Home English Vocabulary Acquisition Through Songs in Chinese Kindergarten Students
Article Publicly Available

English Vocabulary Acquisition Through Songs in Chinese Kindergarten Students

  • Glenn M. Davis

    Glenn M. Davis is a lecturer in the Department of Foreign Languages and Literatures at Tsinghua University in Beijing, China. His research focuses on quantitative measurement of educational outcomes in second and foreign language learning

    EMAIL logo
    and Wenfang Fan

    Wenfang Fan is a professor in the Department of Foreign Languages and Literatures at Tsinghua University in Beijing, China. Her research focuses on functional linguistics and teaching English as a foreign language from grade schools up to tertiary level

Published/Copyright: March 19, 2016
Become an author with De Gruyter Brill

Abstract

This paper reports an empirical study that examined the widespread practice of using songs in language teaching for young learners. The study may represent the first methodologically rigorous assessment of vocabulary acquisition through songs as used in language teaching. Over a seven-week period including fifteen 40-minute classes, three groups of students from two private kindergartens in Beijing were taught five short English phrases of 4-8 words through each of three conditions (songs, choral repetition, control) in a within-subject repeated measures design. Vocabulary acquisition was measured by the number of meaningful morphemes produced by the students in a picture description task administered before and after the teaching period. Results indicated significant acquisition for items learned through songs and choral repetition, but not for control items. The implication is that songs may indeed contain ünportant pedagogical value.

Songs have a long history of recommendation for use in the foreign language classroom. Richards (1969) discussed the benefits of songs for teaching vocabulary, rhythm, sentence patterns, and pronunciation of speech sounds, while cautioning teachers to avoid songs with irregulär or archaic vocabulary, structure, or stress. From a similar pedagogical perspective, Jolly (1975) claimed that songs could break up the boredom associated with the constant repetition that was a feature of the then-dominant audiolingual method, and were thus useful motivational tools that maintained the benefits of drilling phrases whilealso teaching students about the target culture.

More recently, songs have been recommended for use in foreign language classrooms by a variety of authors for different areas such as teaching rhythm (Tuan & An, 2010), listening skills (§evik, 2012), grammar (Saricoban & Metin, 2000), conversation skills (Orlova, 2003), and literacy skills (Paquette & Rieg, 2008), as well as purported benefits in increasing learner involvement (Lo & Li, 1998) and teaching students about the target culture (Keskin, 2011). Surveys of teachers and students have revealed highly positive attitudes toward the use of songs in language classrooms (Chou, 2014; Jarvis, 2013), and several authors have discussed how best to design activities around songs in order to most effectively achieve the above aims (e.g., Claerr & Gargan, 1984; Lorenzutti, 2014; Millington, 2011, Murphey, 1992).

Songs have been reported to be in regulär use in foreign language classrooms in countries and regions all over the world, including the United States (Facella, Rampino, & Shea, 2005), the United Kingdom (Evans & Fisher, 2012), Japan (Hashimoto, 2011), China (Jin & Cortazzi, 2006; Su, 2006), Vietnam (Nguyen, 2011), Greece (Konstantakis & Alexiou, 2012), Malaysia (Setia et al., 2012), and undoubtably many, if not most, others.

However, many of the materials that promote the benefits of songs for foreign language teaching are written from a pedagogical perspective and make little attempt to fully understand theoretical models through which songs might facilitate language learning. This paper will first explore theories and research from psychological studies of songs and memory in order to draw insights about the nature of the interaction, and will then assess previous studies that have attempted to measure the effectiveness of songs in a foreign language classroom (or classroom-like) context.

Theories of songs and memory

The relationship between music and memory has been studied from several angles. Despite the common perception among laypeople that memory for music has Special qualities (Schulkind, 2009), consensus among researchers has yet to be achieved. Rather than showing a consistent advantage for memories associated with songs and music, studies have instead displayed a range of sometimes contradictory findings, leading to difficulty in drawing any general conclusions.

