Startseite The negative concord illusion: an acceptability study with Czech neg-words
Artikel Open Access

The negative concord illusion: an acceptability study with Czech neg-words

  • Radim Lacina ORCID logo EMAIL logo
Veröffentlicht/Copyright: 19. Februar 2025
Linguistics Vanguard
Aus der Zeitschrift Linguistics Vanguard

Abstract

Languages such as Czech exhibit negative concord, a requirement for any neg-word (e.g., nobody) in a clause to be accompanied by sentential negation. Theoretical treatments see this phenomenon as either a case of syntactic agreement or subsume it under negative polarity item (NPI) licensing. Interestingly, in the processing of both agreement and NPIs, the inclusion of a distractor element can induce comprehenders to accept otherwise ungrammatical sentences. We tested whether such grammaticality illusions also arise with negative concord. We ran an acceptability judgement study with native speakers of Czech to see whether the inclusion of an irrelevant negated verb in a relative clause caused them to accept an ungrammatical sentence with an unlicensed neg-word. This tendency was clearly seen in the speeded acceptability judgement task (Experiment 1), but not when participants had enough time for their answers (Experiment 2), a pattern typical for linguistic illusions. Our results thus show that there is indeed an effect – the negative concord illusion.

1 Introduction

In many languages, the type of phrase exemplified by the English no student requires the use of negation on the main verb of the local clause. Take the two following contrasting examples, from Czech, a language exhibiting this behaviour, and Standard English, where this construction leads to a different interpretation altogether:

(1)
Žádný student ne-dostal ze zkoušky za A.
no student neg-got from exam for A
‘No student got an A on the exam.’
(2)
No student didn’t get an A on the exam.

In Czech, the interpretation of (1) is that there was not a single student who got an A. On the other hand, the same use of both the negative quantifier no and sentential negation in Standard English leads to the opposite reading, namely that it was not the case that no student received an A, meaning that there were at least some students who did (de Swart 2009; but for conflicting evidence, see Blanchette et al. 2018). Languages that behave in the way Czech does have become known as “negative concord languages”.

Cases of negative concord appear with a class of words known as “neg-words” (or “n-words” as per Laka 1990), such as the Czech žádný ‘no’ in (1). Giannakidou (2006) defines them as expressions that can be used in a structure with a sentential negation or another neg-word while yielding only one logical negation. There have been many theoretical attempts at analysing negative concord (for an overview, see Giannakidou and Zeijlstra 2017). One class of approaches sees the phenomenon as essentially a case of agreement (Haegeman and Zanuttini 1991; Zanuttini 1991; Zeijlstra 2004, 2008]). The proposal by Zeijlstra suggests that neg-words carry an uninterpretable negative feature that requires checking against an interpretable negative feature that is found on the verb. Under this view, negative concord is not dissimilar from other cases of agreement such as subject-verb agreement. Crucially for our purposes, this approach differentiates neg-words from negative polarity items (NPIs) and has them licensed in the syntax.

Another class of approaches sees neg-words as being subsumed under NPIs (Giannakidou and Quer 1995, 1997]; Ladusaw 1992). Under this view, neg-words in negative concord languages are similar to NPIs such as any. Some proposals see neg-words as universal quantifiers (Giannakidou 2000) that scope above negation; others see them as indefinites (Ladusaw 1992). This approach sees neg-words as in themselves non-negative (for accounts that treat neg-words as negative, see De Swart 2009; De Swart and Sag 2002). They are indefinites similar to phrases with NPIs such as any student, yet they come with a roofing requirement in Ladusaw’s (1992) parlance. What these accounts have in common is that they relate the conditions of the grammaticality of neg-words to the licensing conditions of NPIs.

A proper treatment of the theoretical landscape of negative concord is beyond the scope of this article. What is, however, crucial for the following investigations into the processing of the phenomenon is the understanding that negative concord is treated either as syntactic agreement or as NPI licensing. We return to the theoretical issues in the discussion and suggest how examining the processing of negative concord can inform theory.

While the theoretical literature disagrees on what the correct analysis of negative concord is, psycholinguistic investigations have revealed that both classic NPIs and syntactic agreement are susceptible to interference from grammatically (or semantically) irrelevant elements in online processing. One of the ways this is manifested is in grammaticality illusions. This is a phenomenon whereby under certain conditions, comprehenders mistakenly judge certain sentences as acceptable even though they are in fact ungrammatical (Phillips et al. 2011). Below, we review the literature on how these illusions arise in the general cases of agreement and NPI licensing. We then build a case for why the same effect ought to be seen with negative concord as well.

Let us begin by discussing agreement and its illusion phenomenon – agreement attraction:

(3)
*The key to the cabinets were rusty from many years of disuse.
(4)
*The key to the cabinet were rusty from many years of disuse.

While both (3) and (4) are ungrammatical due to the presence of the plural marking on the verb were, which mismatches the singular number of the subject head the key, comprehenders judge (3) in a speeded acceptability task as grammatical more often than (4) due to the former containing a noun (cabinets), which, while not the actual subject, is plural (Wagers et al. 2009).[1]

These illusions have been well replicated in number agreement both in English (Hammerly et al. 2019; Patson and Husband 2016; Royer 2021) and in other languages, such as Greek (Paspali and Marinis 2020) and Spanish (Gonzalez Alonso et al. 2021). In Czech, Lacina and Dotlačil (2024) showed the presence of grammaticality illusions in number agreement, which were reliable yet of a small effect size. Additionally, there is a body of evidence showing that in reading tasks, the presence of a feature-matching attractor can cause relative speed-ups in the processing of sentences with ungrammatical number agreement (Franck et al. 2015; Jäger et al. 2020; Lago et al. 2015, 2019]; Parker and An 2018; Slioussar 2018; Tucker et al. 2015; Wagers et al. 2009). In general, agreement attraction manifested in comprehension either as illusions or in reading times has been shown to be present in many phenomena, including gender agreement (Gonzalez Alonso et al. 2021; Paspali and Marinis 2020; Slioussar and Malko 2016; Tucker et al. 2021) and honorifics (Kwon and Sturt 2016). The influences of various factors on the effect, for example that of case marking, have also been investigated (Avetisyan et al. 2020).

