Abstract
This study investigates the productive use of semantic and phonetic radicals for Chinese character decoding by Chinese as a foreign language (CFL) learners at different levels of L2 proficiency, focusing on the application of radical knowledge according to the learners’ L1 language families. Using a pseudo-word test developed by Williams, Clay. 2014. The development of intra-character radical awareness in L1 Chinese children: Changing strategies. Journal of the Chinese Language Teachers Association 49(2). 1–26 to measure subject preference for semantic versus phonological decoding processes, subjects are asked to match a provided definition and pronunciation with a character in a multiple choice format which included two pseudo characters designed with radicals which corresponded with the provided definition or pronunciation, respectively. The results demonstrate that reliance on semantic or phonological radicals for character identification varies according to the L1 of the CFL learners; subjects whose L1 is relatively orthographically transparent predominantly make use of phonological processing strategies at all levels of proficiency, whereas those whose L1 are more orthographically opaque demonstrate more malleable processing preferences, with relatively strong semantic radical reliance in the early stage of their language learning, and considerable variability between semantic and phonological processing at intermediate and advanced proficiency levels. The findings suggest that developmental trends of using radical decoding strategies differ among CFL learner groups with varying L1 literacy strategy preferences.
References
Anderson, Richard C, Yu-Min Ku, Wenling Li, Xi Chen, Xinchun Wu & Hua Shu. 2013. Learning to see the patterns in Chinese characters. Scientific Studies of Reading 17(1). 41–56. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2012.68978910.1080/10888438.2012.689789.Search in Google Scholar
Bi, Yanchao, Zaizhu Han, Brendan Weekes & Hua Shu. 2007. The interaction between semantic and the nonsemantic systems in reading: Evidence from Chinese. Neuropsychologia 45(12). 2660–2673. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.00710.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.007.Search in Google Scholar
Cho, Jeung-Ryeul & Hsuan-Chih Chen. 1999. Orthographic and phonological activation in the semantic processing of Korean hanja and hangul. Language & Cognitive Processes 14(5–6). 481–502. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690969938616710.1080/016909699386167.Search in Google Scholar
Coltheart, Max. 1980. Reading phonological recoding and deep dyslexia. In Max Coltheart, Karalyn Patterson & John, C. Marshall (eds.), Deep dyslexia, 197–226. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Search in Google Scholar
Coltheart, Max. 2005. Modeling reading: The dual-route approach. In Margaret J Snowling & Charles Hulme (eds.), The science of reading: A handbook, 6–23. Oxford: Blackwell.10.1002/9780470757642.ch1Search in Google Scholar
Flores d’Arcais & Giovanni, B. 1992. Graphemic, phonological and semantic activation processes during the recognition of Chinese characters. In Hsuan-Chih Chen & Ovid J. L. Tzeng (eds.), Language processing in Chinese, 37–66. Amsterdam: Elsevier.10.1016/S0166-4115(08)61886-5Search in Google Scholar
Feldman, Laurie B. & Witina W. T. Siok. 1999. Semantic radicals in phonetic compounds: Implications for visual character recognition in Chinese. In Jian Wang, Hsuan-Chih Chen, Ralph Radach & Albrecht, Inhoff (eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis, 19–35. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Search in Google Scholar
Hoosain, Rumjahn. 1991. Psycholinguistic implications for linguistic relativity: A case study of Chinese. Hillsdale, N.J: L. Erlbaum Associates.Search in Google Scholar
Katz, Leonard & Laurie Feldman. 1983. Relation between pronunciation and recognition of printed words in deep and shallow orthographies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 9(1). 157. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.9.1.15710.1037/0278-7393.9.1.157.Search in Google Scholar
Li, Hong, Hua Shu, Catherine McBride‐Chang, Hongyun Liu & Hong Peng. 2012. Chinese children’s character recognition: Visuo‐orthographic, phonological processing and morphological skills. Journal of Research in Reading 35(3). 287–307. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01460.x10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01460.x.Search in Google Scholar
Patterson, Karalyn Eve & John Morton. 1985. From orthography to phonology: An attempt at an old interpretation. In K. E. Patterson, J.C. Marshall & M. Coltheart (eds.), Surface dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading, 335–359. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.10.4324/9781315108346-20Search in Google Scholar
Perfetti, Charles & Li Tan. 1998. The time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic activation in Chinese character identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 24(1). 101. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.24.1.10110.1037/0278-7393.24.1.101.Search in Google Scholar
Scholfield, Phil & Gloria Shu-Mei, Chwo. 2005. Are the l1 and l2 word reading processes affected more by writing system or instruction? In Vivian Cook & Benedetta Bassetti (eds.), Second language writing systems, 215–237. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters Ltd.10.21832/9781853597954-010Search in Google Scholar
Shao, Grace. 2015. Chinese as a second language growing in popularity. https://america.cgtn.com/2015/03/03/chinese-as-a-second-language-growing-in-popularity (accessed 3 March 2015).Search in Google Scholar
Shen, Helen H. & Chuanren, Ke. 2007. Radical awareness and word acquisition among nonnative learners of Chinese. The Modern Language Journal 91(1). 97–111. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.2007.00511.x10.1111/j.1540-4781.2007.00511.x.Search in Google Scholar
Shen, Di & Kenneth I. Forster. 1999. Masked phonological priming in reading Chinese words depends on the task. Language & Cognitive Processes 14(5–6). 429–459. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690969938614910.1080/016909699386149.Search in Google Scholar
