Abstract
The frequency with which a word appears in the lexicon has implications for its pronunciation. Numerous studies have shown that high-frequency lemma are characterized by more phonetic reduction than lower-frequency lemma. These findings have proven to be particularly useful in the study of homophones where frequency-related reduction processes can give insights into lexical access theories. The majority of research on homophones and frequency effects has focused on heterographic and semantically unrelated homophones (e.g., English time – thyme) or investigated zero-derived homophones (e.g., English the cut, noun – to cut, verb). Here, zero inflection in German pluralization (e.g., ein Würfel ‘one die’– zwei Würfel ‘two dice’) was investigated to determine if and how frequency effects impact on the acoustic realization of the homophonous singular-plural word pairs. The findings indicate that the number-specified wordforms show acoustic variation related to wordform frequency and the relative frequency of the singular to plural inflected forms. Results differ for durations of wordforms, stem vowels, and final phonemes. Our findings have implications for lexical access theories and can inform about ‘frequency inheritance’ across the singular and plural homophones of the zero-inflected plurals.
5 Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the ‘Research Resettlement Fund for New Faculty’ and the ‘Overhead Fund 2017’ of the College of Education of Seoul National University. We would like to thank Hyuksun Kwon, Hannah Kim, Christian Blum, and Magdalena Pelayo-van Buuren for their help during various stages of data collection. We are very grateful to Ingo Plag for critical comments on an earlier version of this paper. We also thank Gerald Luef for help with the figures.
References
Abramson, A.S. and D.H. Whalen. 2017. “Voice onset time (VOT) at 50: Theoretical and practical issues in measuring voicing distinctions”. Journal of Phonetics 63. 75–86.10.1016/j.wocn.2017.05.002Search in Google Scholar
Antilla, R. 1989. Historical and comparative linguistics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/cilt.6Search in Google Scholar
Arvaniti, A. 2012. “Rhythm classes and speech perception”. In: Niebuhr, O. (ed.), Understanding prosody: The role of context, function and communication. Berlin: de Gruyter. 75–92.10.1515/9783110301465.75Search in Google Scholar
Baker, R.E. and A.R. Bradlow. 2009. “Variability in word duration as a function of probability, speech style, and prosody”. Language and Speech 52. 391–413.10.1177/0023830909336575Search in Google Scholar
Bates, D., M. Maechler, B. Bolker and S. Walker. 2014. “{lme4}: Linear mixed– effects models using Eigen and S4”. R Package version1.1-710.18637/jss.v067.i01Search in Google Scholar
Biedermann, B., G. Blanken and L. Nickels. 2002. “The representation of homophones: Evidence from remediation”. Aphasiology 16 (10/11). 1115–1136.10.1080/02687030244000545Search in Google Scholar
Blevins, J. P. 2016. Words and paradigm morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199593545.001.0001Search in Google Scholar
Boersma, P. and D. Weenink. 2007. Praat (computer program, version 4.5.25). http://www.praat.org/Search in Google Scholar
Brown, E.K. 2009. “The relative importance of lexical frequency in syllable- and word-final /s/ reduction in Cali, Colombia”. In: Collentine, J. (ed.), Selected proceedings of the 11th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project. 165–178.Search in Google Scholar
Brown, E.L. and R. Torres Cacoullos. 2003. “Spanish /s/: A different story from beginning (initial) to end (final)”. In: Núñez–Cedeño, R., L. López and R. Cameron (eds.), A Romance perspective in language knowledge and use: Selected papers from the 31st linguistic symposium of Romance languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 22–38.Search in Google Scholar
Bybee, J. 2000. “Lexical diffusion in regular sound change”. In: Restle, D. and D. Zaefferer (eds.), Sounds and systems: Studies in structure and change. A Festschrift for Theo Vennemann. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 59–74.Search in Google Scholar