Let us now discuss the other processing phenomenon of interest here – npi illusions. Grammatically, NPIs are licensed in the scope of a downward-entailing operator (Ladusaw 1992), which is not the case in the following sentence:

(5)
*The student, who no teacher praises, has ever got an A.

It has been found that when exposed to sentences such as (5), comprehenders come under impression that the NPI ever is licensed even though it is not and that this effect is caused by the presence of the downward-entailing negative quantifier no in the relative clause (Drenhaus et al. 2005; Parker and Phillips 2016; Vasishth et al. 2008), which ought to be irrelevant. Just as with agreement attraction, the effect has been replicated repeatedly in both English and other languages (Muller 2022; Orth et al. 2021; Schwab 2022; Xiang et al. 2009; Yanilmaz and Drury 2018; Yun et al. 2018).

One recent crucial finding in the literature has been that only some licensors and only some NPIs can support illusions. Orth et al. (2021) find that while simple sentential negation grammatically licenses NPIs, its presence in a relative clause as an illusory licensor does not cause the effect in English. In Turkish, on the other hand, sentential negation did lead to illusions (Yanilmaz and Drury 2018). Schwab (2022) finds that in German, only strengthening NPIs such as jemals ‘ever’ can support illusions, while attenuating ones such as so recht ‘really’ do not. These results suggest a specificity to the effect, also dependent on the language in question.

Both classic NPIs and morphosyntactic agreement give rise to illusions and have even been proposed to be explainable by the same theories – cue-based retrieval (see Engelmann et al. 2019; Lewis and Vasishth 2005; Vasishth et al. 2008) and feature percolation accounts (see Eberhard et al. 2005; Orth et al. 2021). Cue-based retrieval theories explain attraction and NPI illusions by stipulating that the effects arise because comprehenders store incoming elements as bundles of features with dependency being established by means of memory retrieval, which depends on featural cues. In ungrammatical sentences where the attractor or illusory licensor matches one of the cues needed, processing speed-ups are predicted (Logačev and Vasishth 2016). Feature percolation accounts, on the other hand, suggest that features such as number, gender, or polarity in the case of NPIs percolate up the syntactic tree causing the illusion (Eberhard et al. 2005; Orth et al. 2021; Patson and Husband 2016). There are, however, many other competing separate theories of agreement attraction (see Dempsey et al. 2022; Hammerly et al. 2019; Häussler 2009; Keshev et al. 2024; Smith et al. 2018) and NPI illusions (see Giannakidou and Etxeberria 2018; Mendia et al. 2018; Schwab 2022; Xiang et al. 2013) that do not necessarily subsume the two phenomena under a single explanatory umbrella.

All of this points us to speculate whether the processing of negative concord is also susceptible to grammaticality illusions. This is an empirical question that has so far not been addressed to our knowledge. In the current study, we do just that by conducting experiments with Czech neg-words and we show that there is indeed a grammaticality illusion.

2 Experiments 1 and 2

We ran two web-based acceptability judgement experiments with native speakers of Czech, a strict negative concord language. Experiment 1 used the speeded acceptability judgement task, while Experiment 2 tested the same sentences without a time limit for participants’ answers. In both experiments, our participants saw sentences with an unlicensed neg-word and an intervening relative clause that included a case of a combination of a licensed neg-word and sentential negation. We predicted illusion effects to arise in the speeded task (Experiment 1) and to be absent when enough time is given to participants (Experiment 2).

Let us now set out precisely what we hypothesize in the current study with regard to the acceptability of Czech sentences with negative concord within both the speeded task and the untimed one. The first hypothesis concerns both Experiment 1 and 2 and sets out a general effect of ungrammaticality:

Hypothesis A:

Sentences with unlicensed neg-words within the matrix clause are judged as ungrammatical more often when compared to sentences where the licensing negative marker is present.

Next, we propose the hypothesis that an illusion effect caused by the presence of negation and a neg-word in a relative clause ameliorates ungrammaticality in the speeded paradigm, but not in the untimed acceptability judgement task, as attraction and NPI illusion effects are seen in speeded tasks and early measures (Phillips et al. 2011):

Hypothesis B:

Sentences with unlicensed neg-words within the matrix clause that include a relative clause with a neg-word licensed by negation are judged as grammatical more often than sentences with a relative clause that does not contain the neg-word and the negation. This occurs when the acceptability judgement task is speeded but not when it is untimed.

2.1 Data availability

The reader may consult the stimuli used in the study, the anonymized data obtained, and the statistical analysis scripts on the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/fpza7.

