Home What predicts productivity? Theory meets individuals
Article
Licensed
Unlicensed Requires Authentication

What predicts productivity? Theory meets individuals

  • Hendrik De Smet ORCID logo EMAIL logo
Published/Copyright: February 26, 2020

Abstract

Because they involve individual-level cognitive processes, psychological explanations of linguistic phenomena are in principle testable against individual behaviour. The present study draws on patterns of individual variation in corpus data to test explanations of productivity. Linguistic patterns are predicted to become more productive with higher type frequencies and lower token frequencies. This is because the formation of abstract mental representations is encouraged by varied types but counteracted by automation of high-frequency types. The predictions are tested for English -ly and -ness-derivation, as used by 698 individual journalists in the New York Times Annotated Corpus and 171 members of Parliament in the Hansard Corpus. Linear regression is used to model individual variation in productivity, in relation to type and token frequency, as well as several other predictor variables. While the expected effects are observed, there is also robust evidence of an interaction effect between type and token frequency, indicating that productivity is highest for patterns with many types and not-too-infrequent tokens. This fits best with a view of entrenchment as both a conservative and creative force in language. Further, some variation remains irreducibly individual and is not explained by currently known predictors of productivity.

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank the editors and anonymous reviewers, as well as audiences in Berlin and London, for their constructive feedback on an earlier version of this paper.

Appendix: Model summaries for ly_NYTACS, ness_NYTACS, ly_HCS and ness_HCS

Ly_NYTACS
OLS linear regression
Dependent variable: Productivity
Observations: 697
Est.S.E.t val.pVIF
(Intercept)−0.200.02−8.840.00NA
Token frequency−0.070.02−2.970.002.58
Type frequency0.520.0318.400.004.19
Main desk ‘culture and lifestyle’0.300.065.160.002.37
Main desk ‘finance’0.150.043.440.002.37
Main desk ‘other’0.160.044.180.002.37
Main desk ‘sports’0.110.052.090.042.37
General productivity0.240.0210.410.002.75
Token frequency: Type frequency0.120.0114.140.001.63
F(8,688) = 511.65, p = 0.00
R2 = 0.86, Adj. R2 = 0.85
Ness_NYTACS
OLS linear regression

Dependent variable: Productivity
Observations: 696
Est.S.E.t val.pVIF
(Intercept)−0.140.02−6.430.00NA
Token frequency−0.120.03−3.480.005.66
Type frequency0.610.0415.710.008.50
Main desk ‘culture and lifestyle’−0.090.06−1.490.142.05
Main desk ‘finance’0.080.041.970.052.05
Main desk ‘other’0.170.044.750.002.05
Main desk ‘sports’0.060.051.200.232.05
General productivity0.260.0210.670.003.11
Token frequency: Type frequency0.120.0112.560.002.76
F(8,687) = 608.84, p = 0.00

R2 = 0.88, Adj. R2 = 0.87
Ly_HCS
OLS linear regression
Dependent variable: Productivity
Observations: 171
Est.S.E.t val.pVIF
(Intercept)−0.090.05−1.700.09NA
Token frequency−0.030.07−0.450.651.80
Type frequency0.660.079.450.002.04
General productivity0.240.054.480.001.23
Token frequency: Type frequency0.140.034.180.001.06
F(4,166) = 62.73, p = 0.00

R2 = 0.60, Adj. R2 = 0.59
Ness_HCS
OLS linear regression
Dependent variable: Productivity
Observations: 171
Est.S.E.t val.pVIF
(Intercept)−0.060.07−0.920.36NA
Token frequency0.030.070.400.691.39
Type frequency0.370.094.250.002.03
General productivity0.230.082.980.001.59
Token frequency: Type frequency0.120.052.310.021.11
F(4,166) = 25.12, p = 0.00
R2 = 0.38, Adj. R2 = 0.36

References

Alexander, Marc & Mark Davies. 2015. The Hansard Corpus 1803–2005. http://www.hansard-corpus.org.Search in Google Scholar