Bybee, J. 2002. “Word frequency and context of use in the lexical diffusion of phonetically conditioned sound change”. Language Variation and Change 14. 261–290.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195301571.003.0011Search in Google Scholar
Byrd, D. 1995. “C-centers revisited”. Phonetica 52. 285–306.10.1159/000262183Search in Google Scholar
Byrd, D., J. Krivokapic and S. Lee. 2006. “How far, how long: On the temporal scope of prosodic boundary effects”. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 120(3). 1589–1599.10.1121/1.2217135Search in Google Scholar
Caramazza, A., A. Costa, M. Miozzo and Y. Bi. 2001. “The specific–word frequency effect: Implication for the representation of homophones in speech production”. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 27. 1430–1450.10.1037/0278-7393.27.6.1430Search in Google Scholar
Cho, T. 2001. “Effects of morpheme boundaries on intergestural timing: Evidence from Korean”. Phonetica 58(3). 129–162.10.1159/000056196Search in Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. and M. Halle. 1968. The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row.Search in Google Scholar
Cohen, C. 2014. “Probabilistic reduction and probabilistic enhancement”. Morphology 24 (4). 291–323.10.1007/s11525-014-9243-ySearch in Google Scholar
Cohen, C. 2015. “Context and paradigms: Two patterns of probabilistic pronunciation variation in Russian agreement suffixes”. the Mental Lexicon 10 (3). 313–338.10.1075/ml.10.3.01cohSearch in Google Scholar
Conwell, E. 2017a. “Are homophones acoustically distinguished in child-directed speech?” Language Learning and Development 13(3). 262–273.10.1080/15475441.2016.1246248Search in Google Scholar
Conwell, E. 2017b. “Prosodic disambiguation of noun/verb homophones in child-directed speech”. Journal of Child Language 44 (3). 734–751.10.1017/S030500091600009XSearch in Google Scholar
Conwell, E. 2017c. “Token frequency effects in homophone production: An elicitation study”. Language and Speech Advance online publication. 10.1177/0023830917737108Search in Google Scholar
Cuetos, F., P. Bonin, J.R. Alameda and A. Caramazza. 2010. “The specific–word frequency effect in speech production: Evidence from Spanish and French”. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 63(4). 750–771.10.1080/17470210903121663Search in Google Scholar
Dellwo, V. 2006. “Rhythm and speech rate: A variation coefficient for ΔC”. In: Karnowski, P.and I. Szigeti (eds.), Language and language-processing. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. 231–241.Search in Google Scholar
Dobson, A. J. 2002. An introduction to Generalized Linear Models. Boca Raton: Chapman & Hall/CRC.10.1201/9781420057683Search in Google Scholar
Drager, K.K. 2011. “Sociophonetic variation and the lemma”. Journal of Phonetics 39 (4). 694–707.10.1016/j.wocn.2011.08.005Search in Google Scholar
Ferreira, V.S. and Z.M. Griffin. 2003. “Phonological influences on lexical (mis)selection”. Psychological Science 14(1). 86–90.10.1111/1467-9280.01424Search in Google Scholar
Field, A. 2005. Discovering statistics using SPSS. London: Sage Publications.10.53841/bpspag.2005.1.56.31Search in Google Scholar
Fillmore, C.J. and B.T. Atkins. 2000. “Describing polysemy: The case of ‘crawl’”. In: Ravin, Y. and C. Leacock (eds.), Polysemy: Theoretical and computational approaches. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 91–110.10.1093/oso/9780198238423.003.0005Search in Google Scholar
Forstmeier, W. and H. Schielzeth. 2011. “Cryptic multiple hypotheses testing in linear models: Overestimated effect sizes and the winner’s curse”. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65. 47–55.10.1007/s00265-010-1038-5Search in Google Scholar
Fougeron, C. 2007. “Word boundaries and contrast neutralization in the case of enchainment in French”. In: Cole, J. and J.I. Hualde (eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology IX: Change in phonology. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 609–642.Search in Google Scholar
Fox, J. and G. Monette. 1992. “Generalized Collinearity Diagnostics”. Journal of the American Statistical Association 87. 178–183.10.1080/01621459.1992.10475190Search in Google Scholar