2.2 Methods

2.2.1 Participants

For Experiment 1, we recruited 121 native speakers of Czech from a student participant pool at Charles University, Prague. The participants were compensated through course credit for their time. We excluded eight participants who reported having grown up bilingual. Next, we excluded one participant whose mean response time (RT) was around 200 ms with many individual trials being below 100 ms, indicating that they had not paid adequate attention to the task. There were 93 women and 28 men in the participant sample. The mean age was 22.6 (SD = 5.2) with 19 being the minimum and 53 the maximum age. Experiment 2 used the same participant pool with 53 people recruited. The demographics were the following: 12 men, 41 women. We excluded four participants based on the bilingualism criterion. The mean age of the sample was 22.8 (SD = 4.6, range 20–52). We first conducted Experiment 1, and afterwards selected another group of students from the participant pool to whom we sent the link to Experiment 2. The two samples did not overlap. We gathered no information regarding the participants’ medical history (e.g., dyslexia, autism).[2]

2.2.2 Materials

We constructed 32 items sets with four versions of each item corresponding to our conditions, which all contained the quantifier neg-word žádný ‘no’ (for experimental evidence on the licensing conditions of this neg-word in Czech, see Dočekal 2020) modifying a direct object in the subject-last position. The preceding material included the main verb of the matrix clause which was either in the affirmative or had the negative prefix ne-. This was our matrix clause negation manipulation (hereafter MCNeg). The subject of the matrix clause was modified by a subject relative clause, which was our relative clause negation manipulation (RCNeg). The clause contained either a licensed neg-word with a negated verb or no negation at all.

Take the example item presented in Table 1. The condition in (d) in Table 1 is the crucial one – it is here that the lack of the negative prefix ne- in the matrix clause makes the sentence ungrammatical, but where we hypothesize that the presence of the negated verb in the relative clause causes an illusion of grammaticality. We expect this condition to be judged as grammatical more often when compared to the condition in (b), in which there is no negation in the relative clause that could potentially serve as the “illusory licensor” or “attractor”. We expect this difference to only be present in Experiment 1 (speeded acceptability) and not Experiment 2 (untimed acceptability).

Table 1:

Experimental item example from Experiments 1 and 2 with glosses according to the Leipzig glossing rules (Comrie et al. 2008).

a. Minulý měsíc ne-vyhrál karbaník, který hledá novou práci, žádnou sázku.
last month neg-won gambler who seeks new job no bet
b. *Minulý měsíc vyhrál karbaník, který hledá novou práci, žádnou sázku.
last month won gambler who seeks new job no bet
c. Minulý měsíc ne-vyhrál karbaník, který ne-hledá žádnou práci, žádnou sázku.
last month neg-won gambler who neg-seeks no job no bet
d. *Minulý měsíc vyhrál karbaník, který ne-hledá žádnou práci, žádnou sázku.
last month won gambler who neg-seeks no job no bet
‘Last month, the gambler, who is seeking a new/no job, didn’t win any bet.’

We also constructed 64 filler sentences that were of varying levels of acceptability. These included completely acceptable sentences such as Jan uviděl dvě policejní auta ‘Jan saw two police cars’, some of which were negated (Někteří lidé se vůbec nebojí pavouků ‘Some people aren’t afraid of spiders at all’). We also included ungrammatical sentences, for example, sentences with gender agreement errors such as *Jarka dnes dostal svoji poštu ‘Jarka.F received.M her post today’, and semantically malformed ones (Nůž se řízl maminkou ‘The knife was cut by the mum’). The ratio of grammatical to ungrammatical sentences in the combined filler experimental stimuli set was 1:1. Therefore, the correct answer ratio was ‘yes’ to half of the overall sentences used and ‘no’ to the other half.

2.2.3 Procedure

Both experiments were conducted online on the PCIbex platform (Zehr and Schwarz 2018). Participants were provided with a hyperlink which hosted the experiment and were given a week to take part in it at their convenience. Upon clicking on the link, the participants first filled out a consent form and a demographic questionnaire. Here, they were asked to provide their registration email address for the participant pool to claim course credit. After receiving instructions, they saw eight practice items. In both experiments, they rated 32 experimental and 64 filler items organized using the Latin square design with pseudo-randomization employed. Participants always saw two filler items between a single critical one. Therefore, no experimental items were adjacent to one another. Each participant saw only one version of each of the quartets of the 32 experimental stimuli sentences. Therefore, no participant ever judged each sentential frame more than once. This was done to avoid participants noticing the manipulation by seeing two or more minimally different sentences. Since we had four conditions, each participant saw eight exemplars of each condition.

In Experiment 1, the participants were told to judge whether the sentences presented sounded natural to them or not by pressing J for ‘yes’ and F for ‘no’. All sentences were presented in the RSVP mode (Potter 2018) with 350 ms per word. After each sentence, the participants had 3 seconds to make their response regarding its acceptability. In Experiment 2, we used a Likert scale ranging from 1 (very unnatural) to 7 (very natural). The participants saw the sentences as ordinary text. There was no time limit for their answer.

2.2.4 Analysis

When analysing the data from Experiment 1, we first excluded any trials with RTs lower than 200 ms, a time we deemed too short for the participants to make a judgement under the condition that they processed the sentence fully. This resulted in 4.1 % of data loss. We then ran a generalized linear mixed-effects model using the lme4 and lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al. 2017) packages in the R programming language (R Core Team 2023) predicting yes-no responses. We used a nested model to test the effect of RCNeg (i.e., the attractor or illusory licensor) within MCNeg (i.e., grammaticality). The random-effects structure of these models included the maximal specification with intercepts and random slopes (main effects and interaction) for both participants and items (Barr et al. 2013).

We also conducted an exploratory analysis of log-transformed RTs (both ‘yes’ and ‘no’ responses). We ran a linear mixed-effects model with the same fixed effects as the responses model. To aid convergence, the random effects were simplified to having only the intercepts for participants and items and the slopes for the main effect of MCNeg for both.

In the analysis of Experiment 2, we first excluded all answers which took the participant in question over 20 seconds, judging this to be a sign of them being distracted or otherwise not being fully focused on the task. This resulted in 3.19 % of data being discarded. We then ran a cumulative link mixed model on the ordinal rating data using the package ordinal (Christensen 2023) with the same nested structure. The random-effects structure included intercepts for participants and items and the slope for the main effect of MCNeg for participants.