Wallace’s (1994) study is often cited to support the idea that songs have Special mnemonic properties. Through a series of experiments, it was found that participants were better able to recall text when it was presented with a melody, rather than spoken normally. However, when the three verses of text were each presented with different melodies, recall was worse than when spoken. Wallace concluded that the rhythmic and structural qualities of the music facilitated retrieval and reconstruction by informing participants about the structural nature of the text (e.g., how many syllables were in the next word).

Other research casts doubt on Wallace’s hypothesis that the structural properties of songs facilitate memory recall. Kilgour, Jakobson, and Cuddy (2000) conducted a partial replication of Wallace’s (1994) methodology, with an additional manipulation of the tempo at which the texts were presented. The researchers found that in Wallace’s original experiments, the sung versions of texts were presented at a slower rate than the spoken versions; when the songs were sped up to the same rate as the spoken texts, the mnemonic advantages of the songs disappeared and instead a small but statistically significant advantage for the spoken versions emerged.

Whereas Wallace’s (1994) and Kilgour et al.’s (2000) studies examined memory of text extracted from real songs, Rainey and Larsen (2002) investigated the effects of setting unrelated texts to commonly known melodies (e.g., Pop Goes The Weasel). As many songs used in foreign language classrooms are specifically designed to put an unrelated text (with some assumed pedagogical value) to music, this latter methodology may be more generalizable to the foreign language classroom context. Rainey and Larsen found no difference in the number of trials required to learn a text sung along to a familiär melody as compared with the same text spoken normally, but the songs were more easily relearned one week later.

Some scholars have proposed that songs may exhibit mnemonic advantages through becoming “earworms” that are continuously repeated in one’s mind without conscious control (Williamson et al., 2011). Murphey (1990) observed the similarities between the song-stuck-in-my-head phenomenon and the “Din” of involuntary rehearsal that often occurs as foreign language learners feel their minds become unconsciously saturated with words and phrases in the target language, and hypothesized that songs may be able to enhance language acquisition through this pathway. If, as Rainey and Larsen’s (2002) results indicate, the mnemonic effects of songs are manifested over longer periods of time, the “earworm” phenomenon may be a contributing factor.

Although various theories regarding the interaction between songs and memory have been proposed, the irregular pattern of results obtained from experiments in this area allows for no obvious theoretical models by which songs might enhance foreign language learning. Thus, this paper will now turn to examining empirical studies of songs and language learning in an attempt to find consensus.

Previous studies

An extensive search of the literature reveals only a small number of studies that have aimed to evaluate any sort of Performance measure for language acquisition through songs. Four previous studies have focused on vocabulary acquisition, a construct that can be somewhat straightforwardly mapped onto the above discussion of songs and memory.

Medina (1990) measured L2 English vocabulary acquisition by L1 Spanish-speaking second grade primary school students in the United States by manipulating an illustrated storybook to produce four groups from two independent variables: sung or spoken audio recording; illustrations or no illustrations. Although no statistically significant findings resulted from the manipulation of the variables, scores at post-test were (descriptively) higher than scores at pre-test, suggesting that the storybook in all conditions (including the two that used songs) resulted in some acquisition of the English vocabulary.

Joyce (2011) also compared sung and spoken versions of storybooks in a study of L1 English acquisition in American kindergarten students. Joyce found similar results: acquisition through the sung and spoken versions could not be differentiated at a statistically significant level.

The authors of the two studies detailed above both concluded that sung storybooks were as effective as spoken storybooks in promoting vocabulary acquisition, thus appearing to lend support to the use of songs in language classrooms. However, both studies were undermined by poor methodology: namely, the absence of a true control group that was not exposed to the material. As any improvements from pre-test to post-test could also be attributable to general cognitive maturation over time (especially given the young age of participants), practice effects from performing the same test twice in a short period of time, exposure to similar materials in external classes, increased familiarity with researchers, or any number of other possibilities, it is also conceivable that the sung and spoken storybooks were equally ineffective.