Gahl, S. 2008. “‘Time’ and ‘thyme’ are not homophones: The effect of lemma frequency on word durations in spontaneous speech”. Language 84. 474–496.10.1353/lan.0.0035Search in Google Scholar
Geyken, A. 2007. “The DWDS corpus: A reference corpus for the German language of the twentieth century”. In: Fellbaum, C. (eds.), Idioms and collocations: Corpus-based linguistic and lexicographic studies. London: Continuum.Search in Google Scholar
Giraudo, H. and M. Voga-Redlinger. 2007. “Lexem-based model vs. morpheme–based model from psycholinguistic perspectives”. In: Montermini, F., G. Boyé and N. Hathout (eds.), Selected proceedings of the 5th Décembrettes: Morphology in Toulouse. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project. 108–114.Search in Google Scholar
Goldhahn, D., T. Eckart and U. Quasthoff. 2012. “Building large monolingual dictionaries at the Leipzig Corpora Collection: From 100 to 200 languages”. Proceedings of the Eighth International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC’12)Search in Google Scholar
Golston, C. and R. Wiese. 1996. “Zero morphology and constraint interaction: Subtraction and epenthesis in German dialects”. In: Booij, G. E. and J. van Marle (eds.), Yearbook of Morphology 1995. Berlin: Springer. 143–159.10.1007/978-94-017-3716-6_8Search in Google Scholar
Gregory, M., W. D. Raymond, A. Bell, E. Fosler-Lussier and D. Jurafsky. 1999. “The effects of collocational strength and contextual predictability in lexical production”. Chicago Linguistic Society 35. 151–166.Search in Google Scholar
Guaïtella, I. 1999. “Rhythm in speech: What rhythmic organizations reveal about cognitive processes in spontaneous speech production versus reading aloud”. Journal of Phonetics 31(4). 509–523.10.1016/S0378-2166(98)00079-4Search in Google Scholar
Guion, S. 1995. “Word frequency effects among homonyms”. Texas Linguistic Forum 35. 103–116.Search in Google Scholar
Hall, C.J. 2014. Morphology and mind. New York: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar
Harley, T.A. 2013. The psychology of language: From data to theory. London: Taylor & Francis.10.4324/9781315859019Search in Google Scholar
Haspelmath, M. 2002. Understanding morphology. London: Arnold.Search in Google Scholar
Hay, J.B. and R.H. Baayen. 2005. “Shifting paradigms: Gradient structure in morphology”. Trends in Cognitive Science 9(7). 342–348.10.1016/j.tics.2005.04.002Search in Google Scholar
Hayes, B. 1989. “Compensatory lengthening in moraic phonology”. Linguistic Inquiry 20. 253–306.Search in Google Scholar
Hazan, V. and R. Baker. 2010. “Does reading clearly produce the same acoustic– phonetic modifications as spontaneous speech in a clear speaking style?” Proceedings of DiSS–LPSSJoint Workshop. 7–10.Search in Google Scholar
Hunt, R.J. 1986. “Percent agreement, Pearson’s correlation, and Kappa as measures of inter–examiner reliability”. Journal of Dental Research 65. 128–130.10.1177/00220345860650020701Search in Google Scholar
Jescheniak, J. D. and W. J. M. Levelt. 1994. “Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form”. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20 (4). 824–843.10.1037/0278-7393.20.4.824Search in Google Scholar
Jurafsky, D., A. Bell, M. Gregory and W.D. Raymond. 2001. “Probabilistic relations between words: Evidence from reduction in lexical production”. In: Bybee, J. and P. Hooper (eds.), Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 229–254.10.1075/tsl.45.13jurSearch in Google Scholar
Katz, J. 2012. “Compression effects in English”. Journal of Phonetics 40(3). 390–402.10.1016/j.wocn.2012.02.004Search in Google Scholar
Kavitskaya, D. 2002. Compensatory lengthening: Phonetics, phonology, diachrony. New York: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar
Kim, S.K. and M. Sumner. 2013. “Effects of emotional prosody on word recognition”. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 134(5). 4232.10.1121/1.4831551Search in Google Scholar
Kuperman, V.M., M. Pluymaekers, M. Ernestur and R.H. Baayen. 2006. “Morphological predictability and acoustic salience of interfixes in Dutch compounds”. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 122. 2018–2024.Search in Google Scholar
