Home Lexical niche and sustainability: an ecolinguistic perspective
Article Open Access

Lexical niche and sustainability: an ecolinguistic perspective

  • Ruifeng Mo and Haozhang Xiao EMAIL logo
Published/Copyright: July 1, 2024

Abstract

This paper proposes the innovative concept of lexical niche to analyze the ecology of vocabulary from an ecolinguistic perspective. Through the examination of niche breadth and overlap, we quantitatively assess the vitality and competition of six typical Chinese eating verbs: shi (食), fan (饭), can (餐), dan (啖), ru (茹), and chi (吃). The analysis reveals their diachronic evolution in the history of the Chinese language (temporal niche), their synchronic distribution in Chinese dialects (spatial niche), and their verb-object collocation (functional niche). The findings indicate the following: (1) Shi, fan, can, dan, and ru originated in Early Old Chinese, while chi emerged in Late Old Chinese. (2) The niche breadths of the six eating verbs are as follows: 2.585, −2.391, −2.242, −0.108, −1.734, and 3.889, respectively. Chi demonstrates the highest vitality, followed by shi; fan, can, dan, and ru exhibit extremely low vitality. (3) Shi originated in the Pre-Qin dynasty, serving as the dominant eating verb in ancient times with robust competition. However, in modern times, it has evolved into a morpheme for compound words, displaying weak sustainability. Chi, born in the Han dynasty, replaced shi as the dominant eating verb by the late Tang dynasty, establishing itself as the most competitive verb to date, characterized by strong sustainability.

1 Introduction

In recent decades, linguistic research has predominantly concentrated on the internal structures of language and the intricate relationship between language, society, and culture. However, in response to the escalating environmental concerns, an increasing number of scholars have incorporated ecological theories into linguistic studies (Cowley 2021; He et al. 2021). This integration aims to explore the dynamic interplay between language and the environment from an ecological perspective (Ma and Stibbe 2022). Termed ecolinguistics, this field presents a fresh approach to examining linguistic phenomena. Ecolinguistics, as an interdisciplinary science amalgamating linguistics and ecology, directs attention to the interactions between language and the ecological environment. It delves into how language influences human ecological behaviors and, conversely, how the ecological environment shapes the structure and function of language (Ha 2023; Penz and Fill 2022; Zhou 2021). Research topics within ecolinguistics encompass language diversity, language contact, language endangerment, language evolution, language and ecological conservation, language and environmental cognition, and ecological discourse criticism, among others (Penz and Fill 2022). Within these topics, scholars have explored issues such as safeguarding endangered languages, leveraging language for promoting ecological protection, and understanding how language impacts people’s cognition of the environment. This paper, from an ecolinguistic perspective, introduces the concept of lexical niche to scrutinize the ecology of vocabulary. In doing so, it provides a theoretical foundation for the sustainable development of lexis.

2 Literature review

Ecolinguistics emerged in the 1970s as an interdisciplinary science combining linguistics and ecology, aiming to investigate the dynamic interaction between the environment and language (Penz and Fill 2022). Over the past decades, ecolinguistics has undergone distinct research frameworks and theoretical systems, notably the Haugenian paradigm and the Hallidayan paradigm (Fill and Mühlhäusler 2001). The former examines the influence of the environment on language, while the latter explores the impact of language on the environment. Either of the two is complete. Haugen (1970, 1972 introduced the metaphorical concept of the ecology of language to illustrate the relationship between a language and its environment, encompassing social, natural, and psychological aspects. He asserted that linguistic ecology relies on its learners, users, and transmitters. Halliday (1990) argues that language is a form of social practice intricately linked to the ecological environment, emphasizing that language actively shapes reality rather than passively reflecting it. Li (1991) pioneered the application of niche theory from ecology to study the Chinese language, defining linguistic niche as the temporal and spatial distribution of language varieties and the environmental elements they inhabit. Dale and Lupyan (2012) proposed the “linguistic niche hypothesis”, suggesting that language variation correlates with the social environment, particularly language use and learning. Their study of over two thousand languages indicated a relationship between population size and language complexity: fewer speakers led to more intricate inflectional structures, or vice versa. Huang (2018) introduces the concept of harmonious discourse analysis (HDA) in the Chinese context, incorporating principles of conscience, proximity, and regulation to analyze different discourses under the philosophy of the unity of human and nature. Xiao (2021) proposes the theoretical construct of the ecolinguistic continuum, highlighting that lexical and grammatical evolution represents an eco-continuum in that the environments/contexts in which language evolves constitute an eco-continuum. In such a continuum, language interacts and aligns with environments (including natural, social, and cognitive-psychological). Language as behavior affects environments, especially cognitive and psychological ones which then influence ones’ physical behavior to nature; likewise, environments also influence language. It is a dialectical process of interplay between subjective and objective variables (Xiao et al. 2023).