2.3 Results

2.3.1 Experiment 1: speeded acceptability

The reader may consult the mean proportion of ‘yes’ responses by condition from Experiment 1 in Figure 1 and in Table 2 and the mean RTs of both types of responses in Figure 2 and in Table 3. The results of the models are summarized in Tables 4 and 5.

Figure 1: 
Mean proportion of ‘yes’ responses by condition from Experiment 1, with standard errors.
Figure 1:

Mean proportion of ‘yes’ responses by condition from Experiment 1, with standard errors.

Table 2:

Descriptive statistics of mean acceptance in Experiment 1 by condition.

Sentence (as per Table 1) MCNeg RCNeg Mean acceptance SD
a. Negated Affirmative 0.68 0.46
b. Affirmative Affirmative 0.08 0.27
c. Negated Negated 0.58 0.49
d. Affirmative Negated 0.14 0.35
Figure 2: 
Mean response times (RTs) of ‘yes’ and ‘no’ responses by condition from Experiment 1, with standard errors.
Figure 2:

Mean response times (RTs) of ‘yes’ and ‘no’ responses by condition from Experiment 1, with standard errors.

Table 3:

Descriptive statistics of response times (RTs) in milliseconds by condition in Experiment 1.

Sentence (as per Table 1) MCNeg RCNeg Mean RTs in ms SD
a. Negated Affirmative 820 641
b. Affirmative Affirmative 631 448
c. Negated Negated 860 661
d. Affirmative Negated 780 591
Table 4:

The fixed effects of the nested responses model (Experiment 1).

Fixed effects Estimate SE z value p value
(Intercept) −1.109 0.167 −6.646 <0.001***
MCNeg 1.828 0.101 18.07 <0.001***
MCNeg (ungrammatical): RCNeg 0.510 0.142 3.58 <0.001***
MCNeg (grammatical): RCNeg −0.272 0.089 −3.05 0.0023**
Table 5:

The fixed effects of the nested log-RTs model (Experiment 1).

Fixed effects Estimate SE df t value p value
(Intercept) 6.420 0.030 112.1 216.16 <0.001***
MCNeg 0.075 0.015 54.74 5.11 <0.001***
MCNeg (ungrammatical): RCNeg 0.082 0.013 3,184 6.21 <0.001***
MCNeg (grammatical): RCNeg 0.019 0.013 3,183 1.45 0.148

The response model revealed that there was a main effect of MCNeg – ungrammatical sentences were judged as acceptable less often compared to grammatical ones. Crucially, there was a significant effect of RCNeg within the ungrammatical condition, suggesting that the negation in the relative clause increased acceptance rates. The effect of RCNeg was in the opposite direction in the grammatical MCNeg condition, associated with lower rates of acceptance.

As for RTs, there was a main effect of MCNeg and within this factor, RCNeg was significant only when nested within the affirmative (ungrammatical) condition, where RTs were longer with the negated (illusory) relative clause.

2.3.2 Experiment 2: untimed judgements

For the untimed judgements of Experiment 2, we report the mean ratings per condition in Figure 3 and in Table 6. The results of the model are in Table 7.

The nested model showed a main effect of MCNeg. Again, ungrammatical sentences were judged by comprehenders as less acceptable. The effect of RCNeg was significant only in the grammatical condition and (marginally) insignificant in the ungrammatical one.

Figure 3: 
Mean ratings by condition from Experiment 2, with standard errors.
Figure 3:

Mean ratings by condition from Experiment 2, with standard errors.

Table 6:

Descriptive statistics of mean ratings in Experiment 2 by condition.

Sentence (as per Table 1) MCNeg RCNeg Mean ratings SD
a. Negated Affirmative 4.58 1.94
b. Affirmative Affirmative 2.03 1.11
c. Negated Negated 3.96 1.92
d. Affirmative Negated 2.24 1.35
Table 7:

The fixed effects of the nested ratings model (Experiment 2).

Fixed effects Estimate SE z value p value
MCNeg 1.767 0.201 8.79 <0.001***
MCNeg (ungrammatical): RCNeg 0.129 0.072 1.80 0.0725
MCNeg (grammatical): RCNeg −0.454 0.069 −6.58 <0.001***

3 Discussion

Let us start with our hypotheses. We can see that Hypothesis A has been confirmed, since in both experiments, ungrammatical sentences with a missing negative marker in the matrix clause were judged as unacceptable more often compared to grammatical ones. Hypothesis B has also received support, since we saw that the presence of negation and a licensed neg-word in the relative clause increased the probability of acceptance for otherwise ungrammatical sentences in the speeded acceptability Experiment 1 but crucially not in the untimed judgement Experiment 2.

Turning now to interpretations, we can say that our results provide the first evidence for the existence of what might be termed the negative concord illusion effect. We ran two judgement experiments with native speakers of Czech where we exposed them to sentences with an unlicensed neg-word where an intervening relative clause was providing an illusory licensor in the form of a negated verb. We found that the inclusion of this syntactically irrelevant negation caused Czech comprehenders to come under a grammaticality illusion in a speeded acceptability task. They judged ungrammatical sentences as acceptable more often when a relative clause contained a negated verb compared to when it was non-negated. However, this effect disappeared when the task did not have a time limit, a pattern typical of linguistic illusions (Phillips et al. 2011).

The RT results also support the illusion interpretation, since we found that comprehenders took longer in the ungrammatical condition when the illusory licensor negation was in place. Presumably, they were experiencing interference from the syntactically irrelevant negation and had to suppress the tendency to answer incorrectly. On the other hand, the grammatical conditions took longer and there was no difference between them. These longer RTs could be evidence of comprehenders weighing in factors beyond syntactic well-formedness and evaluating the sentences’ plausibility and pragmatic felicity.