Lehiste, I. 1972a. Suprasegmentals. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar
Lehiste, I. 1972b. “The timing of utterances and linguistic boundaries”. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 51(6B). 2018–2024.10.1121/1.1913062Search in Google Scholar
Lehiste, I. 1977. “Isochrony reconsidered”. Journal of Phonetics 5. 253–263.10.1016/S0095-4470(19)31139-8Search in Google Scholar
Levelt, W.J.M., A. Roelofs and A.S. Meyer. 1999. “A theory of lexical access in speech production”. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22. 1–38.10.3115/992628.992631Search in Google Scholar
Levelt, W.J.M. and L.R. Wheeldon. 1994. “Do speakers have access to a mental syllabary?” Cognition 50. 239–269.10.1016/0010-0277(94)90030-2Search in Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. 1960. “Some acoustic correlates of word stress in American English”. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 32. 451.10.1121/1.1908095Search in Google Scholar
Lindblom, B. 1990. “Explaining phonetic variation: A sketch of the H&H theory”. In: Hardcastle, W.J. and A. Marchal (eds.), Speech production and speech modelling. Dordrecht: Springer. 403–439.10.1007/978-94-009-2037-8_16Search in Google Scholar
Lohmann, A. 2017. “Cut(N) and cut(V) are not homophones: Lemma frequency affects the duration of noun–verb conversion pairs”. Journal of Linguistics. 1–15.10.1017/S0022226717000378Search in Google Scholar
Lutz, A. 1988. “On the historical phonotactics of English”. In: Kastovsky, D. and B. Bauer (eds.), Luick revisited. Tübingen: Gunter Narr Verlag. 221–240.Search in Google Scholar
Marin, S. and M. Pouplier. 2014. “Articulatory synergies in the temporal organization of liquid clusters in Romanian”. Journal of Phonetics 42. 24–36.10.1016/j.wocn.2013.11.001Search in Google Scholar
Munson, B. 2007. “Lexical access, lexical representation, and vowel production”. In: Cole, J. S. and J.I. Hualde (eds.), Laboratory phonology 9. Berlin: Mouton. 201–228.Search in Google Scholar
Neef, M. 1988. “The reduced syllable plural in German”. In: Fabri, R., A. Ortmann and T. Parodi (eds.), Models of inflection. Tuebingen: Max Niemeyer Verlag. 244–265.Search in Google Scholar
Norris, D. 1994. “Shortlist: A connectionist model of continuous speech recognition”. Cognition 52. 189–234.10.1016/0010-0277(94)90043-4Search in Google Scholar
O’Shaughnessy, D. 1974. “Consonant durations in clusters”. IEEE Transactions on Acoustics Speech and Signal Processing 22. 282–295.10.1109/TASSP.1974.1162588Search in Google Scholar
Pouplier, M. 2012. “The gestural approach to syllable structure: Universal, language-and cluster-specific aspects”. In: Fuchs, S., M. Weirich, D. Pape and P. Perrier (eds.), Speech planning and dynamics. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. 63–96.Search in Google Scholar
Quinn, G.P. and M.J. Keough. 2002. Experimental designs and data analysis for biologists. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511806384Search in Google Scholar
R Team, Development Core. 2017. R: A language and environment for statistical computingR Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria.Search in Google Scholar
Ramus, F., M. Nespor and J. Mehler. 1999. “Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal”. Cognition 73 (3). 265–292.10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00058-XSearch in Google Scholar
Roelofs, A. 1997. “The WEAVER model of word-form encoding in speech production”. Cognition 64 (3). 249–284.10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00027-9Search in Google Scholar
Samlowski, B., P. Wagner and B. Möbius. 2013. “Effects of lexical class and lemma frequency on German homographs”. Interspeech 2013, Lyon.10.21437/Interspeech.2013-163Search in Google Scholar
Scalero, A.C. 2017. Homophony and conversion (zero derivation): The durational differences of seemingly phonetically identical lexeme pairs. (MA thesis, San Diego State University.)Search in Google Scholar
Seyfarth, S., E. Buz and F.T. Jaeger. 2018. “Dynamic hypearticulation of coda voicing contrasts”. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 139(2). EL31–37.10.1121/1.4942544Search in Google Scholar
