Startseite Identification of facilitators and barriers to residents’ use of a clinical reasoning tool
Artikel
Lizenziert
Nicht lizenziert Erfordert eine Authentifizierung

Identification of facilitators and barriers to residents’ use of a clinical reasoning tool

  • Deborah DiNardo EMAIL logo , Sarah Tilstra , Melissa McNeil , William Follansbee , Shanta Zimmer , Coreen Farris und Amber E. Barnato
Veröffentlicht/Copyright: 9. Februar 2018
Diagnosis
Aus der Zeitschrift Diagnosis Band 5 Heft 1

Abstract

Background:

While there is some experimental evidence to support the use of cognitive forcing strategies to reduce diagnostic error in residents, the potential usability of such strategies in the clinical setting has not been explored. We sought to test the effect of a clinical reasoning tool on diagnostic accuracy and to obtain feedback on its usability and acceptability.

Methods:

We conducted a randomized behavioral experiment testing the effect of this tool on diagnostic accuracy on written cases among post-graduate 3 (PGY-3) residents at a single internal medical residency program in 2014. Residents completed written clinical cases in a proctored setting with and without prompts to use the tool. The tool encouraged reflection on concordant and discordant aspects of each case. We used random effects regression to assess the effect of the tool on diagnostic accuracy of the independent case sets, controlling for case complexity. We then conducted audiotaped structured focus group debriefing sessions and reviewed the tapes for facilitators and barriers to use of the tool.

Results:

Of 51 eligible PGY-3 residents, 34 (67%) participated in the study. The average diagnostic accuracy increased from 52% to 60% with the tool, a difference that just met the test for statistical significance in adjusted analyses (p=0.05). Residents reported that the tool was generally acceptable and understandable but did not recognize its utility for use with simple cases, suggesting the presence of overconfidence bias.

Conclusions:

A clinical reasoning tool improved residents’ diagnostic accuracy on written cases. Overconfidence bias is a potential barrier to its use in the clinical setting.


Corresponding author: Deborah DiNardo, MD, MS, Department of Medicine, VA Pittsburgh Healthcare System, University Drive C, Pittsburgh, PA 15240, USA, Phone: +412-360-1709

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Silvia Mamede, MD, PhD, and Henk Schmidt, PhD, at Erasmus University, Rotterdam Netherlands for sharing clinical cases, consulting on the design of the experimental test booklets and providing constructive feedback on the manuscript.

  1. Author contributions: All the authors have accepted responsibility for the entire content of this submitted manuscript and approved submission.

  2. Research funding: This project was funded by a grant from the Hearst Foundations awarded to Dr. William Follansbee.

  3. Employment or leadership: None declared.

  4. Honorarium: None declared.

  5. Competing interests: The funding organization(s) played no role in the study design; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the report for publication.

References

1. Graber ML, Franklin N, Gordon R. Diagnostic error in internal medicine. Arch Intern Med 2005;165:1493–9.10.1001/archinte.165.13.1493Suche in Google Scholar PubMed

2. National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2015. Improving Diagnosis in Health Care. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. https://doi.org/10.17226/21794.10.17226/21794Suche in Google Scholar PubMed

3. Croskerry P, Singhal G, Mamede S. Cognitive debiasing 1: origins of bias and theory of debiasing. BMJ Qual Saf 2013;22:ii58–64.10.1136/bmjqs-2012-001712Suche in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

4. Lambe KA, O’Reilly G, Kelly BD, Curristan S. Dual-process cognitive interventions to enhance diagnostic reasoning: a systematic review. BMJ Qual Saf 2016;25:808–20.10.1136/bmjqs-2015-004417Suche in Google Scholar PubMed

5. Mamede S, Schmidt HG, Penaforte JC. Effects of reflective practice on the accuracy of medical diagnoses. Med Educ 2008;42:468–75.10.1111/j.1365-2923.2008.03030.xSuche in Google Scholar PubMed

6. Mamede S, van Gog T, van den Berge K, Rikers RM, van Saase JL, van Guldener C, et al. Effect of availability bias and reflective reasoning on diagnostic accuracy among internal medicine residents. J Am Med Assoc 2010;304:1198–203.10.1001/jama.2010.1276Suche in Google Scholar PubMed