What requires discussion is our finding of an effect of the negation in the relative clause on the acceptability of grammatical sentences. Prima facie, this appears to contrast with cases of agreement attraction, where “illusions of ungrammaticality” have mostly not been observed (Hammerly et al. 2019; Wagers et al. 2009).[3] However, we argue against this position. The stimuli in the affirmative relative clause conditions replaced the neg-word with an additional modifier (this was done to maintain the same sentence length across conditions), which added more semantic content. This could have increased the sentences’ plausibility. Overall, negated sentences are also more difficult to process in comparison to affirmative ones and they are less felicitous in “out of the blue” contexts (for an overview, see Kaup and Dudschig 2020). We believe that the lower acceptability of grammatical sentences with another negation in the relative clause was due to their decreased pragmatic felicity. That the effect was present even in the untimed Experiment 2 is evidence of this.

One argument against the illusion interpretation is that the two negations present in one of the conditions were causing general processing difficulties resulting in more uncertainty, leading to responses being drawn towards the chance level of 50 %, which would explain the opposite effect of the negation in the relative clause in the grammatical and ungrammatical conditions. This, however, cannot explain the asymmetric pattern observed in the untimed Experiment 2.

It is interesting to compare our negative concord results in Czech to the ones obtained in the domain of agreement attraction in the same language. In general, Czech comprehenders have been found to be mostly immune to the effect and to exhibit it only under the conditions of case syncretism, that is, the sameness of form between different combinations of case and other features (Caha 2019), with very small effect sizes (Chromý et al. 2023a, 2023b; Lacina and Chromý 2022; Lacina and Dotlačil 2024). In comparison, the illusion effect seen here appears to be robust. We speculate that this could be due to the negative marker ne- being invariant in its use – negation is signalled in the same way whether it is in the matrix or relative clause. Therefore, the situation is similar to the syncretic cases where attraction emerges in Czech in that the attractor (or illusory licensor) carries the same form as the correct verb, which allows the illusion to surface. While there has been no study on NPI illusions in Czech, experimental studies have contrasted neg-words and strong NPIs in the language with the focus on neg-raising (Dočekal 2014, 2020]; Dočekal and Dotlačil 2016, 2018]), suggesting that in Czech, neg-words and strong NPIs are distinct categories and are licensed in different environments.

Overall, the results bear the hallmarks of two other linguistic illusions which have been prolifically studied, namely agreement attraction (e.g., Wagers et al. 2009) and NPI illusions (e.g., Vasishth et al. 2008). Yet, under which umbrella the negative concord effect ought to be subsumed cannot be decided based on the current data. However, NPI illusions and agreement attraction have been found to be differently sensitive to specific manipulations, chief among them distance. Parker and Phillips (2016) and Muller (2022) showed that when the NPI is moved further away from the illusory licensor, the effect disappears. Yet the same was not the case for agreement attraction (Experiment 7 in Parker and Phillips 2016). Whether the negative concord illusion is susceptible to distance manipulation could tell us how to classify it.

It has recently been found that NPI illusions are sensitive to both the type of licensor used (Orth et al. 2021) and the type of NPI (Schwab 2022). In English, it was found that simple sentential negation does not give rise to illusions (Orth et al. 2021), but the use of negative quantifiers does. In Turkish, on the other hand, sentential negation has been found to elicit the effect (Yanilmaz and Drury 2018). In our study, we not only used sentential negation in the relative clause, but also another negative quantifier. Whether the illusion stays or disappears when the quantifier is removed and only sentential negation is kept would also be informative regarding the categorization status of the observed effect.

Additionally, further data examining this illusion could also be used as evidence in the theoretical debate on the nature of negative concord. Should it turn out that the processing profile of negative concord patterns more with the behaviour of agreement attraction, it would suggest that the phenomenon ought to be analysed as syntactic agreement. On the other hand, if the negative concord illusion ended up having the characteristics typical of NPI illusions, this would point in favour of subsuming negative concord under NPIs.

While this study provided the first evidence for the existence of an illusion in the processing of negative concord, these results need to be supplemented with further studies to ascertain its robustness. The use of different methods, such self-paced reading or corpus examinations of whether the phenomenon exists in speech errors, and cross-linguistic validation on other closely related Slavic languages such as Polish, and both strict negative concord languages (such as Greek or Hungarian) and non-strict ones, that only allow in certain sentential structures (such as many Romance languages; Giannakidou and Zeijlstra 2017), are in order.

4 Conclusions

The current study revealed that there is a type of grammaticality illusion that can be induced in languages exhibiting negative concord, that is, the requirement of neg-words for sentential negation in the same clause. This was shown in a speeded acceptability experiment with Czech comprehenders where an irrelevant negation in a relative clause caused the illusory acceptance of an unlicensed neg-word in the matrix clause. This effect disappeared in an untimed task. Further research is needed to determine whether this negative concord illusion is a case of NPI or agreement-related processes. This could be done by making use of the previously discovered differential sensitivity of the illusion effect to factors such as distance (Muller 2022).


Corresponding author: Radim Lacina, Department of Czech Language, Faculty of Arts, Masaryk University, Arne Nováka 1, 60200 Brno, Czechia; and Institute of Cognitive Science, Osnabrück University, Wachsbleiche 27, 49090 Osnabrück, German, E-mail:

Award Identifier / Grant number: GO 3378/1-1

Acknowledgment

The author was supported by an Emmy-Noether grant from the German Research Foundation awarded to Nicole Gotzner (GO 3378/1-1, 441607011) and by the German Academic Exchange Service (DAAD, Scholarship programme Nr. 57694189). I would like to thank Jan Chromý for providing me with access to the student participant pool at Charles University, to the attendees of the Biennial of Czech Linguistics conference where this research was presented for their feedback, and to Colin Phillips for inspiring this work through his seminar on linguistic illusions at Oxford University.