Shaiman, S. 2001. “Kinematics of compensatory vowel shortening: The effect of speaking rate and code composition on intra- and inter-articulatory timing”. Journal of Phonetics 29. 89–107.10.1006/jpho.2001.0133Search in Google Scholar
Smith, R. and S. Hawkins. 2012. “Production and perception of speaker–specific phonetic detail at word boundaries”. Journal of Phonetics 40(2). 213–233.10.1016/j.wocn.2011.11.003Search in Google Scholar
Stemberger, J.P. 1991. “Apparent anti–frequency effects in language production: The addition bias and phonological underspecification”. Journal of Memory and Language 30(2). 161–185.10.1016/0749-596X(91)90002-2Search in Google Scholar
Tomaschek, F., D. Arnold, F. Bröker and R.H. Baayen. 2018. “Lexical frequency co– determines the speech–curvature relation in articulation”. Journal of Phonetics 68. 103–116.10.1016/j.wocn.2018.02.003Search in Google Scholar
Tomaschek, F., B.V. Tucker, M. Wieling and R.H. Baayen. 2014. “Vowel articulation affected by word frequency”. 10th ISSP, Cologne, Germany, May 5–8.Search in Google Scholar
Tomaschek, F., M. Wieling, D. Arnold and R.H. Baayen. 2013. “Word frequency, vowel length and vowel quality in speech production: An EMA study of the importance of experience”. Interspeech 2013, Lyon, France.10.21437/Interspeech.2013-347Search in Google Scholar
Watson, D.G., A. Buxó-Lugo and D.C. Simmons. 2015. “The effect of phonological encoding on word duration: Selection takes time”. In: E. Gibson and L. Frazier (eds.), Explicit and implicit prosody in sentence processing. Switzerland: Springer International Publishing. 85–98.10.1007/978-3-319-12961-7_5Search in Google Scholar
Wedel, A.B., A. Kaplan and S. Jackson. 2013. “High functional load inhibits phonological contrast loss: A corpus study”. Cognition 128. 179–186.10.1016/j.cognition.2013.03.002Search in Google Scholar
Wedel, A.B., N. Nelson and R. Sharp. 2018. “The phonetic specificity of contrastive hyperarticulation in natural speech”. Journal of Memory and Language 100. 61–88.10.1016/j.jml.2018.01.001Search in Google Scholar
Whalen, D.H. 1991. “Infrequent words are longer in duration than frequent words”. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 90 (4). 2311.10.1121/1.401072Search in Google Scholar
Wheeldon, L.R. and S. Monsell. 1992. “The locus of repetition priming of spoken word recognition”. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 44. 723–761.10.1080/14640749208401307Search in Google Scholar
White, L. and S.L. Mattys. 2007. “Calibrating rhythm: First language and second language studies”. Journal of Phonetics 35. 501–522.10.1016/j.wocn.2007.02.003Search in Google Scholar
© 2020 Faculty of English, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań, Poland
Articles in the same Issue
- Frontmatter
- Isoglosses and language change: Evidence of the rise and loss of isoglosses from a comparison of early Greek and early English
- The interaction of L2 and L3 levels of proficiency in third language acquisition
- Self-reported communicative distance between Polish and English in formal and informal situational contexts
- Mining historical texts for diachronic spelling variants
- Nigerian newscasters’ English as a model of standard Nigerian English?
- A cognitive semantic exploration of English plant phrasal verbs with the particle out and their Serbian counterparts
- Wordform-specific frequency effects cause acoustic variation in zero-inflected homophones
- Erratum
- Erratum
Articles in the same Issue
- Frontmatter
- Isoglosses and language change: Evidence of the rise and loss of isoglosses from a comparison of early Greek and early English
- The interaction of L2 and L3 levels of proficiency in third language acquisition
- Self-reported communicative distance between Polish and English in formal and informal situational contexts
- Mining historical texts for diachronic spelling variants
- Nigerian newscasters’ English as a model of standard Nigerian English?
- A cognitive semantic exploration of English plant phrasal verbs with the particle out and their Serbian counterparts
- Wordform-specific frequency effects cause acoustic variation in zero-inflected homophones
- Erratum
- Erratum