through the process. I am indebted to two anonymous reviewers for their extensive feedback. I would also like to thank Gabriela Alfaraz for reading earlier versions of this manuscript. The original data collection was part of my doctoral work, which was supervised by Kenneth de Jong and Erik Willis. I thank them for their continued support and mentorship. Portions of this paper were presented at the Linguistics Symposium in Romance Languages, organized by University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, in April 2021. As always, all shortcomings remain my own.
Reading materials
Reading items for CVN syllables.
Stressed | Unstressed |
---|---|
La sartén torcida
‘The crooked skillet’ Un capitán tacaño ‘A cheap captain’ Un tirón terrible ‘A terrible pull’ |
Que visiten temprano
‘May [they] visit early’ Esperan tomando ‘[They] wait drinking’ Cuando tiraron tabaco ‘When [they] threw tocacco’ |
La sartén caliente
‘The hot skillet’ Un capitán casado ‘A married captain’ Un tirón cargado ‘A loaded pull’ |
Que visiten cantando
‘May [they] visit singing’ Esperan callados ‘[They] wait in silence’ Cuando tiraron cadenas ‘When [they] thew chains’ |
La única sartén
‘The only skillet’ Un joven capitán ‘A young captain’ Pasó de un tirón ‘It happened in a go’ |
Que nos visiten
‘May they visit’ Y ahí esperan ‘And there they were’ Digo que tiraron ‘I say they threw’ |
La sartén aceitosa
‘The oily skillet’ Un capitán astuto ‘A clever captain’ Un tirón atento ‘An attentive pull’ |
Cuando visiten Armenia
‘When [they] visit Armenia’ Siempre esperan atentos ‘They always wait attentively’ Cuando tiraron azúcar ‘When they throwed sugar |
Reading items for CVC syllables.
Stressed | Unstressed |
---|---|
Dos cafés templados
‘Two warm coffee houses’ Los papás tacaños ‘The stingy parents’ Los veintidós tomates ‘Twenty-two tomatoes’ |
Los peces torcidos
‘The crooked fish’ Las papas templadas ‘The warm potatos’ Los casos terribles ‘The terrible cases’ |
Ocho cafés calientes
‘Three hot coffees’ Los papás casados ‘The married parents’ Las veintidós cadenas ‘Twenty two chains’ |
Los peces carnosos
‘The fleshy fish’ Las papas calientes ‘The hot potatoes’ Los casos cargados ‘The loaded cases’ |
Los grandes cafés
‘The big coffee houses’ La reunión de papás ‘The parent meeting’ El número veintidós ‘The number twenty two’ |
Los coloridos peces
‘The colorful fish’ La bolsa de papas ‘The bag of potatoes’ Los misteriosos casos ‘The mysterious cases’ |
Los cafés azules
‘The blue coffee houses’ Los papás astutos ‘The clever parents’ Los veintidós aliados ‘The twenty two allies’ |
Los peces azules
‘The blue fish’ Las casas azules ‘Blue houses’ Los casos astutos ‘The clever cases’ |
References
Alba, Orlando. 1982. El español del Caribe: ponencias del VI Simposio de Dialectología. Santiago, República Domincana: Universidad Católica Madre y Maestra, Departamento de Publicaciones.Search in Google Scholar
Alba, Orlando. 1990. Variación fonética y diversidad social en el español de Santiago. Santiago de los Caballeros: Universidad Católica Madre y Maestra.Search in Google Scholar
Alba, Orlando. 2004. Cómo hablamos los dominicanos: un enfoque sociolingüístico. Santo Domingo: Grupo León Jimenes.Search in Google Scholar
Alba, Orlando. 2009. La identidad lingüística de los dominicanos. Santo Domingo: Ediciones Librería La Trinitaria.Search in Google Scholar
Alba, Orlando. 2011. Hipercorrección en los programas de noticias en la televisión de la República Dominicana. In Raúl Ávila, Rodrigo Muñoz & Carlos Garduños (eds.), Variación del español en los medios, 165–192. Mexico City: Colegio de México.10.2307/j.ctv6mtd9s.12Search in Google Scholar
Bakovic, Eric. 2001. Nasal place neutralization in Spanish. In Michelle Minnick Fox, Alexander Williams & Ellen Kaisser (eds.), Proceedings of the 24th annual Penn Linguistics Colloquium (Penn Working Papers in Linguistics), 1–12. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania.Search in Google Scholar
Bates, Douglas, Martin Mächler, Ben Bolker & Steve Walker. 2015. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67(1). 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01.Search in Google Scholar
Beddor, Patrice. 2009. A coarticulatory path to sound change. Language 85(4). 785–821. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.0.0165.Search in Google Scholar
Boersma, Paul & David Weenink. 2016. Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (version 6.0.16). [Computer program]. http://www.praat.org/ (accessed 5 April 2016).Search in Google Scholar
Bongiovanni, Silvina. 2021a. Acoustic investigation of anticipatory vowel nasalization in a Caribbean and a non-Caribbean dialect of Spanish. Linguistics Vanguard 7(1). 20200008. https://doi.org/10.1515/lingvan-2020-0008.Search in Google Scholar