References

Avetisyan, Serine, Sol Lago & Shravan Vasishth. 2020. Does case marking affect agreement attraction in comprehension? Journal of Memory and Language 112. 104087. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2020.104087.Suche in Google Scholar

Barr, Dale J., Roger Levy, Christoph Scheepers & Harry J. Tily. 2013. Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 68(3). 255–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001.Suche in Google Scholar

Blanchette, Frances, Marianna Nadeu, Jeremy Yeaton & Viviane Deprez. 2018. English negative concord and double negation: The division of labor between syntax and pragmatics. Proceedings of the Linguistic Society of America 3(1). 53. https://doi.org/10.3765/plsa.v3i1.4349.Suche in Google Scholar

Caha, Pavel. 2019. Syncretism in morphology. In Oxford research encyclopedia of linguistics. Oxford University Press.10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.552Suche in Google Scholar

Christensen, Rune H. B. 2023. ordinal: Regression models for ordinal data, version 2023.12-4.1. R package. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ordinal.Suche in Google Scholar

Chromý, Jan, Radim Lacina & Jakub Dotlačil. 2023a. Number agreement attraction in Czech comprehension: Negligible facilitation effects. Open Mind 7. 802–836. https://doi.org/10.1162/opmi_a_00107.Suche in Google Scholar

Chromý, Jan, James Liam Brand, Anna Laurinavichyute & Radim Lacina. 2023b. Number agreement attraction in Czech and English comprehension: A direct experimental comparison. Glossa Psycholinguistics 2(1). https://doi.org/10.5070/G6011235.Suche in Google Scholar

Comrie, Bernard, Martin Haspelmath & Balthasar Bickel. 2008. The Leipzig glossing rules: Conventions for interlinear morpheme-by-morpheme glosses. https://www.eva.mpg.de/lingua/pdf/Glossing-Rules.pdf.Suche in Google Scholar

De Swart, Henriëtte. 2009. Expression and interpretation of negation: An OT typology. Dordrecht: Springer.Suche in Google Scholar

De Swart, Henriëtte & Ivan A. Sag. 2002. Negation and negative concord in romance. Linguistics and Philosophy 25(4). 373–417. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1020823106639.10.1023/A:1020823106639Suche in Google Scholar

Dempsey, Jack, Kiel Christianson & Darren Tanner. 2022. Misretrieval but not misrepresentation: A feature misbinding account of post-interpretive effects in number attraction. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 75(9). 1727–1745. https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218211061578.Suche in Google Scholar

Dočekal, Mojmír. 2014. Intervention effects: Negation, universal quantifiers, and Czech. In Ludmila Veselovská & Markéta Janebová (eds.), Complex visibles out there, 369–387. Olomouc, Czech Republic: Palacký University.Suche in Google Scholar

Dočekal, Mojmír. 2020. N-words and NPIs: Between syntax, semantics, and experiments. In Franc Marušič, Petra Mišmaš & Rok Žaucer (eds.), Advances in formal Slavic linguistics 2017, 15–49. Berlin: Language Science Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Dočekal, Mojmír & Jakub Dotlačil. 2016. Experimental evidence for neg-raising in Slavic. Linguistica 56(1). 93–109. https://doi.org/10.4312/linguistica.56.1.93-109.Suche in Google Scholar

Dočekal, Mojmír & Jakub Dotlačil. 2018. When is not-believing believing that not? Proceedings of Sinn und Bedeutung 21(1). 387–398.Suche in Google Scholar

Drenhaus, Heiner, Stefan Frisch & Douglas Saddy. 2005. Processing negative polarity items: When negation comes through the backdoor. In Stephan Kepser & Marga Reis (eds.), Linguistic evidence: Empirical, theoretical, and computational perspectives, 145–164. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.10.1515/9783110197549.145Suche in Google Scholar

Druckman, James N. & Cindy D. Kam. 2011. Students as experimental participants: A defense of the “narrow data base”. In James N. Druckman, Donald P. Greene, James H. Kuklinski & Arthur Lupia (eds.), Cambridge handbook of experimental political science, 41–57. New York: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511921452.004Suche in Google Scholar

Eberhard, Kathleen M., J. Cooper Cutting & Kathryn Bock. 2005. Making syntax of sense: Number agreement in sentence production. Psychological Review 112(3). 531–559. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.112.3.531.Suche in Google Scholar

Engelmann, Felix, Lena A. Jäger & Shravan Vasishth. 2019. The effect of prominence and cue association on retrieval processes: A computational account. Cognitive Science 43(12). e12800. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12800.Suche in Google Scholar

Franck, Julie, Saveria Colonna & Luigi Rizzi. 2015. Task-dependency and structure-dependency in number interference effects in sentence comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology 6. 349. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00349.Suche in Google Scholar

Giannakidou, Anastasia. 2000. Negative … concord? Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 18(3). 457–523. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1006477315705.10.1023/A:1006477315705Suche in Google Scholar

Giannakidou, Anastasia. 2006. N-words and negative concord. In Martin Everaert & Henk van Riemsdijk (eds.), The Blackwell companion to syntax, vol. 1, 327–391. Malden, MA: Wiley.10.1002/9780470996591.ch45Suche in Google Scholar

Giannakidou, Anastasia & Urtzi Etxeberria. 2018. Assessing the role of experimental evidence for interface judgment: Licensing of negative polarity items, scalar readings, and focus. Frontiers in Psychology 9. 59. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00059.Suche in Google Scholar

