Home The ‘curse of knowledge’: when medical expertise can sometimes be a liability
Article Open Access

The ‘curse of knowledge’: when medical expertise can sometimes be a liability

  • Narinder Kapur EMAIL logo
Published/Copyright: April 29, 2024

Medical expertise is associated with excellent patient care and good clinical decision making. While this generally remains true, there is an increasing body of evidence to suggest that in some cases medical expertise can be a liability, with errors more likely to occur in medical experts than in junior staff. Increased awareness of this ‘curse of knowledge’ may result in better patient safety.

To the Editor,

Good clinical decision-making is key to patient care and patient safety [1]. Medical experts, whether they be doctors, nurses, or other healthcare professionals, build up knowledge and experience over the years, and this usually leads to good clinical decisions and other areas of care being of a high standard. Along with building up knowledge comes the formation of abstract concepts and schematic representation of facts and ideas. The benefits of such ‘schemas’ are that they result in faster processing of information and thus faster response in problem-solving settings. All of this is good, and generally leads to efficient decision-making processes. However, schemas are by their nature likely to result in associations being generated as a result of the processing of information. The stronger and more substantive the schema, the more likely that an increasing number of associations will be generated from a stimulus or set of stimuli. This may then result in more diverse and rarer associations being generated. This flip side of a well-established knowledge schema can then lead to certain forms of cognitive errors being more likely to occur, and several studies have found this to be the case.

In a study where expert clinicians were compared with undergraduate students, three clinical vignettes of patients with psychological disorders were presented for retention [2]. The three vignettes were a ‘simple case’, a ‘complex-coherent case’ which had a plausible set of symptoms, and what was termed a ‘complex-incoherent case’, where there was an unlikely set of symptoms. Whilst, after a short delay, experts correctly recalled more details from the vignettes than nonexperts, they also produced more recall of symptoms that had not been presented in the vignettes, what psychologists call ‘false positive responses’ or ‘false alarms’. In addition, while nonexperts produced similar numbers of such false alarms to the three types of clinical vignette, the experts produced fewest false alarms in the case of the complex-coherent vignettes, and the most false alarms to the complex-incoherent vignettes. It was as if the latter type of vignette was forcing the schematic knowledge of the experts to generate more associations.

A similar form of ‘schema error’ was found in a study that compared those undergoing training in diagnostic decision making relating to anxiety disorders, compared to those who were not receiving such training [3]. Those undergoing training, when presented with clinical vignettes of anxiety that they had to remember, but where two common diagnostic features (‘restlessness’, ‘uncontrollable worry’) were missing from the vignette, were more likely than their untrained counterparts to falsely recall that these features were present in the clinical vignettes.

A further study [4] compared experienced clinicians and clinical trainees in their recognition memory for true and false items derived from clinical vignettes they had been asked to remember. These vignettes contained contrasting features – traumatic v non-traumatic event, mild or intense reactions of patients, and subsequent behaviours that were either mild or severe. Researchers found that expert clinicians had more difficulty in distinguishing true from false items than the clinical trainees, and number of years’ experience in the experts was in fact negatively correlated with accuracy of recognition – more experienced experts were more likely to make such errors.

More generally, paradoxes in the performance of experts have been well documented by researchers [5, 6], and individuals with superior autobiographical memory have been found to be just as likely as others to make false memory errors [7]. In addition, the ‘curse of knowledge’ has been linked with being highly specialised and having limitations in integrating and communicating with others, and with understanding their perspective [8], [9], [10]. It would appear that judgement and memory errors in experts may be due to a variety of reasons – the vast, somewhat entrenched schema that has built up over the years that I have referred to above, with such knowledge being a double-edged sword [11]; the fact that such experts with many years of experience will have seen many rare symptoms and diagnoses which are then more likely than in their junior counterparts to be activated in clinical decision settings; the fact that they may suffer from a lack of inhibition in their judgements, since experienced clinicians tend to be older, and ageing is associated with decreased frontal lobe functioning in domains such as inhibition; and the fact that experts may sometimes be more over-confident, and have a greater willingness to break rules, which then results in them being more likely to offer erroneous responses [12]. Greater awareness of the fallibility associated with medical expertise may result in fewer clinical errors being made, and patient safety will of course be the key beneficiary.

In conclusion, we value and benefit from senior clinicians who have many years of experience behind them. In most cases, they are best placed to make accurate diagnoses, but we need to be cognizant of the fact that immense knowledge can on occasion have its downside. Acknowledgement of this may lead to fewer errors in patient care.