Bongiovanni, Silvina. 2021b. On covariation between nasal consonant weakening and anticipatory vowel nasalization: Evidence from a Caribbean and a non-Caribbean dialect of Spanish. Laboratory Phonology 12(1). 1–30. https://doi.org/10.16995/labphon.6444.Search in Google Scholar
Brogan, Franny. 2020. Demystifying Salvadoran [sθ]: Evidence for /s/ lenition. In Alfonso Morales-Front, Michael J. Ferreira, Ronald P. Leow & Cristina Sanz (eds.), Hispanic linguistics: Current issues and new directions, 205–228. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/ihll.26.10broSearch in Google Scholar
Brogan, Franny D. & Mariška A. Bolyanatz. 2018. A sociophonetic account of onset /s/ weakening in Salvadoran Spanish: Instrumental and segmental analyses. Language Variation and Change 30(2). 203–230. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954394518000066.Search in Google Scholar
Büdenbender, Eva-María S. 2010. Comparing Dominican linguistic (in)security in the Dominican Republic and in the diaspora. In Claudia Borgonovo, Manuel Español Echevarría & Phillipe Prévost (eds.), 12th Hispanic linguistics symposium, 148–159. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Search in Google Scholar
Bugel, Talia. 2012. Buenas y no tan buenas variedades del español: Opiniones y actitudes en Montevideo y Buenos Aires. Revista Internacional de Lingüística Iberoamericana 19. 83–107.Search in Google Scholar
Bullock, Barbara & Almeida Jacqueline Toribio. 2014. Dominican Spanish. In Marianna Di Paolo & Arthur K. Spears (eds.), Languages and dialects in the US: Focus on diversity and linguistics, 151–162. London: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar
Bullock, Barbara, Jacqueline Almeida Toribio & Mark Amengual. 2014. The status of s in Dominican Spanish. Lingua 143. 20–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2014.01.009.Search in Google Scholar
Canfield, Delos Lincoln. 1981. Spanish pronunciation in the Americas. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Search in Google Scholar
Cedergren, Henrietta. 1973. The interplay of social and linguistic factors in Panama. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Cedergren, Henrietta & David Sankoff. 1975. Nasals: A sociolinguistic study of change in progress. In Charles Ferguson, Larry Hyman & John Ohala (eds.), Papers from a symposium on nasals and nasalization, 67–80. Stanford, CA: Stanford University.Search in Google Scholar
Chappell, Whitney. 2019. ¿Caribeño o mexicano, profesionista o albañil? Mexican listeners’ evaluations of /s/ aspiration and maintenance in Mexican and Puerto Rican voices. Sociolinguistic Studies (12). 367–393. https://doi.org/10.1558/sols.34154.Search in Google Scholar
Chappell, Whitney. 2021. /s/ weakening in Nicaragua. In Eva Núñez-Méndez (ed.), Sociolinguistic approaches to sibilant variation in Spanish, 217–245. London: Routledge.10.4324/9781003153948-11Search in Google Scholar
Clumeck, Harold. 1976. Patterns of soft palate movements in six languages. Journal of Phonetics 4(4). 337–351. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0095-4470(19)31260-4.Search in Google Scholar
Colantoni, Laura & José Ignacio Hualde. 2013. Introducción: variación fonológica en el español de la Argentina. In Laura Colantoni & Celeste Rodríguez-Louro (eds.), Perspectivas teóricas y experimentales sobre el español de la Argentina, 21–35. Madrid: Iberoamericana.10.31819/9783954871971-003Search in Google Scholar
Colantoni, Laura & Alexei Kochetov. 2012. Nasal variability and speech style: An EPG study of word-final nasals in two Spanish dialects. Italian Journal of Linguistics/Rivista di Linguistica 24. 11–42.Search in Google Scholar
D’Introno, Francesco & Juan Manuel Sosa. 1988. Elisió de nasal o nasalizaciõ de vocal eŋ caraqueño. In Robert Matthew Hammond & Melvin Resnick (eds.), Studies in Caribbean Spanish dialectology, 24–34. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Delvaux, Veronique, Didier Demolin, Bernard Harmegnies & Alain Soquet. 2008. The aerodynamics of nasalization in French. Journal of Phonetics 36(4). 578–606. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2008.02.002.Search in Google Scholar
Elvira-García, Wendy. 2014. Zero-crossing-and-spectral-moments, v.1.3. [Praat script]. Available at: http://stel.ub.edu/labfon/sites/default/files/zero-crossing-and-spectral-moments13.praat.Search in Google Scholar
Erker, Daniel. 2010. A subsegmental approach to coda/s/weakening in Dominican Spanish. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 203. 9–26. https://doi.org/10.1515/ijsl.2010.019.Search in Google Scholar