A third relevant study was conducted by Coyle and Gömez Gracia (2014). The researchers examined English vocabulary acquisition in Spanish preschool children as measured before and after three 30-minute lessons based around the populär children’s song The Wheels on the Bus. Results showed a significant improvement in receptive vocabulary that was consistent up to five weeks later, although productive vocabulary did not show significant acquisition.

Chou (2014) also examined the effects of songs on L2 acquisition as part of an intensive English course in a Taiwanese primary school. Games, songs, and storybooks were used throughout the course, and students performed better after the teaching period on four tests of vocabulary knowledge.

Both Coyle and Gömez Gracia’s (2014) and Chou’s (2014) studies suffer from the same methodological concern as the two storybook studies: inadequate controls. Although it is indeed possible that the songs directly led to the observed vocabulary acquisition, there were no other groups against which to compare. Even if the songs indeed contained some pedagogical value, it is impossible to determine whether another teaching method might be equally or even more effective at teaching the same material. Further, in Chou’s study, the songs were integrated as part of the curriculum and so their effects could not be isolated.

Due to the weak designs of these four studies, no useful evidence can be obtained from them, leaving a bare landscape with regard to empirical knowledge about the effectiveness of songs in any language classroom. Although both public and private resources have been pouring into English education worldwide, empirical support for the use of songs in language teaching is weak at best, and little research has examined their effectiveness and how their design and implementation might affect pedagogical value. The use of songs in the foreign language classroom can thus be seen as a microcosm of the issues surrounding English education at the primary school level: there are plenty of opinions as to what should be done, but few objective attempts to compare different ideas and determine the best way forward.

Method

Research Design

The present study aimed to investigate the effectiveness of songs as used for teaching EFL to young children, and contribute to a literature base that could be used to drive policy changes toward more empirically supported curricula.

One main goal was to address methodological issues present in many previous studies of songs as used for foreign language instruction. As detailed above, previous empirical research into the use of songs in the classroom has failed to provide any strong evidence to differentiate it from any other methods of instruction, and a lack of proper controls in many studies leaves questions as to whether the songs have any pedagogical effects at all. Thus, the present study was designed with both a comparison method of instruction (choral repetition) and a control condition where students were not exposed to the material, allowing for fine distinctions to be drawn regarding the effectiveness of songs as a method of instruction. An identical test (see Measures section) was administered at pre- and post-test in order to assess vocabulary acquisition through a repeated measures (within-subject) design.

The research questions for this study were thus as follows:

  • RQ1. Does the teaching of EFL vocabulary through songs lead to significantly more acquisition than control items?

  • RQ2. Does the teaching of EFL vocabulary through songs lead to significantly more or less acquisition than teaching through choral repetition?

Participants

Intact kindergarten classes from two separate schools were used in this study in an attempt to simulate a realistic Chinese EFL context. School administrators, teachers, and parents agreed to take part in this research study. Classes A, B, and C contained 20, 21, and 23 students, respectively. All students were between 4 and 5 years of age.

The schools were private kindergartens in Beijing and thus may not be representative of the experiences of most Chinese children. Students in these kindergartens received regulär English lessons three times per week, a similar frequency to that offered in many primary schools. Through informal pilot testing and examination of the English instruction materials used by the schools, the English level of the students was estimated to be similar to that of students in first or second grade at regular primary schools. Thus, the results of this study may be more applicable to early-grade primary school instruction at the majority of schools in China.

Measures

The same measure of English vocabulary knowledge was applied at both pre- and post-test. It consisted of a series of 15 small cards (approximately the same size as Standard casino playing cards), each with a cartoon picture on one side. These picrures were taken from the English textbook series (Fan, 2012) in use in some Chinese primary schools, and most consisted of a Single animal performing an action or a scene with one or more people. The children were individually led into a room where the researcher was seated at a table and were told they would be looking at some pictures.