Baayen, Harald. 1993. On frequency, transparency and productivity. In Geert Booij & Jaap van Marle (eds.), Yearbook of morphology 1993, 181–208. Dordrecht: Kluwer.10.1007/978-94-017-3710-4_7Search in Google Scholar

Baayen, Harald & Rochelle Lieber. 1991. Productivity and word-formation in English: A corpus-based study. Linguistics 29. 801–843.10.1515/ling.1991.29.5.801Search in Google Scholar

Barðdal, Jóhanna. 2006. Predicting the productivity of argument structure constructions. Berkeley Linguistics Society 32. 467–478.10.3765/bls.v32i1.3438Search in Google Scholar

Barðdal, Jóhanna. 2008. Productivity: Evidence from case and argument structure in Icelandic. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/cal.8Search in Google Scholar

Blumenthal-Dramé, Alice. 2012. Entrenchment in usage-based theories: What corpus data do and do not reveal about the mind. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.10.1515/9783110294002Search in Google Scholar

Bolinger, Dwight. 1948. On defining the morpheme. Word 4. 18–23.10.1080/00437956.1948.11659323Search in Google Scholar

Booij, Geert. 2010. Construction morphology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1111/j.1749-818X.2010.00213.xSearch in Google Scholar

Bybee, Joan. 1995. Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes 10. 425–455.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195301571.003.0008Search in Google Scholar

Bybee, Joan. 2007. Frequency of use and the organization of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195301571.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Carroll, John B. 1981. Twenty-five years of research in foreign language aptitude. In Karl C. Diller (ed.), Individual differences and universals in language learning aptitude, 83–113. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Search in Google Scholar

Corbin, Danielle. 1987. Morphogie derivationelle et structuration du lexique. Tübingen: Niemeyer.10.1515/9783111358383Search in Google Scholar

Dąbrowska, Ewa. 2008. The later development of an early-emerging system: The curious case of the Polish genitive. Linguistics 46. 629–650.10.1515/LING.2008.021Search in Google Scholar

De Smet, Hendrik. 2016. How gradual change progresses: The interaction between convention and innovation. Language Variation and Change 28. 83–102.10.1017/S0954394515000186Search in Google Scholar

De Smet, Hendrik. 2017. Entrenchment effects in language change. In Hans–Jörg Schmid (ed.), Entrenchment, memory and automaticity: The psychology of linguistic knowledge and language learning, 75–99. Washington: American Psychology Association.10.1037/15969-005Search in Google Scholar

De Smet, Hendrik. 2018. Unwitting inventors: English speakers use –ly-adverbs more creatively when primed. Zeitschrift für Anglistik und Amerikanistik 66. 329–340.10.1515/zaa-2018-0028Search in Google Scholar

De Smet, Hendrik & Freek Van de Velde. 2017. Experimenting on the past: A case study on changing analysability in English –ly-adverbs. English Language and Linguistics 21. 317–340.10.1017/S1360674317000168Search in Google Scholar

Giegerich, Heinz. 2012. The morphology of -ly and the categorial status of ‘adverbs’ in English. English Language and Linguistics 16. 341–359.10.1017/S1360674312000147Search in Google Scholar

Goldberg, Adele E. 2006. Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199268511.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Gregory, Michael. 1967. Aspects of varieties differentiation. Journal of Linguistics 3. 177–274.10.1017/S0022226700016601Search in Google Scholar

Hay, Jennifer. 2003. Causes and consequences of word structure. London: Routledge.10.4324/9780203495131Search in Google Scholar

Hay, Jennifer & Harald Baayen. 2002. Parsing and productivity. In Geert Booij & Jaap van Marle (eds.), Yearbook of Morphology 2001, 203–235. Dordrecht: Kluwer.10.1007/978-94-017-3726-5_8Search in Google Scholar

Hentschel, Gerd & Thomas Menzel. 2002. Marker productivity, structural preferences and frequency: Observations about morphological change in slavonic languages. Indogermanischen Forschungen 107. 1–46.10.1515/if-2002-0102Search in Google Scholar

King, Jonathan & Marcel A. Just. 1991. Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory. Journal of Memory and Language 30. 580–602.10.1016/0749-596X(91)90027-HSearch in Google Scholar