Erker, Daniel. 2012. An acoustically based sociolinguistic analysis of variable coda/s/production in the Spanish of New York City. New York: New York University dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
File-Muriel, Richard & Earl Brown. 2011. The gradient nature of s-lenition in Caleño Spanish. Language Variation and Change 23(2). 223–243. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954394511000056.Search in Google Scholar
Haché de Yunén, Ana María. 1981. La /n/ final de sílaba en el español de Santiago de los Caballeros. In Orlando Alba (ed.), El español del caribe, 143–154. Santiago de los Caballeros: Universidad Católica Madre y Maestra.Search in Google Scholar
Haché de Yunén, Ana María. 2008. Valoraciones sobre nuestra identidad lingüística, a los quince años de la puesta en práctica de un nuevo currículo en la escuela dominicana. Seminario del Caribe: Lenguas, Literaturas, Sociedades.Search in Google Scholar
Hajek, John. 1997. Universals of sound change in nasalization. Oxford: Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar
Henríquez Ureña, Pedro. 1940. El español en Santo Domingo. Buenos Aires: Biblioteca de dialectología hispanoamericana V, Facultad de Filosofía y Letras de la Universidad de Buenos Aires.Search in Google Scholar
Hualde, José Ignacio. 2014. Los sonidos del español. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511719943Search in Google Scholar
Hyman, Ruth. 1956. [ŋ] as an allophone denoting open juncture in several Spanish-American dialects. Hispania 39(3). 293–299. https://doi.org/10.2307/336007.Search in Google Scholar
Jiménez Sabater, Maximiliano. 1975. Más datos sobre el español en la República Dominicana. Santo Domingo: Ediciones Intec.Search in Google Scholar
Krakow, Rena A. 1999. Physiological organization of syllables: A review. Journal of Phonetics 27(1). 23–54. https://doi.org/10.1006/jpho.1999.0089.Search in Google Scholar
Labov, William. 1972. Sociolinguistic patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Search in Google Scholar
Lafford, Barbara. 1986. Valor diagnóstico-social del uso de ciertas variantes de /s/ en el español de Cartagena, Colombia. In Rafael Núñez Cedeño & Iraset Páez (eds.), Estudios sobre la fonología del español del Caribe, 53–75. Caracas: Casa de Bello.Search in Google Scholar
Lederer, Jenny. 2003. The diachronic coronal-velar nasal relationship. In María-Josep Solé, Daniel Recasens & Joaquin Romero (eds.), 15th international Congress of Phonetic Sciences, 2801–2804. Barcelona: Causal Productions.Search in Google Scholar
Lipski, John. 1994. Latin American Spanish. New York: Longman.Search in Google Scholar
Llull, Gabriela & Lilián C. Pinardi. 2014. Actitudes lingüísticas en la Argentina. El español en Buenos Aires: Una aproximación a las representaciones de sus hablantes. En Ana B. Chiquito & Miguel Ángel Quesada Pacheco (eds.), Actitudes lingüísticas de los hispanohablantes hacia el idioma español y sus variantes, Bergen Language and Linguistic Studies (BeLLS), vol. 5, 1–65. Bergen, Norway: The University of Bergen.10.15845/bells.v5i0.676Search in Google Scholar
López Morales, Humberto. 1980. Velarización de “n” en el español de Puerto Rico. LEA: Lingüística Española Actual 2(2). 203–217.Search in Google Scholar
López Morales, Humberto. 1990. En torno a la /s/ final dominicana: cuestiones teóricas. Voz y Letra 1(1). 129–138.Search in Google Scholar
Muñiz-Cachón, Carmen. 2003. Rasgos fónicos del español hablado en Asturias. Archivum LII–LII. 323–349.Search in Google Scholar
Núñez Cedeño, Rafael. 1980. La fonología moderna y el español de Santo Domingo. Santo Domingo: Editorial Taller.10.24201/nrfh.v29i1.2711Search in Google Scholar
Ohala, John. 1971. Monitoring soft palate movements in speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 50(1A). 140. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1977664.Search in Google Scholar
R Core Team. 2019. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Search in Google Scholar
Ramsammy, Michael. 2013. Word-final nasal velarization in Spanish. Journal of Linguistics 49(1). 215–255. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0022226712000187.Search in Google Scholar
Resnick, Melvin. 1975. Phonological variants and dialect identification in Latin American Spanish. Berlin: de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar
Rohena-Madrazo, Marcos. 2011. Sociophonetic variation in the production and perception of obstruent voicing in Buenos Aires Spanish. New York, NY: New York University dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Ruch, Hannah & Jonathan Harrington. 2014. Synchronic and diachronic factors in the change from pre-aspiration to post-aspiration in Andalusian Spanish. Journal of Phonetics 45. 12–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2014.02.009.Search in Google Scholar