The ecology of language refers to the conditions under which a language survives and evolves in a given environment, encompassing the inherent nature of that language. Humboldt (1836) posited that language directly reflects human organic life in mental activity, endowing language with characteristics akin to organic life. Schleicher (1863) drew parallels between the linguistic organism and the natural organism, asserting that language undergoes a biological life process akin to birth, growth, aging, and death. This perspective aligns the law of language development with the process of biological evolution. Greenberg (1956) statistically analyzed linguistic diversity, combining elements such as linguistic diversity, population distribution, and linguistic structure to gauge the language ecology. Voegelin et al. (1967) formally introduced the term ecology to discuss the intricate relationship between languages, emphasizing the need to analyze both internal language structures and external environmental systems. Labov (1972) explored social influences on language, studying pronunciation differences among citizens of different social and economic backgrounds in New York. His research revealed the invisible and profound impact of the social environment on language change. Goatly (1996) introduced the concept of “green grammar”, emphasizing the ecological factors of the language system and asserting that only harmonious grammar can construct the real world. Mufwene (2001) cited language contact examples, analyzing diachronic and synchronic language evolution. Huang (2000) established language vitality indicators based on three factors, ranking the vitality of minority languages in China. Dai and Deng (2001) addressed endangered languages, proposing core indicators for measurement. Xiao and Fan (2011) introduced a PSR assessment model of language ecology with 12 indicators, assessing language vitality comprehensively. Feng (2013) explored the relationship between language ecology and variation, resources, planning, and education, constructing a theory on language ecology. Stibbe (2020 categorized discourse into beneficial, destructive, and neutral categories, aiming to expose growthism and consumerism and advocate for sustainable development in “the stories we live by” (Ma and Stibbe 2022).

Linguistic niche refers to the temporal and spatial position of a language and its functional relationships with other languages in the linguistic ecosystem. Distinct environments shape varied linguistic niches, resulting in diverse linguistic variants. The size of a linguistic niche dictates its vitality and competition. If the niche continually contracts in competition, reaching a point where the minimum environment for sustainable development is unavailable, the language becomes endangered or even extinct. Lewis and Frank (2016) tested the linguistic niche hypothesis by examining both the internal system (structure) and the external system (environment) of language. Their statistical results indicated that languages in colder, smaller places are more complex and less learnable for non-native speakers. Li (1991) pioneered the application of ecological niche theory to study the Chinese language, defining linguistic niche as the temporal and spatial distribution of language varieties and the environmental elements that influence them. In Ecological Chinese, Li (1991) introduced the ecological Chinese system, discussing the internal and external ecology of Chinese, including linguistic structure, dialects, environment, and niche. Wang (2007) explored the causes and preservation of endangered languages from the perspective of linguistic niche, defining it as the position or status of a language in a specific linguistic ecological environment and among a particular group of people. Wang (2014) studied the ecological status of Hangzhou dialects in three dimensions: function, resources, and space-time, concluding that the ecological status of a language is the sum of the resources and functions it occupies in a particular linguistic ecology during a specific historical period and geographical space. Pan (2015) investigated the sustainability of Chinese speaking and writing among college students, examining micro changes in the Chinese niche under the influence of English. The study identified subtle changes in aspects such as alphabetization of morphemes, lexicalization of words, generalization of transliteration, confusion of sound and spelling, Europeanization of grammar, and translationese. Zhang and Wei (2017) applied the theory of linguistic niche to study the emergence, status, and sustainable development of Chinese honorifics, proposing relevant protection measures. Zhang and Huang (2021) measured the niche of the Hmong language and Mandarin Chinese in Jianhe County (Guizhou, China) from the perspective of sustainable language and culture development, revealing their vitality and competition through temporal and spatial niche breadth and niche overlap.

Most of the above researches are static, synchronic, qualitative, and internal, lacking extensive data for scientific results. For a holistic study, this paper introduces the novel concept of lexical niche. Lexical niche refers to the temporal and spatial distribution of words in the lexical system and the functional relationships between words, allowing for the quantification of language ecology. This study utilizes four corpora for lexical statistics, aiming to investigate the ecology of words in terms of temporal, spatial, and functional niches. By employing niche breadth and niche overlap indices, the paper quantifies the vitality and competition of six Chinese eating verbs: shi (食), fan (饭), can (餐), dan (啖), ru (茹), and chi (吃). The study seeks to answer the questions below: (1) When were the six eating verbs originated? (2) Which among them exhibits the highest vitality? (3) What kind of competition do they undergo during their diachronic evolution?

3 Research methodology

In this section, we introduce the novel concept of lexical niche, focusing on the temporal and spatial distribution of words in the lexical system and the functional relationships between words. The aim is to digitize the ecology of vocabulary and unveil the driving factors behind lexical metabolism.

