Abstract
This paper explores fronted verb phrases in German, drawing attention to the difference between passive/unaccusative VPs and fronted agentive vPs. While both kinds of verb phrases have been discussed in the literature as being frontable, it has been largely overlooked that fronted vPs typically come with a certain kind of post-fronting context and a rise-fall or bridge-contour intonation, which is characteristic of I-topicalization. We observe that, unlike VPs, agentive vPs essentially need to be I-topics, with a high tone at the right edge of the fronted domain, in order to be frontable. Given the special context required for fronted vPs, the situation described by the vP does not contain new information but must already have been under discussion and is now being commented on. We present the results of two experimental studies and appeal to the thetic/categorical distinction to offer a new angle on the definiteness effect that has been associated with fronted verb phrases. We propose that a subject-containing fronted vP is associated with a thetic rather than the default categorical judgment, which means that the fronted subject and predicate form only one information-structural unit (a topic) rather than two (topic and comment). Contributing to the literature on theticity, we observe that, unlike in non-fronting thetic statements, the subject in fronted vPs cannot be a true definite. We attribute this to clashing intonation restrictions on theticity in non-fronting constructions versus theticity in just the fronted portion of a sentence.
Acknowledgements
Over the years, we have had many helpful discussions (both in person and via email) regarding the content of this paper and would like to thank the following people: Jorge Hankamer, Susi Wurmbrand, Dan Grodner, Michael Covington, Dennis Ott, Nathan Sanders, Ann Reed, Bill Ladusaw, Peter Culicover, and our two anonymous TLR reviewers. Thanks also to the audiences of our talks at the annual meeting of the LSA in Portland in 2012, at the annual meeting of the DGfS in Frankfurt in 2012, and at the organized session “The Parameters of VP-fronting” at the LSA in Austin in 2017. Any remaining errors are our own, of course.
Appendix 1: Stimulus-norming study: Judgments on plausibility, conventionality, and specificity
used to choose stimuli for the past-participle vP and and ACI vP studies
participants: 26 native speakers of German, mostly living in the US and “Spielgruppler” from a German playgroup in Narberth, PA
materials: 24 non-fronting sentences, describing situations involving an agentive subject and a transitive predicate, with 4 variants each;
a. | Katzen | jagen | Mäuse. | (plausible, conventional, nonspecific) |
cats | chase | mice |
b. | Unsere | Katzen | jagen | Mäuse. (plausible, specific) |
our | cats | chase | mice |
c. | Raubkatzen | jagen | Mäuse. | (plausible, unconventional, nonspecific) |
prey-cats | chase | mice |
d. | Hühner | jagen | Mäuse. | (implausible, unconventional, nonspecific) |
chicken | chase | mice |
a. | Boxer | schlagen | ihre | Gegner k.o. |
boxers | beat | their | opponents knocked-out |
(‘knock out their opponents.’) |
b. | Die | Klitschko | Brüder | schlagen | ihre | Gegner k.o. |
the | Klitschko | brothers | beat | their | opponents knocked-out |
c. | Eishockeyspieler | schlagen | ihre | Gegner k.o. | ||
ice hockey players | beat | their | opponents knocked-out |
b. | Friedensprediger | schlagen | ihre | Gegner k.o. | ||
pacifists | beat | their | opponents knocked-out |
– key conditions were conventional vs. unconventional and specific vs. nonspecific, but conventionality was also compared to plausibility (to see if something that is unconventional could still be plausible)
– 4 presentation lists using balanced Latin square design; each participant saw only one list
procedure: Lime Survey (www.limesurvey.org); participants were asked to answer 3 questions about each sentence on their list, using a scale from 1-5:
How plausible is the described situation? (1: least, 5: most plausible)
How conventional is the described situation? (1: least, 5: most conventional)
How specific is the subject of the sentence? (1: least, 5: most specific)
results:
overall, participants judged the nonspecific condition as less specific, the implausible condition as less plausible, and the unconventional condition as less conventional
trend for plausibility and conventionality to influence each other, but there was still a reliable difference between the conditions
overall, specificity was judged as expected, although some indefinite plurals were judged as surprisingly specific
Appendix 2: Past Participle vP Study
participants: 30 native speakers of German, interviewed in Hannover, Germany
materials: 12 sentences with transitive vPs, again using the content of the non-fronting sentences from Survey 1 of Experiment 1, with 6 variants each:
