Home What is lost when a language dies?
Article
Licensed
Unlicensed Requires Authentication

What is lost when a language dies?

  • Anders Søgaard

    Anders Søgaard is a Professor of Natural Language Processing and Machine Learning and runs the Center for Philosophy of Artificial Intelligence at the University of Copenhagen. He has published in top venues across scientific disciplines and won several prestigious grants and best paper awards – including an ERC Starting Grant, a Google Focused Research Award, and a Carlsberg Semper Ardens Advance.

    EMAIL logo
Published/Copyright: March 29, 2024
Become an author with De Gruyter Brill

Abstract

Nowak argues that the problem with language loss is not linguists’ loss of data or that the loss of a language is often a result of discrimination against its speakers. Instead, the real problem is its speakers’ loss of illocutionary force. I argue that Nowak’s argument rests on two premises that are both empirically unjustified: that cultural knowledge is a prerequisite for illocutionary force, and that language is a prerequisite for illocutionary force. Languages are among the most fascinating accomplishments of mankind, surpassing Machu Picchu and the Great Wall of China in the eyes of many. I think language loss is comparable to loss of species. The intuition that the death of a language is a significant event, reflects that: Something that evolved gradually over hundreds of years, passed on through hundreds of generations and thousands of individual speakers, is irreversibly gone, once and for all. The illocutionary force of its individual speakers is not.


Corresponding author: Anders Søgaard, Center for Philosophy of Artificial Intelligence, University of Copenhagen, Copenhagen, Denmark, E-mail:

About the author

Anders Søgaard

Anders Søgaard is a Professor of Natural Language Processing and Machine Learning and runs the Center for Philosophy of Artificial Intelligence at the University of Copenhagen. He has published in top venues across scientific disciplines and won several prestigious grants and best paper awards – including an ERC Starting Grant, a Google Focused Research Award, and a Carlsberg Semper Ardens Advance.

  1. Competing interests: The author did not receive support from any organization for the submitted work. The author has no competing interests to declare that are relevant to the content of this article.

References

Ambady, Nalini, Jasook Koo, Fiona Lee & Robert Rosenthal. 1996. More than words: Linguistic and nonlinguistic politeness in two cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 70(5). 996–1011. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.70.5.996.Search in Google Scholar

Austin, James L. 1962. How to do things with words. William James Lectures. Oxford, UK. Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Bara, Bruno, Ilaria Cutica & Maurizio Tirassa. 2001. Neuropragmatics: Extralinguistic communication after closed head injury. Brain and Language 77(1). 72–94. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.2000.2430.Search in Google Scholar

Bosco, Francesca, Katiuska Sacco, Livia Colle, Romina Angleri, Ivan Enrici, Gianluca Bo & Bruno Bara. 2004. Simple and complex extralinguistic communicative acts. In Proceedings of the twenty-sixth annual conference of the cognitive science. Chicago, Illinois: Cognitive Science Society, Inc.Search in Google Scholar

De Marco, Ivan, Livia Colle & Monica Bucciarelli. 2007. Linguistic and extralinguistic communication in deaf children. Journal of Pragmatics 39(1). 134–156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2006.08.009.Search in Google Scholar

Dresner, Eli & Susan C. Herring. 2010. Functions of the nonverbal in CMC: Emoticons and illocutionary force. Communication Theory 20(3). 249–268. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.2010.01362.x.Search in Google Scholar

Meneses, Celina Eladia. 2018. Code-switching as a politeness marker in Sangguniang Panlalawigan regular sessions in Albay. Bicol University R&D Journal 21(3). 1–11.Search in Google Scholar

Nowak, Ethan. 2020. Language loss and illocutionary silencing. Mind 129(515). 831–865. https://doi.org/10.1093/mind/fzz051.Search in Google Scholar

Rendall, Drew. 2021. Aping language: Historical perspectives on the quest for semantics, syntax, and other rarefied properties of human language in the communication of primates and other animals. Frontiers in Psychology 12. 675172. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.675172.Search in Google Scholar

Searle, John R. 1975. A taxonomy of illocutionary acts. In Keith Gunderson (ed.), Language, mind and knowledge, 344–369. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.Search in Google Scholar

Searle, John R. & Daniel Vanderveken. 1985. Foundations of illocutionary logic. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.10.1007/1-4020-3167-X_5Search in Google Scholar

Tomasello, Michael. 2015. The usage-based theory of language acquisition. In Cambridge handbooks in language and linguistics, 2nd edn., 89–106. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781316095829.005Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2024-03-29
Published in Print: 2024-04-25

© 2024 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 18.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/ip-2024-2004/html
Scroll to top button