3.1 Research object

This study centers on the concept of eating, excluding drinking. Six typical eating verbs are selected to quantify the lexical ecology: shi, fan, can, dan, ru, and chi. Noun, quantifier, or other parts of speech that share the same words in the corpora are excluded from the statistics.

3.2 Data source

Data for this study are sourced from historical literature and modern dialects, collected from various corpora and dictionaries, including the Chinese Classics Retrieval System by Shaanxi Normal University,[1] CCL corpus by Peking University (http://ccl.pku.edu.cn:8080/ccl_corpus/index.jsp), BCC corpus by Beijing Language and Culture University (http://bcc.blcu.edu.cn/), Modern Chinese Dialects Dictionary (Li 2002), Dictionary of Chinese Dialects (Xu and Gongtian 2020), Chinese Dialect Vocabulary (Wang 1995), Atlas of Chinese Dialects (Cao 2008), and Language Atlas of China (Xiong and Zhang 2012).

The history of Chinese is categorized into four periods: Early Old Chinese, Middle Old Chinese, Late Old Chinese, and Modern Chinese (Table 1). Correspondingly, four corpora (T1/T2/T3/T4) are constructed to record the frequency of eating verbs across the history of Chinese, which indicates the temporal niche breadth of the eating verbs. To ensure scientific results, colloquial books are selected for each stage, considering that oral language has been a primary driving force for word evolution (Li 2011). Each corpus comprises six books, with balanced sizes, resulting in a total of 24 books. The size of each corpus is 1.568, 1.543, 1.592, and 1.574 million words, respectively.

Table 1:

Diachronic corpus for temporal niche.

History Books Size/million
T1 Early Old Chinese (14th century BC–7th century AD) Zhuangzi (‘Sir Zhuang’) 0.078 1.568
Guoyu (‘Discourses of the States’) 0.088
Lüshi Chunqiu (‘Master Lü’s Spring and Autumn Annals’) 0.123
Hanfeizi (‘Sir Hanfei’) 0.106
Shiji (‘Historical Records’) 0.526
Shisanjing Zhushu (‘Annotations of the Thirteen Classics’) 0.647
T2 Middle Old Chinese (7th–14th century AD) Soushen Ji (‘Anecdotes about Spirits and Immortals’) 0.071 1.543
Lunheng (‘On Balance’) 0.246
Qimin Yaoshu (‘Essential Techniques for the Welfare of the People’) 0.141
Shishuo Xinyu (‘A New Account of the Tales of the World’) 0.140
Yanshi Jiaxun (‘Family instructions of Master Yan’) 0.042
Hanshu (‘History of the Han Dynasty’) 0.903
T3 Late Old Chinese (17th–early 20th century) Xinwudai Shi (‘New History of the Five Dynasties’) 0.348 1.592
Wangfanzhi Shi (‘Poems by Wang Fanzhi’) 0.020
Dunhuang Bianwenji Xinshu (‘Ballads and Stories from Dunhuang’) 0.194
Rutang Qiufa Xunli Xingji (‘The Record of a Pilgrimage to China in Search of the Law’) 0.082
Zutang Ji (‘Collection of the Patriarchal Hall’) 0.249
Wudeng Huiyuan (‘Compendium of Five Lamps’) 0.699
T4 Modern Chinese (early 20th century – now) Yuanchao Mishi (‘The Secret History of the Mongols’) 0.048 1.574
Fusheng Liuji (‘Six Records of a Floating Life’) 0.036
Qingpingshantang Huaben (‘Novellas Printed by the Qingping Mountain Pavilion’) 0.137
Yuankan Zajv Sanshi Zhong (‘The Thirty Zaju Plays in Yuan Printings’) 0.130
Chuke Paian Jingqi (‘Amazing Tales-First Series’) 0.362
Hongloumeng (‘A Dream of Red Mansions’) 0.861

The spatial niche breadth of eating verbs is determined by their distribution across various dialectal regions. In the context of Chinese dialects, different dictionaries and atlases propose varying classifications, such as seven dialects, eight dialects, and ten dialects. These differences stem primarily from the subdivision of larger dialectal groups. For instance, the Guanhua area can be further categorized into eight sub-regions, including Northeast, Southwest, Jiaoliao, Beijing, Jilu, Zhongyuan, Jianghuai, and Lanyi dialects. Given their proximity to Mandarin/Putonghua (the official Chinese language), these eight dialects generally do not pose significant communication barriers and can be regarded as a cohesive dialectal family. In our data collection process, we adhere to the categorization outlined in the Modern Chinese Dialect Dictionary (Li 2002), which classifies Chinese dialects into ten distinct groups: Guanhua, Jinyu, Huiyu, Wuyu, Minyu, Xiangyu, Ganyu, Hakka, Cantonese, and Pinghua, as illustrated in Table 2.

Table 2:

Division of Chinese dialects (according to Li 2002).