a. | Katzen | Mäuse | gejagt | haben | hier | schon | oft. |
cats | mice | chased | have | here | already | often |
(fronted, nonspecific, conventional) |
b. | Unsere | Katzen | Mäuse | gejagt | haben | hier | schon | oft. |
our | cats | mice | chased | have | here | already | often |
(fronted, specific) |
c. | Raubkatzen | Mäuse | gejagt | haben | hier | schon | oft. |
prey-cates | mice | chased | have | here | already | often |
(fronted, nonspec., unconventional) |
d. | Katzen | haben | hier | schon | oft | Mäuse | gejagt. |
cats | have | here | already | often | mice | chased |
(nonfronted, nonspec., conventional) |
e. | Unsere | Katzen | haben | hier | schon | oft | Mäuse | gejagt. |
our | cats | have | here | already | often | mice | chased |
(nonfronted, specific) |
f. | Raubkatzen | haben | hier | schon | oft | Mäuse | gejagt. |
prey-cates | have | here | already | often | mice | chased |
(nonfronted, nonspec., unconv.) |
a. | Boxer | ihre | Gegner | k.o. geschlagen | haben | hier | schon | oft. |
boxer | their | opponents | knocked-out | have | here | already | often |
b. | Die | Klitschko | Brüder | ihre | Gegner | k.o. geschlagen | haben |
the | Klitschko | brothers | their | opponents | knocked-out | have |
hier | schon | oft. | ||
here | already | often |
c. | Eishockeyspieler | ihre | Gegner | k.o. geschlagen | haben | hier |
ice-hockey-players | their | opponents | knocked-out | have | here |
schon | oft. | |
already | often |
d. | Boxer | haben | hier | schon | oft | ihre | Gegner | k.o. geschlagen. |
boxer | have | here | already | often | their | opponents | knocked-out |
e. | Die | Klitschko | Brüder | haben | hier | schon | oft | ihre |
the | Klitschko | brothers | have | here | already | often | their |
Gegner | k.o. geschlagen. | ||
opponents | knocked-out |
f. | Eishockeyspieler | haben | hier | schon | oft | ihre |
ice-hockey-players | have | here | already | often | their |
Gegner | k.o. geschlagen. | ||||
opponents | knocked-out |
2X3 design, crossing fronting type (fronted vs. nonfronted) with proposition type (conventional-nonspecific vs. conventional-specific vs. unconventional-nonspecific)
6 presentation lists using balanced Latin square design; 12 target sentences and 44 fillers with various degrees of grammaticality; pseudorandom presentation order; each participant saw only one list
procedure: participants were interviewed one-on-one, were read the sentences, and were asked to give grammaticality judgments, using a scale from 1–5 (see Section 4)
results:
nonfronted sentences were consistently evaluated with a score of 1 or 2, i.e. as immediately acceptable or acceptable in appropriate context, so they were disregarded for the analysis
score means:
fronting, nonspecific, conventional (F, NS, C): 3.25; SD: 1.144
fronting, nonspecific, unconventional (F, NS, UC): 3.68; SD: 1.081
fronting, specific, conventional (F, S, C): 4.15; SD: 0.86
RMSE=1.036; F=11.33; p<0.0001
comparison between proposition types:
difference between F, S, C and F, NS, UC: 0.467; 95% CI: 0.0935, 0.8399
difference between F, S, C and F, NS, C: 0.9; 95% CI: 0.5268, 1.2732
difference between F, NS, NC and F, NS, C: 0.433; 95% CI: 0.0601, 0.8065
=> F, S, C (fronted vP with specific subject) is significantly worse than either of the other two proposition types (fronted vP with nonspecific subject, regardless of conventionality). In addition, F, NS, UC (fronted vP with nonspecific subject and unconventional content) is significantly worse than F, NS, C (fronted vP with unspecific subject and conventional content).
ACI vP Study
participants: 30 native speakers of German, again mostly “Spielgruppler” from the German play group in Narberth, PA
materials: 18 sentences with transitive (agentive vP) AcI-constructions, using the content of the non-fronting sentences from Survey 1, with 6 variants each:
a. | Katzen | Mäuse | jagen | sieht | man | hier | oft. |
cats.acc | mice.acc | chase | sees | one.nom | here | often |
(fronted, nonspecific, conventional) |
b. | Unsere | Katzen | Mäuse | jagen | sieht | man | hier | oft. |
our.acc | cats | mice.acc | chase | sees | one.nom | here | often |
(fronted, specific) |
c. | Raubkatzen | Mäuse | jagen | sieht | man | hier | oft. |
prey-cats.acc | mice.acc | chase | sees | one.nom | here | often |
(fronted, nonspec., unconventional) |
d. | Man | sieht | hier | oft | Katzen | Mäuse | jagen. |
man.nom | sees | here | often | cats.acc | mice.acc | chase |
(nonfronted, nonspec., conventional) |
e. | Man | sieht | hier | oft | unsere | Katzen | Mäuse | jagen. |
one.nom | sees | here | often | our.acc | cats | mice.acc | chase |
(nonfronted, specific) |
f. | Man | sieht | hier | oft | Raubkatzen | Mäuse | jagen. |
one.nom | sees | here | often | prey-cats.acc | mice.acc | chase |
(nonfronted, nonspec., unconventional) |
Boxer ihre Gegner k.o. schalgen sieht man hier oft.
