Home Zur Wahrnehmung des generischen Maskulinums in Erstgliedern von Komposita und maskuliner Epizöna
Article
Licensed
Unlicensed Requires Authentication

Zur Wahrnehmung des generischen Maskulinums in Erstgliedern von Komposita und maskuliner Epizöna

  • Fabian Bross ORCID logo and Lea-Sophie Kurz EMAIL logo
Published/Copyright: December 5, 2023

Abstract

Most German nouns that can be used to refer to people or groups of people have a masculine (e.g., Lehrer ‘male teacher’) and a derived feminine (e.g., Lehrerin ‘female teacher’) form. The grammatically masculine nouns are typically viewed as being ambiguous between a reading which can only be used to refer to men and a reading which can be used to refer to mixed-gender groups (the so-called “generic masculine“). The main question of the present article is whether this generalization also holds for the first elements of nominal compounds (e.g., Lehrer- in Lehrerzimmer ‘teachers’ room’) and for inherently masculine nouns from which no feminine form can be derived (e.g., Gast which is inherently masculine). The empirical data discussed suggest that generic masculine forms used as the first elements of N-N compounds as well as inherently masculine nouns are interpreted rather generically, i.e., not as referring to male individuals only.

Literatur

Braun, Friederike, Anja Gottburgsen, Sabine Sczesny & Dagmar Stahlberg. 1998. Können Geophysiker Frauen sein? Generische Personenbezeichnungen im Deutschen. Zeitschrift für germanistische Linguistik 26. 265–283. https: //doi.org/10.1515/zfgl.1998.26.3.265.10.1515/zfgl.1998.26.3.265Search in Google Scholar

Brendel, Caren & Agnes Speck. 2021. Mitgemacht statt mitgedacht – Grundlagen und praktische Anwendungen einer „Sprache für alle“. Begleittext zum Online Tutorial „Eine Sprache für alle“ für einen inklusiven Sprachgebrauch. Online: https://www.uni-heidelberg.de/md/gsb/begleittext_zum_onlinetraining__gendergerechte_sprache_.pdf, zuletzt aufgerufen am 28.04.2022.Search in Google Scholar

Christensen, Rune Haubo Bojesen. 2019. ordinal—Regression models for ordinal data. R package version 2019.12–10. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ordinal.Search in Google Scholar

De Backer, Maarten & Ludovic De Cuypere. 2012. The interpretation of masculine personal nouns in German and Dutch: A comparative experimental study. Language Sciences 34(3). 253–268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2011.10.001.10.1016/j.langsci.2011.10.001Search in Google Scholar

Diewald, Gabriele & Anja Steinhauer. 2017. Richtig gendern: Wie Sie angemessen und verständlich schreiben. Berlin: Dudenverlag.Search in Google Scholar

Doleschal, Ursula. 1992. Movierung im Deutschen: eine Darstellung der Bildung und Verwendung weiblicher Personenbezeichnungen. Unterschleissheim/München: Lincom.Search in Google Scholar

von Fintel, Kai 2004. Would you believe it? The King of France is back! (Presuppositions and truth-value intuitions). In Marga Reimer & Anne Bezuidenhout (Hrsg.), Descriptions and Beyond, 315–341. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oso/9780199270514.003.009Search in Google Scholar

Friedrich, Marcus CG, Veronika Drößler, Nicole Oberlehberg & Elke Heise. 2021. The Influence of the Gender Asterisk („Gendersternchen“) on Comprehensibility and Interest. Frontiers in Psychology 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.760062.10.3389/fpsyg.2021.760062Search in Google Scholar

Gabriel, Ute, Pascal Gygax, Oriane Sarrasin, Alan Garnham & Jane Oakhill. 2008. Au pairs are rarely male: Norms on the gender perception of role names across English, French, and German. Behavior Research Methods 40(1). 206–212. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.1.206.10.3758/BRM.40.1.206Search in Google Scholar

Gabriel, Ute, Pascal M. Gygax & Elisabeth A. Kuhn. 2018. Neutralising linguistic sexism: Promising but cumbersome? Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 21(5). 844–858. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430218771742.10.1177/1368430218771742Search in Google Scholar

Gabriel, Ute & Franziska Mellenberger. 2004. Exchanging the generic masculine for gender-balanced forms – The impact of context valence. Swiss Journal of Psychology/Schweizerische Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Revue Suisse de Psychologie 63(4). 273. https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185.63.4.273.10.1024/1421-0185.63.4.273Search in Google Scholar

Gygax, Pascal, Ute Gabriel, Oriane Sarrasin, Jane Oakhill & Alan Garnham. 2008. Generically intended, but specifically interpreted: When beauticians, musicians, and mechanics are all men. Language and Cognitive Processes 23(3). 464–485. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960701702035.10.1080/01690960701702035Search in Google Scholar

Hansen, Karolina, Cindy Littwitz & Sabine Sczesny. 2016. The social perception of heroes and murderers: Effects of gender-inclusive language in media reports. Frontiers in psychology 7. 369. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00369.10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00369Search in Google Scholar