Division of Chinese dialects
D1 D2 D3 D4 D5 D6 D7 D8 D9 D10
D1 D1 D1 D1 D1 D1 D1 D1
I II III IV V VI VII VIII
  1. Note: D1, Guanhua; I, Dongbei Guanhua; II, Jiaoliao Guanhua; III, Beijing Guanhua; IV, Jilu Guanhua; V, Zhongyuan Guanhua; VI, Jianghuai Guanhua; VII, Lanyin Guanhua; VIII, Xi’an Guanhua; D2, Jinyu; D3, Huiyu; D4, Wuyu; D5, Xiangyu; D6, Ganyu; D7, Hakka; D8, Minyu; D9, Cantonese; D10, Pinghua.

The functional niche breadth is delineated by the collocative objects associated with the six eating verbs. In this study, the collocations of eating verbs were extracted from a corpus constructed for this purpose and pertinent Chinese dialect dictionaries. Fourteen categories of objects were identified, including meal, cake, meat, fruit, vegetable, grass, porridge, sauce, tea, wine, water, milk, cigarette, and gas, as presented in Table 3.

Table 3:

Object categories of the six eating verbs.

Eating verbs Objects
Shi Meal, cake, meat, fruit, vegetable, grass, porridge, sauce, tea, wine, water, milk, cigarette, gas
Fan
Can
Dan
Ru
Chi

3.3 Research method

This study delves into the ecological position and evolution of six eating verbs, exploring these aspects in three dimensions: temporal, spatial, and functional. The temporal dimension investigates the diachronic evolution of the verbs, the spatial dimension examines their synchronic distribution, and the functional dimension explores their collocation. The study exclusively focuses on eating verbs, necessitating data tagging to precisely identify the verbs. Natural Language Processing & Information Retrieval Sharing Platform (NLPIR) (http://www.nlpir.org) is employed for lexical segmentation and tagging, followed by manual proofreading to ensure accuracy. R 4.2.2 is utilized for computing the lexical niche, while Python 3.1.1 is applied for data visualization.

The study employs the niche metric to quantitatively describe the ecological niches of the selected verbs and explore interspecific relationships. This metric is typically measured using two indicators: niche breadth and niche overlap. Niche breadth represents the sum of various resources available to an organism, indicating its adaptation to the environment and resource utilization. A wider niche suggests a generalist species, while a narrower niche signifies specialization. Specialized species with low vitality face disadvantages in resource competition, whereas generalized species with high vitality exhibit strong competitive abilities. Niche overlap reflects the efficiency and degree of resource sharing among species. According to the principle of competitive exclusion, greater overlap leads to more potential competition, influencing community structure and distribution. Potential competition may not necessarily occur, and the intensity of competition can be reflected by the increase or decrease of data. The lexical niche breadth (Bi) is determined by the standardized number of resources occupied by verb i. Temporal niche breadth is denoted by the frequency of eating verbs across different historical periods of the Chinese language. Spatial niche breadth is characterized by the number of dialectal regions where the eating verb is distributed. Functional niche breadth is indicated by the categories of the eating verb’s collocative objects. Lexical niche overlap (O ij ) is quantified by the ratio of the number of resources shared by verb i and verb j to the total number of resources (periods of Chinese language, dialectal regions, collocative objects). The niche overlap index ranges from 0 to 1. A value of 1 signifies complete overlap between verb i and verb j, indicating potential competition. Conversely, a value of 0 indicates no overlap between the two verbs, signifying no competition. A higher niche overlap index suggests a greater potential for competition.

4 Result and discussion

4.1 Niche breadth and vitality of eating verbs

Based on the mean value of three-dimensional niche breadth (Table 4), the eating verbs could be categorized into three categories: narrow niche (Bi < 0), medium niche (0 ≤ Bi < 1), and wide niche (Bi ≥ 1). The mean values of three-dimensional niche breadth for the eating verbs are as follows: chi (3.889), shi (2.585), dan (−0.108), ru (−1.734), can (−2.242), fan (−2.391). Dan, ru, can, and fan fall into the category of narrow niche verbs, exhibiting low vitality and sustainability. Chi and shi are classified as wide niche verbs, showcasing high vitality and sustainability.

Table 4:

Niche breadth of six eating verbs.