The cards were shown one at a time, and the child was prompted to say what was on the card. No further cues as to how the children were to describe the cards were given, except to ask the children to speak in English if they spoke Chinese instead. Responses were recorded on a small audio recorder and coded for number of meaningful morphemes produced, following the long tradition of research assessing mean length of utterance (MLU) that dates back to Brown (1973). In this fashion, a response of “the lion is angry” was scored as 4 morphemes, while “lion” by itself was scored as 1.

Procedures

The study followed a basic pre-test, Intervention, post-test design. Administration of the vocabulary knowledge pre-test (see Measures section) was conducted over two days, and the Intervention period began two days after the completion of all pre-tests, with the post-test following directly after the Intervention period.

The Intervention period consisted of 15 lessons of 40 minutes in length over a seven-week period, with one to three lessons per week during this period. Due to holidays and other obligations at the schools, the pattern of lessons was somewhat irregulär, which mirrors the real-life classroom Situation for EFL teachers in China. All groups were taught by the same teacher, who has several years’ experience teaching EFL to young children.

Every lesson followed the same format. At the beginning of each lesson, the teacher would sing the same warm up song with all of the students. The bulk of each lesson was spent learning and singing songs, with any additional time devoted to games and activities that did not use the language targeted in this study.

The songs in the middle of each lesson (containing the language targeted in this study) were taken from the English Grade 1 and 2 textbooks. All songs consisted of new texts set to familiär melodies such as Twinkle Twinkle Little Star and The More We Get Together, or very simple and repetitive novel melodies.

When a new song was being introduced or a familiär song rehearsed, a relevant picture (identical to the one displayed on the cards in the vocabulary knowledge test) was displayed on a large screen. This allowed students to learn and relate the words of the song to the context of the picture and what actions or characteristics were being shown.

As the study followed a repeated measures (within-subject) design, all items were assigned to all students. The 15 items were assigned to the three classes with five items in each of three conditions: song, repeat, and control. This assignment followed the pattern shown in Table 1 and resulted in each class being exposed to a different combination of items through each of the three conditions. Items learned through song were learned and rehearsed as described above. With items in the repeat condition, the picture was shown on the screen and students were asked to repeat after the teacher in a choral repetition fashion. The teacher repeated the phrase shown in Table 1 twice per class for each item/ picture combination. Students were not exposed to control items or associated pictures during the intervention period of the study, and only encountered the associated pictures at pre- and post-test.

Table 1.

Corresponding Song and Experimental Condition by Class for Each Item

PhraseSongClass AClass BClass C
Hie monkey has a long tailMonkey SongSRC
The pig is fatPig SongCSR
The rabbit is fastRabbit SongRCS
The giraffe is tallGiraffe SongSRc
The wolf is lightWolf SongCSR
The duck is sadDuck SongRCS
The fox is happyFox SongSRc
The lion is angryFox SongSRc
The fox took away my boneFox SongsRc
The three Little pigs are very afraidThe Three Little Pigs SongcSR
A big bad wolf is coming to usThe Three Little Pigs SongcSR
It’s warm and rainySpring SongRCS
It's hot and sunnySummer SongCSR
It's cool and windyAutumn SongRCS
Leaves fall from treesAutumn SongRCS

Note. S = song, R = repeat, C = control.

Results

Participants’ responses to the vocabulary knowledge test were coded for MLU as described in the Measures section. Descriptive statistics are shown in Table 2. All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS version 20. A repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted with time (pre-test, post-test) and condition (song, repeat, control) as independent variables.

Table 2.