7. Berner ES, Graber ML. Overconfidence as a cause of diagnostic error in medicine. Am J Med 2008;121:S2–23.10.1016/j.amjmed.2008.01.001Suche in Google Scholar PubMed

8. Meyer AN, Payne VL, Meeks DW, Rao R, Singh H. Physicians’ diagnostic accuracy, confidence, and resource requests: a vignette study. JAMA Intern Med 2013;173:1952–8.10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.10081Suche in Google Scholar PubMed

9. Zwaan L, Monteiro S, Sherbino J, Ilgen J, Howey B, Norman G. Is bias in the eye of the beholder? A vignette study to assess recognition of cognitive biases in clinical case workups. BMJ Qual Saf 2017;26:104–10.10.1136/bmjqs-2015-005014Suche in Google Scholar PubMed

10. Croskerry P, Singhal G, Mamede S. Cognitive debiasing 2: impediments to and strategies for change. BMJ Qual Saf 2013;22(Suppl 2)ii:65–72.10.1136/bmjqs-2012-001713Suche in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

11. Kapur N, Parand A, Soukup T, Reader T, Sevdalis N. Aviation and healthcare: a comparative review with implications for patient safety. JRSM Open 2016;7:2054270415616548.10.1177/2054270415616548Suche in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

12. Gandhi TK, Kachalia A, Thomas EJ, Puopolo AL, Yoon C, Brennan TA, et al. Missed and delayed diagnoses in the ambulatory setting: a study of closed malpractice claims. Ann Intern Med 2006;145:488–96.10.7326/0003-4819-145-7-200610030-00006Suche in Google Scholar PubMed

13. Kachalia A, Gandhi TK, Puopolo AL, Yoon C, Thomas EJ, Griffey R, et al. Missed and delayed diagnoses in the emergency department: a study of closed malpractice claims from 4 liability insurers. Ann Emerg Med 2007;49: 196–205.10.1016/j.annemergmed.2006.06.035Suche in Google Scholar PubMed

14. Jagsi R, Kitch BT, Weinstein DF, Campbell EG, Hutter M, Weissman JS. Residents report on adverse events and their causes. Arch Intern Med 2005;165:2607–13.10.1001/archinte.165.22.2607Suche in Google Scholar PubMed

15. Tversky A, Kahneman D. Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science 1974;185:1124–31.10.21236/AD0767426Suche in Google Scholar

16. Schmidt HG, Rikers RM. How expertise develops in medicine: knowledge encapsulation and illness script formation. Med Educ 2007;41:1133–9.10.1111/j.1365-2923.2007.02915.xSuche in Google Scholar PubMed

17. Monteiro SD, Sherbino J, Patel A, Mazzetti I, Norman GR, Howey E. Reflecting on diagnostic errors: taking a second look is not enough. J Gen Intern Med 2015;30:1270–4.10.1007/s11606-015-3369-4Suche in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

18. Sherbino J, Dore KL, Wood TJ, Young ME, Gaissmaier W, Kreuger S, et al. The relationship between response time and diagnostic accuracy. Acad Med 2012;87:785–91.10.1097/ACM.0b013e318253acbdSuche in Google Scholar PubMed

19. Norman G, Sherbino J, Dore K, Wood T, Young M, Gaissmaier W, et al. The etiology of diagnostic errors: a controlled trial of system 1 versus system 2 reasoning. Acad Med 2014;89:277–84.10.1097/ACM.0000000000000105Suche in Google Scholar PubMed


Article note:

Prior presentations: This work was presented previously in the following venues

  • Society for General Internal Medicine, Oral Abstract Presentation, April 2015.

  • Society to Improve Diagnosis in Medicine, Poster Presentation, September 2015.


Received: 2017-10-16
Accepted: 2018-1-11
Published Online: 2018-2-9
Published in Print: 2018-3-28

©2018 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Heruntergeladen am 18.9.2025 von https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/dx-2017-0037/html
Button zum nach oben scrollen