Giannakidou, Anastasia & Josep Quer. 1995. Two mechanisms for the licensing of negative indefinites. In 6th annual meeting of the formal linguistics society of Mid-America (FLSM VI), 103–114. Bloomington: Indiana University Linguistics Club Publications.Suche in Google Scholar

Giannakidou, Anastasia & Josep Quer. 1997. Long-distance licensing of negative indefinites. In Danielle Forget, María Luisa Rivero, France Martineau & Paul Hirschbühler (eds.), Negation and polarity, 95–114. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Suche in Google Scholar

Giannakidou, Anastasia & Hedde Zeijlstra. 2017. The landscape of negative dependencies: Negative concord and n-words. The Wiley Blackwell companion to syntax, 2nd edn 1–38. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.10.1002/9781118358733.wbsyncom102Suche in Google Scholar

Gonzalez Alonso, Jorge, Ian Cunnings, Hiroki Fujita, David Miller & Jason Rothman. 2021. Gender attraction in sentence comprehension. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 6(1). https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.1300.Suche in Google Scholar

Haegeman, Liliane & Raffaella Zanuttini. 1991. Negative heads and the neg criterion. The Linguistic Review 8. 233–252. https://doi.org/10.1515/tlir.1991.8.2-4.233.Suche in Google Scholar

Hammerly, Christopher, Adrian Staub & Brian Dillon. 2019. The grammaticality asymmetry in agreement attraction reflects response bias: Experimental and modeling evidence. Cognitive Psychology 110. 70–104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2019.01.001.Suche in Google Scholar

Häussler, Jana. 2009. The emergence of attraction errors during sentence comprehension. Konstanz: Universität Konstanz PhD thesis.Suche in Google Scholar

Jäger, Lena A., Daniela Mertzen, Julie A. Van Dyke & Shravan Vasishth. 2020. Interference patterns in subject-verb agreement and reflexives revisited: A large-sample study. Journal of Memory and Language 111. 104063. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2019.104063.Suche in Google Scholar

Kaup, Barbara & Carolin Dudschig. 2020. Understanding negation: Issues in the processing of negation. In Viviane Déprez & M. Teresa Espinal (eds.), The Oxford handbook of negation, 635–655. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198830528.013.33Suche in Google Scholar

Keshev, Maayan, Mandy Cartner, Aya Meltzer-Asscher & Dillon Brian. 2024. A working memory model of sentence processing as binding morphemes to syntactic positions. Topics in Cognitive Science 17(1). 88–105. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12780.Suche in Google Scholar

Kuznetsova, Alexandra, Per B. Brockhoff & Rune H. B. Christensen and others. 2017. lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software 82(13). 1–26. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13.Suche in Google Scholar

Kwon, Nayoung & Patrick Sturt. 2016. Attraction effects in honorific agreement in Korean. Frontiers in Psychology 7. 1302. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01302.Suche in Google Scholar

Lacina, Radim & Jan Chromý. 2022. No agreement attraction facilitation observed in Czech: Not even syncretism helps. In Proceedings of the annual meeting of the cognitive science society, vol. 44, 2423–2430. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/2vq4g622.10.31234/osf.io/ktyv8Suche in Google Scholar

Lacina, Radim & Jakub Dotlačil. 2024. Grammaticality illusions in Czech: A speeded acceptability study of agreement attraction. In Proceedings of the annual meeting of the cognitive science society, vol. 46. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/86945505.10.31234/osf.io/3rdc7Suche in Google Scholar

Ladusaw, William A. 1992. Expressing negation. Proceedings of SALT 2. 237–260. https://doi.org/10.3765/salt.v2i0.3030.Suche in Google Scholar

Lago, Sol, Diego E. Shalom, Mariano Sigman, Ellen F. Lau & Colin Phillips. 2015. Agreement attraction in Spanish comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language 82. 133–149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2015.02.002.Suche in Google Scholar

Lago, Sol, Martina Gračanin-Yuksek, Duygu Fatma Şafak, Orhan Demir, Bilal Kırkıcı & Claudia Felser. 2019. Straight from the horse’s mouth: Agreement attraction effects with Turkish possessors. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 9(3). 398–426. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.17019.lag.Suche in Google Scholar

Laka, Itziar. 1990. Negation in syntax: On the nature of functional categories and projections. Cambridge: Massachusetts Institute of Technology PhD thesis.Suche in Google Scholar

Leivada, Evelina & Marit Westergaard. 2020. Acceptable ungrammatical sentences, unacceptable grammatical sentences, and the role of the cognitive parser. Frontiers in Psychology 11. 364. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00364.Suche in Google Scholar

Lewis, Richard L. & Shravan Vasishth. 2005. An activation-based model of sentence processing as skilled memory retrieval. Cognitive Science 29(3). 375–419. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_25.Suche in Google Scholar

Logačev, Pavel & Shravan Vasishth. 2016. A multiple-channel model of task-dependent ambiguity resolution in sentence comprehension. Cognitive Science 40(2). 266–298. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12228.Suche in Google Scholar

Mendia, Jon Ander, Ethan Poole & Brian Dillon. 2018. Spurious NPI licensing and exhaustification. Proceedings of SALT 28. 233–250. https://doi.org/10.3765/salt.v28i0.4437.Suche in Google Scholar

Muller, Hanna Ellen. 2022. What could go wrong? Linguistic illusions and incremental interpretation. College Park: University of Maryland, College Park PhD thesis.Suche in Google Scholar

Orth, Wesley, Masaya Yoshida & Shayne Sloggett. 2021. Negative polarity item (NPI) illusion is a quantification phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 47(6). 906. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000957.Suche in Google Scholar

