Home Wordform-specific frequency effects cause acoustic variation in zero-inflected homophones
Article
Licensed
Unlicensed Requires Authentication

Wordform-specific frequency effects cause acoustic variation in zero-inflected homophones

  • Eva Maria Luef EMAIL logo and Jong-Seung Sun
Published/Copyright: March 1, 2021

Abstract

The frequency with which a word appears in the lexicon has implications for its pronunciation. Numerous studies have shown that high-frequency lemma are characterized by more phonetic reduction than lower-frequency lemma. These findings have proven to be particularly useful in the study of homophones where frequency-related reduction processes can give insights into lexical access theories. The majority of research on homophones and frequency effects has focused on heterographic and semantically unrelated homophones (e.g., English time – thyme) or investigated zero-derived homophones (e.g., English the cut, noun – to cut, verb). Here, zero inflection in German pluralization (e.g., ein Würfel ‘one die’– zwei Würfel ‘two dice’) was investigated to determine if and how frequency effects impact on the acoustic realization of the homophonous singular-plural word pairs. The findings indicate that the number-specified wordforms show acoustic variation related to wordform frequency and the relative frequency of the singular to plural inflected forms. Results differ for durations of wordforms, stem vowels, and final phonemes. Our findings have implications for lexical access theories and can inform about ‘frequency inheritance’ across the singular and plural homophones of the zero-inflected plurals.

5

5 Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the ‘Research Resettlement Fund for New Faculty’ and the ‘Overhead Fund 2017’ of the College of Education of Seoul National University. We would like to thank Hyuksun Kwon, Hannah Kim, Christian Blum, and Magdalena Pelayo-van Buuren for their help during various stages of data collection. We are very grateful to Ingo Plag for critical comments on an earlier version of this paper. We also thank Gerald Luef for help with the figures.

References

Abramson, A.S. and D.H. Whalen. 2017. “Voice onset time (VOT) at 50: Theoretical and practical issues in measuring voicing distinctions”. Journal of Phonetics 63. 75–86.10.1016/j.wocn.2017.05.002Search in Google Scholar

Antilla, R. 1989. Historical and comparative linguistics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/cilt.6Search in Google Scholar

Arvaniti, A. 2012. “Rhythm classes and speech perception”. In: Niebuhr, O. (ed.), Understanding prosody: The role of context, function and communication. Berlin: de Gruyter. 75–92.10.1515/9783110301465.75Search in Google Scholar

Baker, R.E. and A.R. Bradlow. 2009. “Variability in word duration as a function of probability, speech style, and prosody”. Language and Speech 52. 391–413.10.1177/0023830909336575Search in Google Scholar

Bates, D., M. Maechler, B. Bolker and S. Walker. 2014. “{lme4}: Linear mixed– effects models using Eigen and S4”. R Package version1.1-710.18637/jss.v067.i01Search in Google Scholar

Biedermann, B., G. Blanken and L. Nickels. 2002. “The representation of homophones: Evidence from remediation”. Aphasiology 16 (10/11). 1115–1136.10.1080/02687030244000545Search in Google Scholar

Blevins, J. P. 2016. Words and paradigm morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199593545.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Boersma, P. and D. Weenink. 2007. Praat (computer program, version 4.5.25). http://www.praat.org/Search in Google Scholar

Brown, E.K. 2009. “The relative importance of lexical frequency in syllable- and word-final /s/ reduction in Cali, Colombia”. In: Collentine, J. (ed.), Selected proceedings of the 11th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project. 165–178.Search in Google Scholar

Brown, E.L. and R. Torres Cacoullos. 2003. “Spanish /s/: A different story from beginning (initial) to end (final)”. In: Núñez–Cedeño, R., L. López and R. Cameron (eds.), A Romance perspective in language knowledge and use: Selected papers from the 31st linguistic symposium of Romance languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 22–38.Search in Google Scholar