Körtvélyessy, Lívia & Pavol Štekauer. 2014. Derivation in a social context. In Rochelle Lieber & Pavol Štekauer (eds.), The Oxford handbook of derivational morphology, 407–423. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Langacker, Ronald W. 1987. Foundations of Cognitive Grammar, Vol. 1, Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Long, Jacob A. 2019. jtools: Analysis and presentation of social scientific data. R package version 2.0.0, https://cran.r-project.org/package=jtools.Search in Google Scholar

Lüdecke, Daniel. 2018. sjPlot: Data visualization for statistics in social science. R package version 2.4.1.9000, https://cran.r-project.org/package=sjPlot.Search in Google Scholar

Mollin, Sandra. 2007. The Hansard hazard: Gauging the accuracy of British parliamentary transcripts. Corpora 2. 187–210.10.3366/cor.2007.2.2.187Search in Google Scholar

Nevalainen, Terttu, Helena Raumolin-Brunberg & Heikki Mannila. 2011. The diffusion of language change in real time: Progressive and conservative individuals and the time depth of change. Language Variation and Change 23. 1–43.10.1017/S0954394510000207Search in Google Scholar

Pinker, Steven. 1999. Words and rules: The ingredients of language. New York: Basic Books.Search in Google Scholar

Plag, Ingo. 1999. Morphological productivity: Structural constraints in English derivation. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Search in Google Scholar

Plag, Ingo. 2003. Word-formation in English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511841323Search in Google Scholar

Rumelhart, David E. & James L. McClelland. 1987. On learning the past tenses of English verbs: Implicit rules or parallel distributed processing? In Brian MacWhinney (ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition, 195–248. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.Search in Google Scholar

Säily, Tanja. 2018. Change or variation? Productivity of the suffixes -ness and -ity. In Terttu Nevalainen, Minna Palander-Collin & Tanja Säily (eds.), Patterns of change in 18th-century English: A sociolinguistic approach, 197–218. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/ahs.8.12saiSearch in Google Scholar

Sandhaus, Evan. 2008. The New York Times Annotated Corpus LDC2008T19. DVD. Philadelphia: Linguistic Data Consortium.Search in Google Scholar

Schmid, Hans-Jörg. 2017. A framework for understanding linguistic entrenchment and its psychological foundations. In Hans-Jörg Schmid (ed.), Entrenchment and the psychology of language learning: How we reorganize and adapt linguistic knowledge, 9–35. Berlin: De Gruyter.10.1037/15969-002Search in Google Scholar

Schmid, Hans-Jörg. 2018. Unifying entrenched tokens and schematized types as routinized commonalities of linguistic experience. Yearbook of the German Cognitive Linguistics Association 6. 167–182.10.1515/gcla-2018-0008Search in Google Scholar

Stefanowitsch, Anatol. 2008. Negative entrenchment. A usage-based approach to negative evidence. Cognitive Linguistics 19. 513–531.10.1515/COGL.2008.020Search in Google Scholar

Štekauer , James & Ewa Dąbrowska. 2014. Lexically specific knowledge and individual differences in adult native speakers’ processing of the English passive. Applied Psycholinguistics 35. 97–118.10.1017/S0142716412000367Search in Google Scholar

Taylor, John R. 2002. Cognitive Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Verhagen, Véronique, Maria Mos, Ad Backus & Joost Schilperoord. 2018. Predictive language processing revealing usage-based variation. Language and Cognition 10. 329–373.10.1017/langcog.2018.4Search in Google Scholar

Wickham, Hadley 2016. ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. New York: Springer-Verlag. https://cran.r-project.org/package=ggplot210.1007/978-3-319-24277-4Search in Google Scholar

Yu, Alan C. L. & Georgia Zellou. 2019. Individual differences in language processing: Phonology. Annual Review of Linguistics 5. 131–150.10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011516-033815Search in Google Scholar

Received: 2019-04-08
Revised: 2020-01-07
Accepted: 2020-01-24
Published Online: 2020-02-26
Published in Print: 2020-06-25

© 2020 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 25.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/cog-2019-0026/html
Scroll to top button