Niche breadth Shi Fan Can Dan Ru Chi
Temporal 1.563 −0.634 −0.640 −0.595 −0.666 0.972
Spatial 0.063 −0.695 −0.695 −0.316 −0.316 1.957
Functional 0.959 −1.062 −0.907 0.803 −0.751 0.959
Mean value 2.585 −2.391 −2.242 −0.108 −1.734 3.889

4.1.1 Temporal niche breadth and vitality

In terms of temporal niche (Figure 1), shi demonstrates the widest niche breadth (1.563) and exhibits high vitality. Being an ancient word transmitted from antiquity to the present, shi is utilized in Cantonese, Min, and Hakka dialects, resulting in a high frequency across temporal corpora. The assessment of vocabulary niche breadth should consider two factors: richness and evenness of distribution. Ru exhibits the lowest temporal niche breadth (−0.666) due to its relatively low richness (occurring 5, 7, 7, and 3 times in T1/T2/T3/T4, respectively). Chi ranks second in temporal niche breadth (0.972) owing to its high frequency (2,542 occurrences in total). Born in the history of Middle Old Chinese, chi is a relatively new word compared to the ancient shi. In Modern Mandarin Chinese, chi has dominated the semantic field of eating, while shi has undergone degradation into a morpheme, persisting in compound words and idioms such as shi yan (食盐, ‘table salt’), mei shi (美食, ‘delicious food’), and ruorou qiang shi (弱肉强食, ‘if you are weak, you are the meat; if you are strong, you eat the meat’). This shift highlights chi’s ascendancy in modern usage, contrasting with shi’s historical prominence.

Figure 1: 
Temporal distribution of six eating verbs.
Figure 1:

Temporal distribution of six eating verbs.

4.1.2 Spatial niche breadth and vitality

In terms of spatial niche (Figure 2), chi distinguishes itself with the widest niche breadth (1.957), indicating exceptional vitality attributed to its prevalence across eight Chinese dialects. Notably, modern Cantonese encompasses not only shi in Guangzhou but also chi in Yangjiang. While shi would be traced back to Early Old Chinese, chi derives from Middle Old Chinese. In contrast, fan and can exhibit the narrowest spatial niche breadth (−0.695), stemming from their unique presence in the Guanhua area. Dan and ru rank second to last in spatial niche breadth (−0.316), representing small regional dialect words, with dan used in Minyu (Xiamen in Fujian Province, Leizhou in Zhejiang province) and Huiyu (Jixi in Anhui province), and ru in Guanhua and Wuyu.

Figure 2: 
Spatial distribution of six eating verbs.
Figure 2:

Spatial distribution of six eating verbs.

4.1.3 Functional niche breadth vitality

In terms of functional niche (Figure 3), shi and chi exhibit the widest niche breadth (0.959), both of which collocate with fourteen categories of objects: meal, cake, meat, fruit, vegetable, grass, porridge, sauce, tea, wine, water, milk, cigarette, and gas. Fan possesses the smallest functional niche breadth (−1.062), as it solely pertains to dining a specific meal, with cereal as the object. Can encompasses two categories of objects (meal and gas) with a functional niche breadth of −0.907. Ru’s objects span meat, grass, and gas, resulting in a functional niche breadth of −0.751. Dan displays greater versatility, with a functional niche breadth of 0.803, encompassing objects from all categories except meal.

Figure 3: 
Collocation (objects) of six eating verbs.
Figure 3:

Collocation (objects) of six eating verbs.

4.2 Niche overlap and competition of eating verbs

4.2.1 Temporal niche overlap and competition

In the temporal dimension (Table 5), the niche overlap indexes between shi and fan, can, dan, ru are all 1.000. This is because they are archaic words originating from ancient times and spanning the four periods of the Chinese language, from Early Old Chinese to Modern Chinese. In contrast, chi shares a temporal niche overlap index of 0.500 with fan, can, dan, and ru. This discrepancy is attributed to chi being a Middle Old Chinese verb, while the other four are Early Old Chinese verbs, resulting in a significant temporal gap between them. As shown in Table 6, shi, born in ancient times, dominates the eating domain in Early Old Chinese and Middle Old Chinese. In contrast, chi, a comparatively newer term, asserts its dominance in Late Old Chinese and Modern Chinese. The frequency of shi diminishes over time, dropping from 1,085 occurrences in corpus T1 to 63 times in T4, indicating a continual decline. Conversely, chi emerges victorious from T3 to T4, with its frequency increasing from 0 to 2003. chi initiates competition with shi after its inception, gradually establishing itself as the predominant eating verb. According to Jia and Wu (2017), by the late Tang dynasty, chi had successfully replaced shi, maintaining its dominance ever since. Archaic words such as fan, can, dan, and ru appear sporadically in the corpus throughout history, experiencing minimal competition.

Table 5:

Temporal niche overlap index of six eating verbs.

Niche overlap Shi Fan Can Dan Ru Chi
Shi /
Fan 1.000 /
Can 1.000 1.000 /
Dan 1.000 1.000 1.000 /
Ru 1.000 1.000 1.000 1.000 /
Chi 0.500 0.500 0.500 0.500 0.500 /
Table 6:

Frequency of six eating verbs.