Bybee, J. 2000. “Lexical diffusion in regular sound change”. In: Restle, D. and D. Zaefferer (eds.), Sounds and systems: Studies in structure and change. A Festschrift for Theo Vennemann. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 59–74.Search in Google Scholar

Bybee, J. 2002. “Word frequency and context of use in the lexical diffusion of phonetically conditioned sound change”. Language Variation and Change 14. 261–290.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195301571.003.0011Search in Google Scholar

Byrd, D. 1995. “C-centers revisited”. Phonetica 52. 285–306.10.1159/000262183Search in Google Scholar

Byrd, D., J. Krivokapic and S. Lee. 2006. “How far, how long: On the temporal scope of prosodic boundary effects”. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 120(3). 1589–1599.10.1121/1.2217135Search in Google Scholar

Caramazza, A., A. Costa, M. Miozzo and Y. Bi. 2001. “The specific–word frequency effect: Implication for the representation of homophones in speech production”. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 27. 1430–1450.10.1037/0278-7393.27.6.1430Search in Google Scholar

Cho, T. 2001. “Effects of morpheme boundaries on intergestural timing: Evidence from Korean”. Phonetica 58(3). 129–162.10.1159/000056196Search in Google Scholar

Chomsky, N. and M. Halle. 1968. The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row.Search in Google Scholar

Cohen, C. 2014. “Probabilistic reduction and probabilistic enhancement”. Morphology 24 (4). 291–323.10.1007/s11525-014-9243-ySearch in Google Scholar

Cohen, C. 2015. “Context and paradigms: Two patterns of probabilistic pronunciation variation in Russian agreement suffixes”. the Mental Lexicon 10 (3). 313–338.10.1075/ml.10.3.01cohSearch in Google Scholar

Conwell, E. 2017a. “Are homophones acoustically distinguished in child-directed speech?” Language Learning and Development 13(3). 262–273.10.1080/15475441.2016.1246248Search in Google Scholar

Conwell, E. 2017b. “Prosodic disambiguation of noun/verb homophones in child-directed speech”. Journal of Child Language 44 (3). 734–751.10.1017/S030500091600009XSearch in Google Scholar

Conwell, E. 2017c. “Token frequency effects in homophone production: An elicitation study”. Language and Speech Advance online publication. 10.1177/0023830917737108Search in Google Scholar

Cuetos, F., P. Bonin, J.R. Alameda and A. Caramazza. 2010. “The specific–word frequency effect in speech production: Evidence from Spanish and French”. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 63(4). 750–771.10.1080/17470210903121663Search in Google Scholar

Dellwo, V. 2006. “Rhythm and speech rate: A variation coefficient for ΔC”. In: Karnowski, P.and I. Szigeti (eds.), Language and language-processing. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. 231–241.Search in Google Scholar

Dobson, A. J. 2002. An introduction to Generalized Linear Models. Boca Raton: Chapman & Hall/CRC.10.1201/9781420057683Search in Google Scholar

Drager, K.K. 2011. “Sociophonetic variation and the lemma”. Journal of Phonetics 39 (4). 694–707.10.1016/j.wocn.2011.08.005Search in Google Scholar

Ferreira, V.S. and Z.M. Griffin. 2003. “Phonological influences on lexical (mis)selection”. Psychological Science 14(1). 86–90.10.1111/1467-9280.01424Search in Google Scholar

Field, A. 2005. Discovering statistics using SPSS. London: Sage Publications.10.53841/bpspag.2005.1.56.31Search in Google Scholar

Fillmore, C.J. and B.T. Atkins. 2000. “Describing polysemy: The case of ‘crawl’”. In: Ravin, Y. and C. Leacock (eds.), Polysemy: Theoretical and computational approaches. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 91–110.10.1093/oso/9780198238423.003.0005Search in Google Scholar

Forstmeier, W. and H. Schielzeth. 2011. “Cryptic multiple hypotheses testing in linear models: Overestimated effect sizes and the winner’s curse”. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65. 47–55.10.1007/s00265-010-1038-5Search in Google Scholar

