Home Playing by/with the rules: Creativity in language, games, and art
Article
Licensed
Unlicensed Requires Authentication

Playing by/with the rules: Creativity in language, games, and art

  • Bert Cappelle

    Bert Cappelle is a Senior Lecturer of English Linguistics at the University of Lille. He has a penchant for paradoxes, both real and apparent ones.

    EMAIL logo
Published/Copyright: July 4, 2020
Become an author with De Gruyter Brill

Abstract

Bergs and Kompa (Creativity within and outside the linguistic system. Cognitive Semiotics 13. 1, 2020) discuss creativity in language, which they see as largely rule-bound, as opposed to ‘true,’ rule-breaking creativity in the arts. However, the distinction between intra- and extra-system creativity is not always easy to make. Languages have evolved into efficient systems for communication and occasionally allow for divergences of their own norms, within limits of comprehensibility, just like games are developed to enable maximum player creativity or even rule bending (as in the case of Monopoly Cheaters Edition). The paradox of systems licencing violations of their own norms and rules is similar to the one underlying avant-garde as one or more movements in the history of art, when breaking with expectations was the vogue of the time — hence, somehow to be expected. Judging art as innovative or not also depends on what we adopt as our artistic frame of reference. Furthermore, single works of art or single artists can be ahead of the times in some respects but not in others. Turning again to language, I agree with Bergs and Kompa that competent speakers abide by the rules, which implies such language users also know (perhaps not always fully consciously) how to exploit in-built mechanisms that make them sound creative.


Corresponding author: Bert Cappelle, University of Lille, Lille, France, E-mail:

Special Issue: Construction Grammar and Creativity edited by Thomas Hoffmann.


About the author

Bert Cappelle

Bert Cappelle is a Senior Lecturer of English Linguistics at the University of Lille. He has a penchant for paradoxes, both real and apparent ones.

References

Adger, David. 2019. Language unlimited: The science behind our most creative power. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Audring, Jenny & Geert Booij. 2016. Cooperation and coercion. Linguistics 54(4). 617–637. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling-2016-0012.Search in Google Scholar

Bergs, Alexander. 2016. Under pressure: Norms, rules, and coercion in linguistic analyses and literary readings. In Michael Burke & Emily Troscianko (eds.), Cognitive literary science: Dialogues between literature and cognition, 279–302. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190496869.003.0015Search in Google Scholar

Bergs, Alexander & Nikola Kompa. 2020. Creativity within and outside the linguistic system. Cognitive Semiotics 13. 1.10.1515/cogsem-2020-2025Search in Google Scholar

Burgard, Timothy Anglin. 1991. Picasso and appropriation. The Art Bulletin 73(3). 479–494. https://doi.org/10.1080/00043079.1991.10786770.Search in Google Scholar

Clark, Herbert H. & Susan E. Brennan. 1991. Grounding in communication. In Lauren B. Resnick, John M. Levine & Stephanie D. Teasley (eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition, 127–149. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.10.1037/10096-006Search in Google Scholar

Clark, Eve V. & Herbert H. Clark. 1979. When nouns surface as verbs. Language 55(4). 767–781. https://doi.org/10.2307/412745.Search in Google Scholar

Eitelmann, Matthias & Dagmar Haumann. 2019. Extravagant morphology. Description of workshop held at SLE2019. https://sle2019.eu/downloads/workshops/WS%206%20Extravagant%20morphology.pdf. (accessed 1 November 2019).10.22233/20412495.1119.1Search in Google Scholar

Hawkins, John A. 2004. Efficiency and complexity in grammars. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199252695.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Keller, Rudi. 1994. On language change: The invisible hand in language. London: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Lauwers, Peter & Dominique Willems. 2011. Coercion: Definition and challenges, current approaches and new trends. Linguistics 49(6). 1219–1235. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.2011.034.Search in Google Scholar

Leclercq, Benoît. 2019. On the semantics-pragmatics interface: A theoretical bridge between construction grammar and relevance theory. University of Lille dissertation.Search in Google Scholar

Michaelis, Laura Adrienne. 2004. Type-shifting in construction grammar: An integrated model of aspectual coercion. Cognitive Linguistics 15(1). 1–67.10.1515/cogl.2004.001Search in Google Scholar

Norde, Muriel & Sarah Sippach. 2019. Nerdalicious scientainment: A network analysis of English libfixes. Word Structure 12(3). 353–384. https://doi.org/10.3366/word.2019.0153.Search in Google Scholar

Stalnaker, Robert C. 2002. Common ground. Linguistics and Philosophy 25(5–6). 701–721. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020867916902.10.1023/A:1020867916902Search in Google Scholar

Ziegler, Debra. 2007. A word of caution on coercion. Journal of Pragmatics 39(5). 990–1028. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2006.07.014.Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2020-07-04

© 2020 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 24.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/cogsem-2020-2026/html
Scroll to top button