Corresponding author: Professor Narinder Kapur, Research Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, University College London, 1-19 Torrington Place, London, WC1E 7HB, UK, E-mail:

  1. Research ethics: Not applicable.

  2. Informed consent: Not applicable.

  3. Author contributions: The author has accepted responsibility for the entire content of this manuscript and approved its submission.

  4. Competing interests: The author states no conflict of interest.

  5. Research funding: None declared.

  6. Data availability: Not applicable.

References

1. Croskerry, P. Cognitive autopsy. A root cause analysis of medical decision making. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2020.10.1093/med/9780190088743.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

2. Webb, C, Keeley, J, Eakin, D. Are clinicians better than lay judges at recalling case details? An evaluation of expert memory. J Clin Psychol 2023;72:384–400. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22256.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

3. Foster, R, Webb, C, Keeley, J, Eakin, D. An investigation of training, schemas, and false recall of diagnostic features of mental disorders. Train Educ Prof Psychol 2023;11:174–81. https://doi.org/10.1037/tep0000145.Search in Google Scholar

4. Weine, E, Kim, N. Systematic distortions in clinicians’ memories for client cases: increasing causal coherence. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 2019;45:196–212. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000589.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

5. Dror, I. The paradox of human expertise: why experts get it wrong. In: Kapur, N, editor. The paradoxical brain. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2011:177–88 pp.10.1017/CBO9780511978098.011Search in Google Scholar

6. Boutin, K-D, Davis, C, Hevner, A, Leger, P, Labonte-LeMoyne, E. Don’t overthink it: the paradoxical nature of expertise for the detection of errors in conceptual business process models. Front Neurosci 2022;16:982764. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2022.982764.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

7. Patihis, L, Frenda, S, LePort, A, Peterson, N, Nichols, R, Stark, CE, et al.. False memories in highly superior autobiographical memory individuals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2013;110:20947–52. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1314373110.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

8. Lourenco, A, Baird, G. Optimising radiology reports for patients and referring physicians: mitigating the curse of knowledge. Acad Radiol 2020;27:436–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acra.2019.03.026.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

9. Rosenbaum, L. Cursed by knowledge – building a culture of psychological safety. N Engl J Med 2019;380:786–90. https://doi.org/10.1056/nejmms1813429.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

10. Tullis, J, Feder, B. The ‘curse of knowledge’ when predicting others’ knowledge. Mem Cognit 2023;51:1214–34. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-022-01382-3.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

11. Chen, X, Varghese, L, Jagust, W. A double-edged sword: the role of prior knowledge in memory aging. Front Aging Neurosci 2022;14. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2022.874767.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

12. Hotlman, M. Paradoxes of professionalism and error in complex systems. J Biomed Inf 2011;44:395–401. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbi.2009.08.002.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Received: 2024-04-08
Accepted: 2024-04-09
Published Online: 2024-04-29

© 2024 the author(s), published by De Gruyter, Berlin/Boston

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Articles in the same Issue

  1. Frontmatter
  2. Editorial
  3. Should APTT become part of thrombophilia screening?
  4. Review
  5. n-3 fatty acids and the risk of atrial fibrillation, review
  6. Guidelines and Recommendations
  7. Root cause analysis of cases involving diagnosis
  8. Opinion Papers
  9. What is diagnostic safety? A review of safety science paradigms and rethinking paths to improving diagnosis
  10. Interprofessional clinical reasoning education
  11. Original Articles
  12. Quality of heart failure registration in primary care: observations from 1 million electronic health records in the Amsterdam Metropolitan Area
  13. Typology of solutions addressing diagnostic disparities: gaps and opportunities
  14. Diagnostic errors and characteristics of patients seen at a general internal medicine outpatient clinic with a referral for diagnosis
  15. Cost-benefit considerations of the biased diagnostician
  16. Delayed diagnosis of new onset pediatric diabetes leading to diabetic ketoacidosis: a retrospective cohort study
  17. Monocyte distribution width (MDW) kinetic for monitoring sepsis in intensive care unit
  18. Are shortened aPTT values always to be attributed only to preanalytical problems?
  19. External Quality Assessment (EQA) scheme for serological diagnostic test for SARS-CoV-2 detection in Sicily Region (Italy), in the period 2020–2022
  20. Recent mortality rates due to complications of medical and surgical care in the US
  21. Short Communication
  22. The potential, limitations, and future of diagnostics enhanced by generative artificial intelligence
  23. Case Report – Lessons in Clinical Reasoning
  24. Lessons in clinical reasoning – pitfalls, myths, and pearls: a case of persistent dysphagia and patient partnership
  25. Letters to the Editor
  26. The ‘curse of knowledge’: when medical expertise can sometimes be a liability
  27. A new approach for identifying innate immune defects
Downloaded on 13.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/dx-2024-0064/html
Scroll to top button