Fougeron, C. 2007. “Word boundaries and contrast neutralization in the case of enchainment in French”. In: Cole, J. and J.I. Hualde (eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology IX: Change in phonology. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 609–642.Search in Google Scholar

Fox, J. and G. Monette. 1992. “Generalized Collinearity Diagnostics”. Journal of the American Statistical Association 87. 178–183.10.1080/01621459.1992.10475190Search in Google Scholar

Gahl, S. 2008. “‘Time’ and ‘thyme’ are not homophones: The effect of lemma frequency on word durations in spontaneous speech”. Language 84. 474–496.10.1353/lan.0.0035Search in Google Scholar

Geyken, A. 2007. “The DWDS corpus: A reference corpus for the German language of the twentieth century”. In: Fellbaum, C. (eds.), Idioms and collocations: Corpus-based linguistic and lexicographic studies. London: Continuum.Search in Google Scholar

Giraudo, H. and M. Voga-Redlinger. 2007. “Lexem-based model vs. morpheme–based model from psycholinguistic perspectives”. In: Montermini, F., G. Boyé and N. Hathout (eds.), Selected proceedings of the 5th Décembrettes: Morphology in Toulouse. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project. 108–114.Search in Google Scholar

Goldhahn, D., T. Eckart and U. Quasthoff. 2012. “Building large monolingual dictionaries at the Leipzig Corpora Collection: From 100 to 200 languages”. Proceedings of the Eighth International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC’12)Search in Google Scholar

Golston, C. and R. Wiese. 1996. “Zero morphology and constraint interaction: Subtraction and epenthesis in German dialects”. In: Booij, G. E. and J. van Marle (eds.), Yearbook of Morphology 1995. Berlin: Springer. 143–159.10.1007/978-94-017-3716-6_8Search in Google Scholar

Gregory, M., W. D. Raymond, A. Bell, E. Fosler-Lussier and D. Jurafsky. 1999. “The effects of collocational strength and contextual predictability in lexical production”. Chicago Linguistic Society 35. 151–166.Search in Google Scholar

Guaïtella, I. 1999. “Rhythm in speech: What rhythmic organizations reveal about cognitive processes in spontaneous speech production versus reading aloud”. Journal of Phonetics 31(4). 509–523.10.1016/S0378-2166(98)00079-4Search in Google Scholar

Guion, S. 1995. “Word frequency effects among homonyms”. Texas Linguistic Forum 35. 103–116.Search in Google Scholar

Hall, C.J. 2014. Morphology and mind. New York: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Harley, T.A. 2013. The psychology of language: From data to theory. London: Taylor & Francis.10.4324/9781315859019Search in Google Scholar

Haspelmath, M. 2002. Understanding morphology. London: Arnold.Search in Google Scholar

Hay, J.B. and R.H. Baayen. 2005. “Shifting paradigms: Gradient structure in morphology”. Trends in Cognitive Science 9(7). 342–348.10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.002Search in Google Scholar

Hayes, B. 1989. “Compensatory lengthening in moraic phonology”. Linguistic Inquiry 20. 253–306.Search in Google Scholar

Hazan, V. and R. Baker. 2010. “Does reading clearly produce the same acoustic– phonetic modifications as spontaneous speech in a clear speaking style?” Proceedings of DiSS–LPSSJoint Workshop. 7–10.Search in Google Scholar

Hunt, R.J. 1986. “Percent agreement, Pearson’s correlation, and Kappa as measures of inter–examiner reliability”. Journal of Dental Research 65. 128–130.10.1177/00220345860650020701Search in Google Scholar

Jescheniak, J. D. and W. J. M. Levelt. 1994. “Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form”. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20 (4). 824–843.10.1037/0278-7393.20.4.824Search in Google Scholar

Jurafsky, D., A. Bell, M. Gregory and W.D. Raymond. 2001. “Probabilistic relations between words: Evidence from reduction in lexical production”. In: Bybee, J. and P. Hooper (eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 229–254.10.1075/tsl.45.13jurSearch in Google Scholar