Average Mean Length of Utterance Produced for Each Item

GroupNMSD
Pre-test
Song430.600.59
Repeat430.590.51
Control430.580.48
Post-test
Song431.481.09
Repeat431.300.94
Control430.660.52

Mauchly’s test revealed that the assumption of sphericity was violated for the interaction between time and condition, □2(2) = 8.27, p = .02. As such, degrees of freedom were corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser estimates of sphericity (ε = .85).

All significant effects are reported at p < .001. There was a significant main effect of time, F(1, 42) = 31.05, partial|2 = .43, with post-test scores on the vocabulary acquisition measure higher than pre-test scores.

There was also a significant main effect of condition, F(2, 84) = 16.07, partial|2 = .28. Contrasts revealed a non-significant difference between the song and repeat conditions, F(1, 42) = 2.22, p = .14, but significant differences between the song and control conditions, F(1, 42) = 22.29, partial|2 = .35, and between the repeat and control conditions, F(1, 42) = 18.22, partial|2 = .30. This reflects significantly better Performance in the song and repeat conditions than in the control condition, but no differentiation between the song and repeat conditions.

There was a significant interaction effect between time and condition, F(1.69, 71.02) = 18.72, partial|2 = .31. This indicates that changes between students’ pre-test and post-test scores depended on the condition in which texts were learned. Contrasts revealed significant interactions when comparing scores from the song condition to the control condition, F(1, 42) = 25.66, partial|2 = .38, and when comparing scores from the repeat condition to the control condition, F(1, 42) = 19.08, partial|2 = .31. As indicated in Figure 1, these interactions reflect more positive change from pre-test to post-test (i.e., more acquisition of usable vocabulary) when texts were learned under the song or repeat condition as opposed to the control condition. Contrasts revealed no significant interaction when comparing scores from the repeat condition to the song condition, F(1, 42) = 2.64, p = .11.

Figure 1. Average MLU for items at pre-test and post-test, grouped by condition.
Figure 1.

Average MLU for items at pre-test and post-test, grouped by condition.

Discussion and Conclusions

The results of this study suggested that songs and choral repetition were equally effective at promoting English vocabulary acquisition in Chinese kindergarten students over a period of 15 classes of 40 minutes. There did not appear to be any Special characteristics that made songs more or less effective than choral repetition, but rather both conditions led to significantly more acquisition than control items, which did not improve from pre-test to post-test.

These findings accord with previous research demonstrating no significant differences between acquisition of the target language measure through songs and through other teaching methods. This apparent equality in effecting language acquisition in children mirrors the results of other empirical studies with measures such as vocabulary acquisition through sung or spoken storybooks (Joyce, 2011; Medina, 1990) and text comprehension for sung or spoken storybooks (Kouri & Telander, 2008).

Although there was no evidence to suggest that songs were any more or less effective than choral repetition at promoting vocabulary acquisition, the present study still provides critical information. Previous studies of vocabulary acquisition through song (Chou, 2014; Coyle & Gömez Gracia, 2014; Joyce, 2011; Medina, 1990) lacked adequate controls and thus conclusions that songs directly led to vocabulary acquisition were overambitious. The key inclusion, in the present study, of control items that were not taught or shown to the children over the Intervention period and showed no improvement from pre-test to post-test allows for the preliminary (and long overdue) Statement that songs may indeed provide some value in promoting language acquisition. As control items did not lead to any acquisition over the same time period as items in the song and choral repetition conditions, alternative explanations for acquisition, such as general cognitive maturation, practice effects from performing the same test multiple times, external exposure to similar materials, and increased familiarity with researchers, can be ruled out. Thus, it can then be tentatively concluded that both songs and choral repetition do indeed have some level of positive pedagogical value beyond their observed beneficial effects on motivation (Chou, 2014; Jarvis, 2013; Jolly, 1975; Lo & Li, 1998).