History of Chinese Shi Fan Can Dan Ru Chi
Early Old Chinese 1,085 38 3 6 5 0
Middle Old Chinese 2,107 18 24 85 7 2
Late Old Chinese 196 10 23 19 7 537
Modern Chinese 63 6 12 21 3 2,003
Total 3,451 72 62 131 22 2,542

4.2.2 Spatial niche overlap and competition

In the spatial dimension (Table 7), the highest overlap index is observed between ru and chi (0.200). Both verbs are distributed in Guanhua and Wuyu dialects, indicating some potential competition. The overlap indexes of shi and fan, shi and can, shi and ru, fan and can, dan and can, dan and ru are all 0.000, signifying no overlapping at all and therefore no competition. This is because they belong to different dialects: shi appears in Cantonese, Minyu, and Hakka; fan and can appear in Guanhua; dan appears in Minyu (Xiamen in Fujian Province, Leizhou in Zhejiang province) and Huiyu (Jixi in Anhui province); ru appears in the Wuyu and Guanhua, as shown in Table 8.

Table 7:

Spatial niche overlap index of six eating verbs.

Niche overlap Shi Fan Can Dan Ru Chi
Shi /
Fan 0.000 /
Can 0.000 0.100 /
Dan 0.100 0.000 0.000 /
Ru 0.000 0.100 0.100 0.000 /
Chi 0.100 0.100 0.100 0.100 0.200 /
Table 8:

Dialectal distribution of six eating verbs.

Chinese dialects Shi Fan Can Dan Ru Chi
Guanhua 0 1 1 0 1 1
Jinyu 0 0 0 0 0 1
Wuyu 0 0 0 0 1 1
Huiyu 0 0 0 1 0 1
Ganyu 0 0 0 0 0 1
Xiangyu 0 0 0 0 0 1
Hakka 1 0 0 0 0 0
Minyu 1 0 0 1 0 0
Cantonese 1 0 0 0 0 1
Pinghua 0 0 0 0 0 1
Total 3 1 1 2 2 8

4.2.3 Functional niche overlap and competition

In the functional dimension (Table 9), the highest overlap index is observed between shi and chi (1.000), indicating intense potential competition. Both shi and chi belong to generalized species, encompassing objects from all 14 categories: meal, cake, meat, fruit, vegetable, grass, porridge, sauce, tea, wine, water, milk, cigarette, and gas, as shown in Table 10. Dan covers 13 categories of objects, displaying high functional niche overlap indexes with shi, and chi (0.929 in both pairs), suggesting fierce potential competition. In contrast, the functional niche overlap indexes of ru and fan, ru and can, fan and dan are all zero, signifying no overlapping and therefore no competition. Ru implies eating greedily, with objects primarily related to meat, grass, and wine. Fan refers to consuming a specific meal, primarily involving grains and food. Can is quite formal and refers to dining, with objects including meal and wind (metaphor). Dan signifies gulping, with objects mainly related to fruit and meat. Chi and shi, being relatively civilized in terms of eating, exhibit a more elegant and diverse coverage of various objects.

Table 9:

Functional niche overlap index of six eating verbs.

Niche overlap Shi Fan Can Dan Ru Chi
Shi /
Fan 0.071 /
Can 0.143 0.071 /
Dan 0.929 0.000 0.071 /
Ru 0.214 0.000 0.000 0.214 /
Chi 1.000 0.071 0.143 0.929 0.214 /
Table 10:

Collocative objects of six eating verbs.

Objects Shi Fan Can Dan Ru Chi
Meal 1 1 1 0 0 1
Cake 1 0 0 1 0 1
Meat 1 0 0 1 1 1
Fruit 1 0 0 1 0 1
Vegetable 1 0 0 1 0 1
Grass 1 0 0 1 1 1
Porridge 1 0 0 1 0 1
Sauce 1 0 0 1 0 1
Tea 1 0 0 1 0 1
Wine 1 0 0 1 1 1
Water 1 0 0 1 0 1
Milk 1 0 0 1 0 1
Cigarette 1 0 0 1 0 1
Gas 1 0 1 1 0 1
Total 14 1 2 13 3 14

5 Conclusion

Words compete, survival of the fittest. This study delves into the lexical ecology of six Chinese eating verbs, tracing their journey from ancient to modern sounds, from southern to northern accents, and from elegant to vulgar expressions. Our exploration quantifies the vitality and competition of these verbs through two key metrics: lexical niche breadth and overlap. The findings reveal a nuanced linguistic landscape: shi and chi inhabit a wide lexical niche, showcasing high vitality and sustainability. However, shi, once the dominant eating verb in ancient times, has evolved into a morpheme for compound words, displaying weakened sustainability in modern times. In contrast, chi, born in the Han dynasty and replacing shi as the dominant eating verb by the late Tang dynasty, stands as the most competitive and sustainable to date; dan and ru, occupying a narrow lexical niche, demonstrate very low vitality and sustainability. Rooted in Early Old Chinese, they contribute to the linguistic continuum with their distinct characteristics; can and fan, confined to a super narrow lexical niche, exhibit little vitality and sustainability. Born in Early Old Chinese, their limited competitive edge reflects in their diminished presence in modern linguistic contexts.