Katz, J. 2012. “Compression effects in English”. Journal of Phonetics 40(3). 390–402.10.1016/j.wocn.2012.02.004Search in Google Scholar

Kavitskaya, D. 2002. Compensatory lengthening: Phonetics, phonology, diachrony. New York: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Kim, S.K. and M. Sumner. 2013. “Effects of emotional prosody on word recognition”. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 134(5). 4232.10.1121/1.4831551Search in Google Scholar

Kuperman, V.M., M. Pluymaekers, M. Ernestur and R.H. Baayen. 2006. “Morphological predictability and acoustic salience of interfixes in Dutch compounds”. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 122. 2018–2024.Search in Google Scholar

Lehiste, I. 1972a. Suprasegmentals. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Lehiste, I. 1972b. “The timing of utterances and linguistic boundaries”. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 51(6B). 2018–2024.10.1121/1.1913062Search in Google Scholar

Lehiste, I. 1977. “Isochrony reconsidered”. Journal of Phonetics 5. 253–263.10.1016/S0095-4470(19)31139-8Search in Google Scholar

Levelt, W.J.M., A. Roelofs and A.S. Meyer. 1999. “A theory of lexical access in speech production”. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22. 1–38.10.3115/992628.992631Search in Google Scholar

Levelt, W.J.M. and L.R. Wheeldon. 1994. “Do speakers have access to a mental syllabary?” Cognition 50. 239–269.10.1016/0010-0277(94)90030-2Search in Google Scholar

Lieberman, P. 1960. “Some acoustic correlates of word stress in American English”. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 32. 451.10.1121/1.1908095Search in Google Scholar

Lindblom, B. 1990. “Explaining phonetic variation: A sketch of the H&H theory”. In: Hardcastle, W.J. and A. Marchal (eds.), Speech production and speech modelling. Dordrecht: Springer. 403–439.10.1007/978-94-009-2037-8_16Search in Google Scholar

Lohmann, A. 2017. “Cut(N) and cut(V) are not homophones: Lemma frequency affects the duration of noun–verb conversion pairs”. Journal of Linguistics. 1–15.10.1017/S0022226717000378Search in Google Scholar

Lutz, A. 1988. “On the historical phonotactics of English”. In: Kastovsky, D. and B. Bauer (eds.), Luick revisited. Tübingen: Gunter Narr Verlag. 221–240.Search in Google Scholar

Marin, S. and M. Pouplier. 2014. “Articulatory synergies in the temporal organization of liquid clusters in Romanian”. Journal of Phonetics 42. 24–36.10.1016/j.wocn.2013.11.001Search in Google Scholar

Munson, B. 2007. “Lexical access, lexical representation, and vowel production”. In: Cole, J. S. and J.I. Hualde (eds.), Laboratory phonology 9. Berlin: Mouton. 201–228.Search in Google Scholar

Neef, M. 1988. “The reduced syllable plural in German”. In: Fabri, R., A. Ortmann and T. Parodi (eds.), Models of inflection. Tuebingen: Max Niemeyer Verlag. 244–265.Search in Google Scholar

Norris, D. 1994. “Shortlist: A connectionist model of continuous speech recognition”. Cognition 52. 189–234.10.1016/0010-0277(94)90043-4Search in Google Scholar

O’Shaughnessy, D. 1974. “Consonant durations in clusters”. IEEE Transactions on Acoustics Speech and Signal Processing 22. 282–295.10.1109/TASSP.1974.1162588Search in Google Scholar

Pouplier, M. 2012. “The gestural approach to syllable structure: Universal, language-and cluster-specific aspects”. In: Fuchs, S., M. Weirich, D. Pape and P. Perrier (eds.), Speech planning and dynamics. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. 63–96.Search in Google Scholar

Quinn, G.P. and M.J. Keough. 2002. Experimental designs and data analysis for biologists. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511806384Search in Google Scholar

R Team, Development Core. 2017. R: A language and environment for statistical computingR Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria.Search in Google Scholar