Pedagogical implications

The present study provided an example of songs facilitating vocabulary acquisition in a realistic Chinese EFL context. Much more remains to be determined regarding the role of songs in effective curricula and how their design can be optimized to increase pedagogical value, but the current finding that songs directly led to some non-zero acquisition will be of use to teachers and administrators. However, it should also be noted that the comparison method of vocabulary instruction used in this study, choral repetition, led to the same amount of acquisition over the teaching period. Thus, it is unclear whether the lay idea that memory for songs has Special qualities (Schulkind, 2009) holds true or has any effect when applied to language acquisition.

Rather, it may instead be more prudent to include songs as one part of an effective foreign language curriculum, alongside other methods such as choral repetition. Previous studies (Chou, 2014; Jarvis, 2013) have noted that songs, when integrated into a foreign language program at the primary school level, are well received by students and may lead to higher levels of motivation.

Future directions for research

The present study used songs that consisted of new texts set to familiär or simple and repetitive melodies. Rainey and Larsen (2002) found that setting texts to familiär melodies led to easier relearning, but their study used only a Single text for each melody, whereas the present study examined multiple texts set to the same familiär melodies. Further research may be able to determine whether the setting of multiple texts to the same melody aids or detracts from acquisition.

Another thread of research has explored the role of gestures in acquiring foreign language vocabulary (e.g., Kelly, McDevitt, & Esch, 2009; Macedonia & Klimesch, 2014; Porter, 2012; see Macedonia, 2014, for a review). As songs are often accompanied by gestures when used in foreign language classrooms (e.g., Claerr & Gargan, 1984; Coyle & Gömez Gracia, 2014; Gan & Chong, 1998; Tuan & An, 2010), further investigation of the interaction between songs and gestures may also lead to valuable findings.

About the authors

Glenn M. Davis

Glenn M. Davis is a lecturer in the Department of Foreign Languages and Literatures at Tsinghua University in Beijing, China. His research focuses on quantitative measurement of educational outcomes in second and foreign language learning

Wenfang Fan

Wenfang Fan is a professor in the Department of Foreign Languages and Literatures at Tsinghua University in Beijing, China. Her research focuses on functional linguistics and teaching English as a foreign language from grade schools up to tertiary level

References

Brown, R. (1973). Development of the first language in the human species. American Psychologist, 28(2), 97-106. doi:10.1037/h003420910.1037/h0034209Search in Google Scholar

Chou, M. (2014). Assessing English vocabulary and enhancing young English as a Foreign Language (EFL) learners’ motivation through games, songs, and stories. Education 3-13, 42(3), 284-297. doi:10.1080/03004279.2012.68089910.1080/03004279.2012.680899Search in Google Scholar

Claerr, T. A., & Gargan, R. (1984). The role of songs in the foreign language classroom. OMLTAJournal, 28-32.Search in Google Scholar

Coyle, Y., & Gömez Gracia, R. (2014). Using songs to enhance L2 vocabulary acquisition in preschool children. ELT Journal, 68(3), 276-285. doi:10.1093/elt/ccu01510.1093/elt/ccu015Search in Google Scholar

Evans, M., & Fisher, L. (2012). Emergent communities of practice: Secondary schools’ interaction with primary school foreign language teaching and learning. The Language Learning Journal, 40(2), 157-173. doi:10.1080/09571736.2011.56735710.1080/09571736.2011.567357Search in Google Scholar

Facella, M. A., Rampino, K. M., & Shea, E. K. (2005). Effective teaching strategies for English language learners. Bilingual Research Journal, 29(1), 209-221. doi:10.1080/15235882.2005.101 6283210.1080/15235882.2005.101 62832Search in Google Scholar

Fan, W. F. (2012). English. Beijing: Tsinghua University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Gan, L., & Chong, S. (1998). The rhythm of language: Fostering oral and listening skills in Singapore pre-school children through an integrated music and language arts program. Early Child Development and Care, 144, 39-45. doi:10.1080/030044398144010510.1080/0300443981440105Search in Google Scholar