Harnisch, Rüdiger. 2016. Das generische Maskulinum schleicht zurück. In Andreas Bittner & Constanze Spieß (Hrsg.), Formen und Funktionen, 159–174. Berlin & Boston: De Gruyter. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110478976010.10.1515/9783110478976-010Search in Google Scholar

Heise, Elke. 2000. Sind Frauen mitgemeint? Eine empirische Untersuchung zum Verständnis des generischen Maskulinums und seiner Alternativen. Sprache & Kognition 19. https://doi.org/10.1024/0253-4533.19.12.3.10.1024//0253-4533.19.12.3Search in Google Scholar

Hornscheidt, Lann & Ja’n Sammla. 2021. Wie schreibe ich divers? Wie schreibe ich gendergerecht? Ein Praxis-Handbuch zu Gender und Sprache. Hiddensee: w_orte und meer.Search in Google Scholar

Horvath, Lisa K., Elisa F. Merkel, Anne Maass & Sabine Sczesny. 2016. Does gender-fair language pay off? The social perception of professions from a cross-linguistic perspective. Frontiers in Psychology 6. 2018. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02018.10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02018Search in Google Scholar

Irmen, Lisa & Nadja Roßberg. 2004. Gender markedness of language: The impact of grammatical and nonlinguistic information on the mental representation of person information. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 23(3). 272–307. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X04266810.10.1177/0261927X04266810Search in Google Scholar

Körner, Anita, Bleen Abraham, Ralf Rummer & Fritz Strack. 2022. Gender representations elicited by the gender star form. Journal of Language and Social Psychology. 0261927X221080181. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X221080181.10.1177/0261927X221080181Search in Google Scholar

Kotthoff, Helga. 2020. Gender-Sternchen, Binnen-I oder generisches Maskulinum, ... (Akademische) Textstile der Personenreferenz als Registrierungen? Linguistik Online 103(3). 105–127. https://doi.org/10.13092/lo.103.7181.10.13092/lo.103.7181Search in Google Scholar

Kotthoff, Helga. 2022. Zwischen berechtigtem Anliegen und bedenklicher Symbolpolitik. Aus Politik und Zeitgeschichte 72. 12–13.Search in Google Scholar

Kotthoff, Helga & Damaris Nübling. 2018. Genderlinguistik. Eine Einführung in Sprache, Gespräch und Geschlecht. Tübingen: Narr Francke Attempto.Search in Google Scholar

Lenth, Russell V. 2016. Least-Squares Means: The R Package lsmeans. Journal of Statistical Software 69(1). 1–33. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v069.i01.10.18637/jss.v069.i01Search in Google Scholar

Müller-Spitzer, Carolin. 2022. Zumutung, Herausforderung, Notwendigkeit? Zum Stand der Forschung zu geschlechtergerechter Sprache. Aus Politik und Zeitgeschichte 72. 23–29.Search in Google Scholar

Nübling, Damaris & Renata Szczepaniak. 2008. On the way from morphology to phonology: German linking elements and the role of the phonological word. Morphology 18(1). 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-008-9120-7. 10.1007/s11525-008-9120-7Search in Google Scholar

R Core Team. 2020. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria. https://www.Rproject.org/.Search in Google Scholar

Sczesny, Sabine, Magda Formanowicz & Franziska Moser. 2016. Can genderfair language reduce gender stereotyping and discrimination? Frontiers in Psychology 7. 25. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00025.10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00025Search in Google Scholar

Sczesny, Sabine, Franziska Moser & Wendy Wood. 2015. Beyond sexist beliefs: How do people decide to use gender-inclusive language? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 41(7). 943–954. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167215585727.10.1177/0146167215585727Search in Google Scholar

Shanon, Benny. 1976. On the two kinds of presuppositions in natural language. Foundations of Language 14(2). 247–249.Search in Google Scholar

Stahlberg, Dagmar, Friederike Braun, Lisa Irmen & Sabine Sczesny. 2007. Representation of the sexes in language. In Klaus Fiedler (Hrsg.), Social communication, 163–187. New York & Hove: Psychology Press.Search in Google Scholar

Stahlberg, Dagmar & Sabine Sczesny. 2001. Effekte des generischen Maskulinums und alternativer Sprachformen auf den gedanklichen Einbezug von Frauen. Psychologische Rundschau 52. 131–140.10.1026//0033-3042.52.3.131Search in Google Scholar

Stahlberg, Dagmar, Sabine Sczesny & Friederike Braun. 2001. Name your favorite musician: Effects of masculine generics and of their alternatives in German. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 20(4). 464–469. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X01020004004.10.1177/0261927X01020004004Search in Google Scholar

Tonhauser, Judith, David Beaver, Craige Roberts & Mandy Simons. 2013. Toward a taxonomy of projective content. Language 89(1). 66–109.10.1353/lan.2013.0001Search in Google Scholar

Zifonun, Gisela. 2018. Die demokratische Pflicht und das Sprachsystem: Erneute Diskussion um einen geschlechtergerechten Sprachgebrauch. Sprachreport 34(4). 44–56.Search in Google Scholar

Online erschienen: 2023-12-05
Erschienen im Druck: 2023-11-30

© 2023 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 11.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/zgl-2023-2022/html
Scroll to top button