Parker, Dan & Adam An. 2018. Not all phrases are equally attractive: Experimental evidence for selective agreement attraction effects. Frontiers in Psychology 9. 1566. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01566.Suche in Google Scholar

Parker, Dan & Colin Phillips. 2016. Negative polarity illusions and the format of hierarchical encodings in memory. Cognition 157. 321–339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.08.016.Suche in Google Scholar

Paspali, Anastasia & Theodoros Marinis. 2020. Gender agreement attraction in Greek comprehension. Frontiers in Psychology 11. 717. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00717.Suche in Google Scholar

Patson, Nikole D. & E. Matthew Husband. 2016. Misinterpretations in agreement and agreement attraction. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 69(5). 950–971. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.992445.Suche in Google Scholar

Phillips, Colin, Matthew W. Wagers & Ellen F. Lau. 2011. Grammatical illusions and selective fallibility in real-time language comprehension. In Jeffrey Runner (ed.), Experiments at the interfaces, 147–180. Leiden: Brill.10.1163/9781780523750_006Suche in Google Scholar

Potter, Mary C. 2018. Rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP): A method for studying language processing. In David E. Kieras & Marcel A. Just (eds.), New methods in reading comprehension research, 91–118. Abingdon-on-Thames: Routledge.10.4324/9780429505379-5Suche in Google Scholar

R Core Team. 2023. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/.Suche in Google Scholar

Royer, Adam J. 2021. The role of prosodic phrasing on the acceptability of agreement attraction in English. Los Angeles: University of California, Los Angeles.Suche in Google Scholar

Schwab, Juliane. 2022. Lexical variation in NPI illusions: NPI illusions as a scalar phenomenon. Glossa Psycholinguistics 1(1). 21. https://doi.org/10.5070/G6011106.Suche in Google Scholar

Slioussar, Natalia. 2018. Forms and features: The role of syncretism in number agreement attraction. Journal of Memory and Language 101. 51–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2018.03.006.Suche in Google Scholar

Slioussar, Natalia & Anton Malko. 2016. Gender agreement attraction in Russian: Production and comprehension evidence. Frontiers in Psychology 7. 1651. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01651.Suche in Google Scholar

Smith, Garrett, Julie Franck & Whitney Tabor. 2018. A self-organizing approach to subject–verb number agreement. Cognitive Science 42. 1043–1074. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12591.Suche in Google Scholar

Stanczak, Karolina & Isabelle Augenstein. 2021. A survey on gender bias in natural language processing. ArXiv. https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2112.14168.Suche in Google Scholar

Tucker, Matthew A., Ali Idrissi & Diogo Almeida. 2015. Representing number in the real-time processing of agreement: Self-paced reading evidence from Arabic. Frontiers in Psychology 6. 347. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00347.Suche in Google Scholar

Tucker, Matthew A., Ali Idrissi & Diogo Almeida. 2021. Attraction effects for verbal gender and number are similar but not identical: Self-paced reading evidence from modern standard Arabic. Frontiers in Psychology 3774. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.586464.Suche in Google Scholar

Vasishth, Shravan, Sven Brüssow, Richard L. Lewis & Heiner Drenhaus. 2008. Processing polarity: How the ungrammatical intrudes on the grammatical. Cognitive Science 32(4). 685–712. https://doi.org/10.1080/03640210802066865.Suche in Google Scholar

Wagers, Matthew, Ellen Lau & Colin Phillips. 2009. Agreement attraction in comprehension: Representations and processes. Journal of Memory and Language 61(2). 206–237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2009.04.002.Suche in Google Scholar

Wesołek, Sarah, Piotr Gulgowski, Joanna Błaszczak & Marzena Żygis. 2023. Illusions of ungrammaticality in the perception of non-native accented speech. In J. Volín & R. Skarnitzl (eds.), Proceedings of the 20th international congress of phonetic science – ICPhS 2023, 7–11. International Phonetic Association.Suche in Google Scholar

Xiang, Ming, Brian Dillon & Colin Phillips. 2009. Illusory licensing effects across dependency types: ERP evidence. Brain and Language 108(1). 40–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2008.10.002.Suche in Google Scholar

Xiang, Ming, Julian Grove & Anastasia Giannakidou. 2013. Dependency-dependent interference: NPI interference, agreement attraction, and global pragmatic inferences. Frontiers in Psychology 4. 708. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00708.Suche in Google Scholar

Yanilmaz, Aydogan & John E. Drury. 2018. Prospective NPI licensing and intrusion in Turkish. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 33(1). 111–138. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2017.1371779.Suche in Google Scholar

Yun, Jiwon, So-Young Lee & John E. Drury. 2018. Negative polarity illusion in Korean. In Céleste Guillemot, Tomoyuki Yoshida & Seunghun J. Lee (eds.), Proceedings of WAFL (MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 88), 1–10. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Suche in Google Scholar

Zanuttini, Raffaella. 1991. Syntactic properties of sentential negation: A comparative study of Romance languages. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania PhD thesis.Suche in Google Scholar

Zehr, Jeremy & Florian Schwarz. 2018. PennController for internet based experiments (IBEX). https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/MD832.Suche in Google Scholar

Zeijlstra, Hedde. 2004. Sentential negation and negative concord. Utrecht: Utrecht University PhD thesis.Suche in Google Scholar

Zeijlstra, Hedde. 2008. Negative concord is syntactic agreement. Unpublished manuscript. University of Amsterdam.Suche in Google Scholar

Received: 2024-04-09
Accepted: 2025-01-22
Published Online: 2025-02-19

© 2025 the author(s), published by De Gruyter, Berlin/Boston

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Heruntergeladen am 26.9.2025 von https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/lingvan-2024-0061/html
Button zum nach oben scrollen