This study pioneers an ecolinguistic perspective, conducting a pilot test to measure the lexical niche and unveil the sustainability of vocabulary: a novel application within quantitative linguistics. The significance of understanding vocabulary sustainability extends to its implications for teaching and learning. It is crucial to acknowledge the limitations of this study, notably the absence of a comparative analysis with English eating verbs. In envisioning an exceptional Eco-Project of Vocabulary, visualizing the DNA of each word – tracing its birth, prototype, family, and history – holds immense potential for unraveling the intricate dynamics of linguistic evolution.


Corresponding author: Haozhang Xiao, School of Foreign Studies, 12526 South China Agricultural University , Guangzhou, China; and Institute of Foreign Linguistics and Applied Linguistics, 12526 South China Agricultural University , Guangzhou, China, E-mail:

References

Cao, Zhiyun. 2008. Hanyu fangyan dituji [Atlas of Chinese dialects]. Beijing: Commercial Press.Search in Google Scholar

Cowley, Stephen J. 2021. Ecolinguistics reunited: Rewilding the territory. Journal of World Languages 7(3). 405–427. https://doi.org/10.1515/jwl-2021-0025.Search in Google Scholar

Dai, Qingxia & Youling Deng. 2001. Binwei yuyan yanjiu zhong dingxing dingwei wenti de chubu sikao [On the classification and positioning of endangered language]. Zhongyang Minzu Daxue Xuebao (Renwen Shehui Kexue Ban) [Journal of Central University for Nationalities (Humanities and Social Sciences)] 28(2). 120–125.Search in Google Scholar

Dale, Rick & Gary Lupyan. 2012. Understanding the origins of morphological diversity: The linguistic niche hypothesis. Advances in Complex Systems 15(3). 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1142/s0219525911500172.Search in Google Scholar

Feng, Guangyi. 2013. Introduction to the ecology of language. Beijing: People’s Press.Search in Google Scholar

Fill, Alwin & Peter Mühlhäusler (eds.). 2001. The ecolinguistics reader: Language, ecology and environment. London: Continuum.Search in Google Scholar

Goatly, Andrew. 1996. Green grammar and grammatical metaphor, or language and myth of power, or metaphors we live by. Journal of Pragmatics 25(4). 537–560. https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-2166(95)00057-7.Search in Google Scholar

Greenberg, Joseph H. 1956. The measurement of linguistic diversity. Language 32(1). 109–115. https://doi.org/10.2307/410659.Search in Google Scholar

Ha, Changchen. 2023. The year’s work in ecolinguistics 2022. Journal of World Languages 9(2). 231–252. https://doi.org/10.1515/jwl-2023-0001.Search in Google Scholar

Halliday, Michael A. K. 1990. New ways of meaning: The challenge to applied linguistics. Journal of Applied Linguistics 6(13). 7–36.Search in Google Scholar

Haugen, Einar. 1970. On the ecology of languages. Paper presented at the conference Toward the Description of the Languages of the World, Burg Wartenstein, Austria, August 1–8.Search in Google Scholar

Haugen, Einar. 1972. The ecology of language. In Anwar S. Dil (ed.), The ecology of language: Essays by Einar Haugen, 325–339. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

He, Wei, Ran Gao & Jiahuan Liu. 2021. Shengtai huayu fenxi xinfazhan yanjiu [New developments of ecological discourse analysis]. Beijing: Tsinghua University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Huang, Xing. 2000. Zhongguo shaoshu minzu yuyan huoli yanjiu [On the vitality of Chinese ethnic minority languages]. Beijing: Minzu University of China Press.Search in Google Scholar

Huang, Guowen. 2018. Cong shengtai piping huayu fenxi dao hexie huayu fenxi [From ecological discourse analysis to harmonious discourse analysis]. Zhongguo Waiyu [Foreign Languages in China] 15(4). 39–46.Search in Google Scholar

Humboldt, von Wilhelm. 1836. Hongbao yuyan zhexue [Collection of Humboldt: Language philosophy]. Beijing: Commercial Press.Search in Google Scholar

Jia, Yanzi & Fuxiang Wu. 2017. Cihui leixingxue shijiaoxia hanyu chihelei dongci yanjiu [A study of Chinese eating and drinking verbs from the perspective of lexical typology]. Shijie Hanyu Jiaoxue [Chinese Teaching in the World] 31(3). 361–381.Search in Google Scholar

Labov, William. 1972. Sociolinguistic patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Search in Google Scholar

Lewis, Molly & Michael, C. Frank. 2016. Linguistic niches emerge from pressures at multiple timescales. In Proceedings of the 38th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Philadelphia, USA, August 10–13. https://langcog.stanford.edu/papers_new/lewis-2016-cogsci.pdf (accessed 4 June 2024).Search in Google Scholar