Die Klitschko Brüder ihre Gegner k.o. schlagen sieht man hier oft.
Eishockeyspieler ihre Gegner k.o. schlagen sieht man hier oft.
Man sieht hier oft Boxer ihre Gegner k.o. schlagen.
Man sieht hier oft die Klitschko Brüder ihre Gegner k.o. schlagen.
Man sieht hier oft Eishockeyspieler ihre Gegner k.o. schlagen.
2X3 design, crossing fronting type (fronted vs. nonfronted) with proposition type (conventional-nonspecific vs. conventional-specific vs. unconventional-nonspecific)
6 presentation lists using balanced Latin square design; 18 target sentences and 44 fillers with various degrees of grammaticality; pseudorandom presentation order; each participant saw only one list
Appendix 3: Combined analysis of past participle vP and ACI vP studies
A linear effects model with a random effect of subject was run with fixed factors vP type, fronting, and sentence type, where vP type was evaluated across subjects and fronting and sentence type were evaluated within subjects.
Numerator df | Denominator df | F | Significance | |
Intercept | 1 | 58.813 | 1030.766 | <0.001 |
vP type | 1 | 58.813 | 16.818 | <0.001 |
Fronting | 1 | 829.744 | 1362.392 | <0.001 |
Sentence type | 2 | 829.744 | 22.523 | <0.001 |
vP type*fronting | 1 | 829.744 | 195.950 | <0.001 |
vP type*S type | 2 | 829.744 | 0.317 | 0.729 |
Fronting*S type | 2 | 829.744 | 11.815 | <0.001 |
vP type*fronting*S type | 2 | 829.744 | 2.183 | 0.113 |
References
Abels, Klaus. 2003. Successive cyclicity, anti-locality, and adposition stranding. Storrs, CT: University of Connecticut dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Allerton, David J. & Alan Cruttenden. 1979. Three reasons for accenting a definite subject. Journal of Linguistics 15(1). 49–53.10.1017/S0022226700013104Search in Google Scholar
Beckman, Mary & Janet Pierrehumbert. 1986. Intonational structure in English and Japanese. Phonology Yearbook 3. 255–310.10.1017/S095267570000066XSearch in Google Scholar
Behagel, Otto. 1909. Beziehungen zwischen Umfang und Reihenfolge von Satzgliedern. Indogermanische Forschungen 25. 110–142.Search in Google Scholar
Brentano, Franz. 1973. Psychology from an empirical standpoint. Translated by Antos C. Rancuello, D. B. Terrell & Linda L. McAlister from Psychologie vom empirischen Standpunkt [1874, 1924]. London and New York: Routledge.10.28937/978-3-7873-2608-2Search in Google Scholar
Büring, Daniel. 1997. The meaning of topic and focus: The 59th street bridge accent. London: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar
Büring, Daniel. 2003. On D-trees, beans, and B-accents. Linguistics & Philosophy 26(5). 511–545.10.1023/A:1025887707652Search in Google Scholar
Büring, Daniel. 2006. Focus projection and default prominence. In V. Molnár & S. Winkler (eds.), The architecture of focus, 321–346. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110922011.321Search in Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2000. Minimalist inquiries: The framework. In R. Martin, D. Michaels & J. Uriagereka (eds.), Step by step: Essays on minimalist syntax in honor of Howard Lasnik, 89–156. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2001. Derivation by phase. In M. Kenstowicz (ed.), Ken Hale, A life in language, 1–51. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar
Culicover, Peter & Susanne Winkler. To appear. In Verner Egeland, Valeria Molnar & Susanne Winkler (eds.), The Architecture of Topic. Presented at The Architecture of Topic conference. Lund, December 2014.Search in Google Scholar
De Kuthy, Kordula & W. Detmar Meurers. 2003. The secret life of focus exponents, and what it tells us about fronted verbal projections. In S. Müller (ed.), Proceedings of the HPSG03 Conference. CSLI Publications.10.21248/hpsg.2003.6Search in Google Scholar
Diesing, Molly. 1996. Semantic variables and object shift. In H. Thráinsson, S. Epstein & S. Peter (eds.), Studies in comparative Germanic syntax II, 66–84. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishing.10.1007/978-94-010-9806-9_3Search in Google Scholar
Drubig, Hans Bernhard. 1992. Zur Frage der grammatischen Repräsentation thetischer und kategorischer Sätze. In J. Jacobs (ed.), Informationsstruktur und Grammatik (Linguistische Berichte Sonderheft 4), 142–195. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag.10.1007/978-3-663-12176-3_6Search in Google Scholar
Erteschik-Shir, Nomi. 2007. Information structure: The syntax-discourse interface. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Féry, Caroline. 1993. German Intonational patterns. Tübingen: Niemeyer.10.1515/9783111677606Search in Google Scholar