Ramus, F., M. Nespor and J. Mehler. 1999. “Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal”. Cognition 73 (3). 265–292.10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00058-XSearch in Google Scholar

Roelofs, A. 1997. “The WEAVER model of word-form encoding in speech production”. Cognition 64 (3). 249–284.10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00027-9Search in Google Scholar

Samlowski, B., P. Wagner and B. Möbius. 2013. “Effects of lexical class and lemma frequency on German homographs”. Interspeech 2013, Lyon.10.21437/Interspeech.2013-163Search in Google Scholar

Scalero, A.C. 2017. Homophony and conversion (zero derivation): The durational differences of seemingly phonetically identical lexeme pairs. (MA thesis, San Diego State University.)Search in Google Scholar

Seyfarth, S., E. Buz and F.T. Jaeger. 2018. “Dynamic hypearticulation of coda voicing contrasts”. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 139(2). EL31–37.10.1121/1.4942544Search in Google Scholar

Shaiman, S. 2001. “Kinematics of compensatory vowel shortening: The effect of speaking rate and code composition on intra- and inter-articulatory timing”. Journal of Phonetics 29. 89–107.10.1006/jpho.2001.0133Search in Google Scholar

Smith, R. and S. Hawkins. 2012. “Production and perception of speaker–specific phonetic detail at word boundaries”. Journal of Phonetics 40(2). 213–233.10.1016/j.wocn.2011.11.003Search in Google Scholar

Stemberger, J.P. 1991. “Apparent anti–frequency effects in language production: The addition bias and phonological underspecification”. Journal of Memory and Language 30(2). 161–185.10.1016/0749-596X(91)90002-2Search in Google Scholar

Tomaschek, F., D. Arnold, F. Bröker and R.H. Baayen. 2018. “Lexical frequency co– determines the speech–curvature relation in articulation”. Journal of Phonetics 68. 103–116.10.1016/j.wocn.2018.02.003Search in Google Scholar

Tomaschek, F., B.V. Tucker, M. Wieling and R.H. Baayen. 2014. “Vowel articulation affected by word frequency”. 10th ISSP, Cologne, Germany, May 5–8.Search in Google Scholar

Tomaschek, F., M. Wieling, D. Arnold and R.H. Baayen. 2013. “Word frequency, vowel length and vowel quality in speech production: An EMA study of the importance of experience”. Interspeech 2013, Lyon, France.10.21437/Interspeech.2013-347Search in Google Scholar

Watson, D.G., A. Buxó-Lugo and D.C. Simmons. 2015. “The effect of phonological encoding on word duration: Selection takes time”. In: E. Gibson and L. Frazier (eds.), Explicit and implicit prosody in sentence processing. Switzerland: Springer International Publishing. 85–98.10.1007/978-3-319-12961-7_5Search in Google Scholar

Wedel, A.B., A. Kaplan and S. Jackson. 2013. “High functional load inhibits phonological contrast loss: A corpus study”. Cognition 128. 179–186.10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.002Search in Google Scholar

Wedel, A.B., N. Nelson and R. Sharp. 2018. “The phonetic specificity of contrastive hyperarticulation in natural speech”. Journal of Memory and Language 100. 61–88.10.1016/j.jml.2018.01.001Search in Google Scholar

Whalen, D.H. 1991. “Infrequent words are longer in duration than frequent words”. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 90 (4). 2311.10.1121/1.401072Search in Google Scholar

Wheeldon, L.R. and S. Monsell. 1992. “The locus of repetition priming of spoken word recognition”. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 44. 723–761.10.1080/14640749208401307Search in Google Scholar

White, L. and S.L. Mattys. 2007. “Calibrating rhythm: First language and second language studies”. Journal of Phonetics 35. 501–522.10.1016/j.wocn.2007.02.003Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2021-03-01
Published in Print: 2020-12-16

© 2020 Faculty of English, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań, Poland

Downloaded on 10.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/psicl-2020-0024/html
Scroll to top button