Li, Guozheng. 1991. Shengtai hanyuxue [Ecological Chinese]. Changchun: Jilin Education Press.Search in Google Scholar

Li, Rong. 2002. Xiandai hanyu fangyan dacidian [Modern Chinese dialect dictionary]. Nanjing: Jiangsu Education Press.Search in Google Scholar

Li, Rulong. 2011. Hanyu cihuixue lunji [Collection of Chinese lexicology]. Xiamen: Xiamen University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Ma, Chen & Arran Stibbe. 2022. The search for new stories to live by: A summary of ten ecolinguistics lectures delivered by Arran Stibbe. Journal of World Languages 8(1). 164–187. https://doi.org/10.1515/jwl-2021-0031.Search in Google Scholar

Mufwene, Salikoko S. 2001. The ecology of language evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511612862Search in Google Scholar

Pan, Qianying. 2015. Yingyu dui Hanyu yuyan shengtai de yingxiang [Analysis of the micro-changes of Chinese linguistic niche under the impact of English]. Waiyu Dianhua Jiaoxue [Technology Enhanced Foreign Language Education] 162(2). 66–71.Search in Google Scholar

Penz, Hermine & Alwin Fill. 2022. Ecolinguistics: History, today, and tomorrow. Journal of World Languages 8(2). 232–253. https://doi.org/10.1515/jwl-2022-0008.Search in Google Scholar

Schleicher, August. 1863. Darwinism tested by the science of language. Dialect 14. 373–383.Search in Google Scholar

Stibbe, Arran. 2020. Ecolinguistics: Language, ecology and the stories we live by, 2nd edn. London: Routledge.10.4324/9780367855512Search in Google Scholar

Voegelin, Carl F., Florence M. Voegelin & Noel W. SchutzJr. 1967. The language situation in Arizona as part of the Southwest culture area. In Dell H. Hymes & William E. Bittle (eds.), Studies in southwestern ethnolinguistics, 403–451. Hague: Mouton.Search in Google Scholar

Wang, Futang. 1995. Hanyu fangyan cihui [Chinese dialect vocabulary]. Beijing: Chinese Press.Search in Google Scholar

Wang, Hongyan. 2007. On the endangered languages from the perspective of linguistic niche. Wuhan: South-Central Minzu University MA thesis.Search in Google Scholar

Wang, Min. 2014. The niche of dialects under ecological civilization. Jinhua: Zhejiang Normal University MA thesis.Search in Google Scholar

Xiao, Haozhang. 2021. Shengtai yuyanxue lianxutong linian [Ecolinguistic continuum and the multi-dimensional alignment continuum model]. Waiyu Jiaoxue yu Yanjiu [Foreign Language Teaching and Research] 53(4). 483–495.Search in Google Scholar

Xiao, Zihui & Junjun Fan. 2011. Yuyan shengtai de jiance yu pinggu zhibiao tixi [Monitoring and evaluation index system of language ecology]. Yuyan Kexue [Language Science] 10(3). 270–280.Search in Google Scholar

Xiao, Haozhang, Weiwei Zhang & Ruifeng Mo. 2023. A multidimensional alignment sustainability model for language development. Language Sciences 100. 101572. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2023.101572.Search in Google Scholar

Xiong, Zhenghui & Zhenxing Zhang. 2012. Zhongguo yuyan dituji [Language atlas of China]. Beijing: Commercial Press.Search in Google Scholar

Xu, Baohua & Yilang Gongtian. 2020. Hanyu fangyan cidian [Chinese dialect dictionary]. Beijing: Chinese Publishing House.Search in Google Scholar

Zhang, Lan & Guowen Huang. 2021. Quantitative research methods of linguistic niche and cultural sustainability. Sustainability 13(17). 110157212. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13179586.Search in Google Scholar

Zhang, Xianliang & Ying Wei. 2017. Cong yuyan shengtaiwei shijiao kan Hanyu jingyu [A study on Chinese honorifics from the perspective of linguistic niche]. Guangxi Minzu Daxue Xuebao (Zhexue Shehui Kexue Ban) [Journal of Guangxi Minzu University (Philosophy and Social Science Edition)] 39(5). 155–160.Search in Google Scholar

Zhou, Wenjuan. 2021. Ecolinguistics: A half-century overview. Journal of World Languages 7(3). 461–486. https://doi.org/10.1515/jwl-2021-0022.Search in Google Scholar

Received: 2023-03-01
Accepted: 2024-06-05
Published Online: 2024-07-01
Published in Print: 2024-12-17

© 2024 the author(s), published by De Gruyter and FLTRP on behalf of BFSU

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Downloaded on 17.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/jwl-2024-0012/html
Scroll to top button