Shu, Hua & Richard Anderson. 1997. Role of radical awareness in the character and word acquisition of Chinese children. Reading Research Quarterly 32(1). 78. https://doi.org/10.1598/rrq.32.1.510.1598/rrq.32.1.5.Search in Google Scholar
Shu, Hua & Richard C. Anderson. 1999. Semantic radicals in phonetic compounds: Implications for visual character recognition in Chinese. In Jian Wang, Hsuan-Chih Chen, Ralph Radach & Albrecht, Inhoff (eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis, 1–18. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Search in Google Scholar
Tan, Li, Rumjahn Hoosain & Witina Siok. 1996. Activation of phonological codes before access to character meaning in written Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 22(4). 865. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.4.86510.1037/0278-7393.22.4.865.Search in Google Scholar
Tong, Xiuhong, Xiuli Tong & Catherine McBride. 2017. Radical sensitivity is the key to understanding Chinese character acquisition in children. Reading and Writing 30(6). 1251–1265. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-017-9722-810.1007/s11145-017-9722-8.Search in Google Scholar
Wang, Min, Ying Liu & Charles A. Perfetti. 2004. The implicit and explicit learning of orthographic structure and function of a new writing system. Scientific Studies of Reading 8(4). 357–379. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0804_310.1207/s1532799xssr0804_3.Search in Google Scholar
Weekes, Brendan S., May Jane Chen & Yin Wen Gang. 1997. Anomia without dyslexia in Chinese. Neurocase 3(1). 51–60. https://doi.org/10.1080/1355479970840403410.1080/13554799708404034.Search in Google Scholar
Williams, Clay. 2013. Emerging development of semantic and phonological routes to character decoding in Chinese as a foreign language learner. Reading and Writing 26(2). 293–315. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9368-510.1007/s11145-012-9368-5.Search in Google Scholar
Williams, Clay. 2014. The development of intra-character radical awareness in L1 Chinese children: Changing strategies. Journal of the Chinese Language Teachers Association 49(2). 1–26.Search in Google Scholar
Williams, Clay. 2016. L1 literacy strategy impact on l2 word-priming effects. Thought 1. 44–57.Search in Google Scholar
Williams, Clay & Thomas Bever. 2010. Chinese character decoding: A semantic bias? Reading and Writing 23(5). 589–605. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9228-010.1007/s11145-010-9228-0.Search in Google Scholar
Wong, Yu Ka. 2017. The role of radical awareness in Chinese-as-a-second-language learners’ Chinese character reading development. Language Awareness 26(3). 211–225. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658416.2017.140003910.1080/09658416.2017.1400039.Search in Google Scholar
Wu, J. T. & I. M. Liu. 1997. Phonological activation in pronouncing characters. In Hsuan-Chih Chen (eds.), The cognitive processing of Chinese and related Asian languages, 47–64. Hong Kong: Chinese University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Zhang, Jie, Hong, Li, Qiong Dong, Jie Xu & Elizabeth Sholar. 2016. Implicit use of radicals in learning characters for nonnative learners of Chinese. Applied PsychoLinguistics 37(3). 507–527. https://doi.org/10.1017/s014271641500009010.1017/s0142716415000090.Search in Google Scholar
Zhou, Xiaolin & William Marslen-Wilson. 2000. The relative time course of semantic and phonological activation in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 26(5). 1245. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.124510.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1245.Search in Google Scholar
© 2020 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston
Articles in the same Issue
- Frontmatter
- Special section: Chinese Language Education Research in the Asia Pacific Contexts; Guest editor: Danping Wang
- Introduction to special issue
- Motivational strategies in Chinese language classrooms: A comparative study of novice and experienced teachers
- Interacting with native speakers of Chinese through online learning communities: A case study with East Asian learners
- Chinese vocabulary learning strategies by English-speaking learners: A think-aloud study
- Localising Chinese language curriculum construction: A case study in an Australian primary school
- Learning Mandarin tones through pitch-time diagrams: A computer-assisted visual approach
- Special section: Developing Chinese Language Skills and Intercultural Competence; Guest editor: Irene Shidong An
- Differentiation of radical processing strategies in character decoding by L2 learners of Chinese according to L1-type
- Teachers’ perceptions of effective strategies for developing intercultural competence
- Introducing Chinese Hanzi at the beginning of teaching Chinese as a Foreign Language (CFL)
- 影响少数民族学生阅读理解能力的语言及认知因子研究———以藏族和彝族小学生为例
Articles in the same Issue
- Frontmatter
- Special section: Chinese Language Education Research in the Asia Pacific Contexts; Guest editor: Danping Wang
- Introduction to special issue
- Motivational strategies in Chinese language classrooms: A comparative study of novice and experienced teachers
- Interacting with native speakers of Chinese through online learning communities: A case study with East Asian learners
- Chinese vocabulary learning strategies by English-speaking learners: A think-aloud study
- Localising Chinese language curriculum construction: A case study in an Australian primary school
- Learning Mandarin tones through pitch-time diagrams: A computer-assisted visual approach
- Special section: Developing Chinese Language Skills and Intercultural Competence; Guest editor: Irene Shidong An
- Differentiation of radical processing strategies in character decoding by L2 learners of Chinese according to L1-type
- Teachers’ perceptions of effective strategies for developing intercultural competence
- Introducing Chinese Hanzi at the beginning of teaching Chinese as a Foreign Language (CFL)
- 影响少数民族学生阅读理解能力的语言及认知因子研究———以藏族和彝族小学生为例