Hashimoto, K. (2011). Compulsory “foreign language activities” in Japanese primary schools. Currenthsues in Language Planning, 12(2), 167-184. doi:10.1080/14664208.2011.58595810.1080/14664208.2011.585958Search in Google Scholar

Jarvis, S. (2013). How effective is it to teach a foreign language in the Foundation Stage through songs and rhymes? Education 3-13, 41(1), 47-54. doi:10.1080/03004279.2012.71009910.1080/03004279.2012.710099Search in Google Scholar

Jin, L., & Cortazzi, M. (2006). Changing practices in Chinese cultures of learning. Language, Cultureand Curriculum, 19(1), 5-20. doi:10.1080/0790831060866875110.1080/07908310608668751Search in Google Scholar

Jolly, Y. S. (1975). The use of songs in teaching foreign languages. The Modern Language Journal, 59(1), 11-14. doi:10.1111/j.l540-4781.1975.tb03618.x10.1111/j.l540-4781.1975.tb03618.xSearch in Google Scholar

Joyce, M. F. (2011). Vocabulary acquisition with kindergarten children using song picture books. Doctoral dissertation, Northeastern University. Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/2047/d2000236510.17760/d20002365Search in Google Scholar

Kelly, S. D., McDevitt, X, & Esch, M. (2009). Brief training with co-speech gesture lends a hand to word learning in a foreign language. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(2), 313-334. doi:10.1080/0169096080236556710.1080/01690960802365567Search in Google Scholar

Keskin, F. (2011). Using songs as audio materials in teaching Turkish as a foreign language. The Turkish Online Journal of Educational Technology, 10(4), 378-383.Search in Google Scholar

Kilgour, A. R., Jakobson, L. S., & Cuddy, L. L. (2000). Music training and rate of presentation as mediators of text and song recall. Memory & Cognition, 28(5), 700-710. doi:10.3758/ BF0319840410.3758/ BF03198404Search in Google Scholar

Konstantakis, N., & Alexiou, T. (2012). Vocabulary in Greek young learners’ English as a foreign language course books. The Language Learning Journal. doi:10.1080/09571736.2012.65822210.1080/09571736.2012.658222Search in Google Scholar

Kouri, T., & Telander, K. (2008). Children’s reading comprehension and narrative recall in sung and spoken story contexts. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 24(3), 329-349. doi:10.1177/026565900809629610.1177/0265659008096296Search in Google Scholar

Lo, R. S. M., & Li, H. C. F. (1998). Songs enhance learner involvement: Materials development. English Teaching Forum, 36(3), 8-11.Search in Google Scholar

Lorenzutti, N. (2014). Beyond the gap fill: Dynamic activities for song in the EFL classroom. English Teaching Forum, 52(1), 14-2.Search in Google Scholar

Macedonia, M. (2014). Bringing back the body into the mind: Gestures enhance word learning in foreign language. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1-6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.0146710.3389/fpsyg.2014.01467Search in Google Scholar

Macedonia, M., & Klimesch, W. (2014). Long-term effects of gestures on memory for foreign language words trained in the classroom. Mind, Brain, and Education, 8(2), 74-88. doi:10.1111/mbe.l204710.1111/mbe.l2047Search in Google Scholar

Medina, S. L. (1990). The effects of music upon second language vocabulary acquisition. ERIC Document Reproduction Service No ED, 352, 834. Retrieved from http://eric.ed.gov/ ERICWebPortal/recordDetail?accno=ED352834Search in Google Scholar

Millington, N. T. (2011). Using songs effectively to teach English to young learners. Language Education in Asia, 2(1), 134-141. doi:10.5746/LEiA/ll/V2/Il/All/Millington10.5746/LEiA/ll/V2/Il/All/MillingtonSearch in Google Scholar

Murphey, T. (1990). The song stuck in my head phenomenon: A melodic din in the lad? System, 18(1), 53-64. doi:10.1016/0346-251X(90)90028-410.1016/0346-251X(90)90028-4Search in Google Scholar