Salvador, Gregorio. 1987. Estudios dialectológicos. Madrid: Paraninfo.Search in Google Scholar
Solé, María-Josep. 1992. Phonetic and phonological processes: The case of nasalization. Language and Speech 35(1–2). 29–43. https://doi.org/10.1177/002383099203500204.Search in Google Scholar
Terrell, Tracy. 1975. La nasal implosiva y final en el español de Cuba. Anuario de Letras 13. 257–271.Search in Google Scholar
Terrell, Tracy. 1978. La aspiración y elisión de /s/ en el español porteño. Anuario de Letras 16. 41–66.Search in Google Scholar
Terrell, Tracy. 1986. La desaparición de /s/ posnuclear a nivel léxico en el habla dominicana. In Rafael A. Nuñez Cedeño, Iraset Páez Urdaneta & Jorge M. Guitart (eds.), Estudios sobre la fonología del español del Caribe, 117–134. Caracas: La Casa de Bello.Search in Google Scholar
Toribio, Almeida Jacqueline. 2000. Nosotros somos dominicanos: Language and self-definition among Dominicans. In Ana Roca (ed.), Research on Spanish in the United States: Linguistic issues and challenges, 252–270. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Search in Google Scholar
Trigo Ferre, Rosario Lorenza. 1988. The phonological derivation and behavior of nasal glides. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Waltermire, Mark. 2021. Final /s/ in Uruguayan Spanish-Portuguese. In Eva Núñez-Méndez (ed.), Sociolinguistic approaches to sibilant variation in Spanish, 279–299. London: Routledge.10.4324/9781003153948-14Search in Google Scholar
Wickham, Hadley, Mara Averick, Jennifer Bryan, Winston Chang, Lucy D’Agostino McGowan, Romain François, Garrett Grolemund, Alex Hayes, Lionel Henry, Jim Hester, Max Kuhn, Thomas Lin Pedersen, Evan Miller, Stephan Milton Bache, Kirill Müller, Jeroen Ooms, David Robinson, Dana Paige Seide, Vitalie Spinu, Kohske Takahashi, Davis Vaughan, Claus Wilke, Kara Woo & Hiroaki Yutani. 2019. Welcome to the tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software 4(43). 1686. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.01686.Search in Google Scholar
Willis, Erik & Rebecca Ronquest. 2022. A sociophonetic exploration of coda liquids and vocalization in Cibao Dominican Spanish. In Manuel Díaz-Campos (ed.), The Routledge handbook of variationist approaches to Spanish, 207–230. London: Routledge.10.4324/9780429200267-20Search in Google Scholar
Winter, Bodo. 2019. Statistics for linguists: An introduction using R. London: Routledge.10.4324/9781315165547Search in Google Scholar
Zhang, Jie. 2000. Non-contrastive features and categorical patterning in Chinese diminutive suffixation: “Max [F]” or “Ident [F]”? Phonology 17(3). 427–478. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0952675701003979.Search in Google Scholar
© 2024 the author(s), published by De Gruyter, Berlin/Boston
This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Articles in the same Issue
- Frontmatter
- Research Articles
- Progressive aspect across temporalities: variation between synthetic and analytic forms in L1 and L2 Spanish
- Accounting for effects of monitored speech: vowel nasalization, nasal consonant weakening and /s/ production in Spanish
- Variation, contact, and change in Boston Spanish: how social meaning shapes stylistic practice and bilingual optimization
- First things third? The extension of canonically third-person singular inflections to first-person singular subjects in adult heritage Spanish
- Anti-homophony and rhizotony in the Spanish preterite
- Tenho cantado and venho cantando in Brazilian Portuguese
- Consequences of prosodic variation for spatiotemporal organization in Spanish stop-lateral clusters
- Gender agreement among Russian learners of Spanish in an instructed versus naturalistic learning environment
Articles in the same Issue
- Frontmatter
- Research Articles
- Progressive aspect across temporalities: variation between synthetic and analytic forms in L1 and L2 Spanish
- Accounting for effects of monitored speech: vowel nasalization, nasal consonant weakening and /s/ production in Spanish
- Variation, contact, and change in Boston Spanish: how social meaning shapes stylistic practice and bilingual optimization
- First things third? The extension of canonically third-person singular inflections to first-person singular subjects in adult heritage Spanish
- Anti-homophony and rhizotony in the Spanish preterite
- Tenho cantado and venho cantando in Brazilian Portuguese
- Consequences of prosodic variation for spatiotemporal organization in Spanish stop-lateral clusters
- Gender agreement among Russian learners of Spanish in an instructed versus naturalistic learning environment