Murphey, T. (1992). Music and song. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Nguyen, H. T. M. (2011). Primary English language education policy in Vietnam: Insights from implementation. Current Issues in Language Planning, 12(2), 225-249. doi:10.1080/14664208.2011.59704810.1080/14664208.2011.597048Search in Google Scholar

Orlova, N. F. (2003). Helping prospective EFL teachers learn how to use songs in teaching conversation classes. The Internet TESL Journal, 9(3). Retrieved from http://iteslj.org/ Techniques/Orlova-Songs.htmlSearch in Google Scholar

Paquette, K. R., & Rieg, S. A. (2008). Using music to support the literacy development of young English language learners. Early Childhood Education Journal, 36(3), 227-232. doi:10.1007/S10643-008-0277-910.1007/S10643-008-0277-9Search in Google Scholar

Porter, A. (2012). A helping hand with language learning: Teaching French vocabulary with gesture. The Language Learning Journal, iFirst article, 1-21. doi:10.1080/09571736.2012.75068110.1080/09571736.2012.750681Search in Google Scholar

Rainey, D. W., & Larsen, J. D. (2002). The effect of familiär melodies on initial learning and long-term memory for unconnected text. Music Perception, 20(2), 173-186. doi:10.1525/mp.2002.20.2.17310.1525/mp.2002.20.2.173Search in Google Scholar

Richards, J. (1969). Songs in language learning. TESOL Quarterly, 3(2), 161-174. doi: 10.2307/3586103.10.2307/3586103Search in Google Scholar

Saricoban, A., & Metin, E. (2000). Songs, verse and games for teaching grammar. The Internet TESL Journal, 6(10). Retrieved from http://iteslj.org/Techniques/Saricoban-Songs.htmlSearch in Google Scholar

Schulkind, M. D. (2009). Is memory for music Special? Annais ofthe New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 216-24. doi:10.1111/J.1749-6632.2009.04546.x10.1111/J.1749-6632.2009.04546.xSearch in Google Scholar

Setia, R., Rahim, R. A., Nair, G. K. S., Adam, A. F. binti M., Husin, N., Sabapathy, E., Seman, N. A. (2012). English songs as means of aiding students’ proficiency development. Asian Social Science, 8(7), 270-274. doi:10.5539/ass.v8n7p27010.5539/ass.v8n7p270Search in Google Scholar

§evik, M. (2012). Teaching listening skills to young learners through “listen and do” songs.English Teaching Forum, 50(3), 10-17. Retrieved from http://americanenglish.state.gov/resources/ enghsh-teaching-forum-2012-volume-50-number-3#child-1351Search in Google Scholar

Su, Y. (2006). EFL teachers’ perceptions of English language policy at the elementary level in Taiwan. Educational Studies, 32(3), 265-283. doi:10.1080/0305569060063121810.1080/03055690600631218Search in Google Scholar

Tuan, L. T., & An, P. T. V. (2010). Teaching English rhythm by using songs. Studies in Literature and Language, 1(2), 13-29.Search in Google Scholar

Wallace, W. T. (1994). Memory for music: Effect ofmelody on recall of text. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(6), 1471-1485. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.6.147110.1037/0278-7393.20.6.1471Search in Google Scholar

Williamson, V. J., Jilka, S. R., Fry, J., Finkel, S., Müllensiefen, D., & Stewart, L. (2011). How do “earworms” Start? Classifying the everyday circumstances of involuntary musical imagery. Psychology of Music, 40(3), 259-284. doi:10.1177/030573561141855310.1177/0305735611418553Search in Google Scholar


(Copyedited by DING Yanren)


Published Online: 2016-3-19
Published in Print: 2016-3-1

© FLTRP, Walter de Gruyter, Cultural and Education Section British Embassy

Downloaded on 8.10.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/cjal-2016-0004/html?licenseType=free
Scroll to top button