Féry, Caroline. 2007. The prosody of topicalization. In K. Schwabe & S. Winkler (eds.), On information structure, meaning and form: Generalizations across languages, 69–86. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/la.100.06ferSearch in Google Scholar
Francis, Elaine. 2010. Grammatical weight and relative clause extraposition in English. Cognitive Linguistics 21(1). 35–74.10.1515/cogl.2010.002Search in Google Scholar
Frascarelli, Mara & Hinterhölzl. Roland. 2007. Types of topics in German and Italian. In K. Schwabe & S. Winkler (eds.), On information structure, meaning and form: Generalizations across languages, 87–116. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/la.100.07fraSearch in Google Scholar
Frey, Werner. 2006. Contrast and movement to the German prefield. In V. Molnár & S. Winkler (eds.), The architecture of focus, 235–264. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.10.1515/9783110922011.235Search in Google Scholar
Frey, Werner. 2015. NP-incorporation in German. In Olga Borik & Berit Gehrke (eds.), The syntax and semantics of pseudo-incorporation, 225–261. Leiden: Brill.10.1163/9789004291089_008Search in Google Scholar
Gibson, Edward. 1998. Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition 68. 1–76.10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00034-1Search in Google Scholar
Grewendorf. 1989. Ergativity in German. Dordrecht: Foris.10.1515/9783110859256Search in Google Scholar
Grice, Martine, Stefan Baumann & Benzmüller. Ralf. 2005. German intonation in autosegmental-metrical phonology. In Sun-Ah Jun (ed.), Prosodic typology: The phonology and intonation and phrasing, 55–83. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199249633.003.0003Search in Google Scholar
Grodner, Daniel & Edward Gibson. 2005. Consequences of the serial nature of linguistic input for sentential complexity. Cognitive Science 29. 261–290.10.1207/s15516709cog0000_7Search in Google Scholar
Haider, Hubert. 1990. Topicalization and other puzzles of German syntax. In G. Grewendorf & W. Sternefeld (eds.), Scrambling and Barriers, 93–112. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/la.5.06haiSearch in Google Scholar
Haider, Hubert. 2006. Mittelfeld phenomena. In M. Everaert & H. van Riemsdijk (eds.), The syntax companion (SynCom) Vol. 3. 204–274. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar
Hale, Kenneth & Samuel Keyser. 1993. On argument structure and the lexical expression of syntactic relations. In K. Hale & S. Keyser (eds.), View from building20, 53–109. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar
Hawkins, John. 1994. A performance theory of order and constituency. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray. 1972. Semantics in generative grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar
Jacobs, Joachim. 1997. I-Topikalisierung. Linguistische Berichte 168. 91–133.Search in Google Scholar
Kratzer, Angelika. 1984. On deriving differences between German and English. Berlin: Ms., Technische Universität.Search in Google Scholar
Kratzer, Angelika. 1996. Severing the external argument from its verb. In J. Rooryck & L. Zaring (eds.), Phrase structure and the lexicon, 109–137. Dordrecht: Kluwer.10.1007/978-94-015-8617-7_5Search in Google Scholar
Krifka, Manfred. 1984. Fokus, Topik, syntaktische Struktur und semantische Interpretation. Unpublished manuscript.Search in Google Scholar
Krifka, Manfred. 1998. Scope inversion under the rise-fall contour in German. Linguistic Inquiry 29. 75–112.10.1162/002438998553662Search in Google Scholar
Kuroda, Sige-Yuki. 1972. The categorical and the thetic judgments. Foundations of Language 9. 153–185.Search in Google Scholar
Ladusaw, William. 1994. Thetic and categorical, stage and individual, weak and strong. In M. Harvey & L. Santelmann (eds.), Proceedings from semantics and linguistic theory IV, 220–229. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.10.3765/salt.v4i0.2463Search in Google Scholar
McCloskey, James. 1997. Subjecthood and subject positions. In Liliane Haegeman (ed.), Elements of grammar, 197–235. Dordrecht: Kluwer.10.1007/978-94-011-5420-8_5Search in Google Scholar
McNally, Louise. 1997. A semantics for the English existential construction. New York: Garland.Search in Google Scholar
McNally, Louise. 2016. Existential. In M. Aronoff (ed.), Oxford bibliographies in linguistics, New York: Oxford University Press. Entry Launch Date: 28/4/2016. 10.1093/OBO/9780199772810-0070.Search in Google Scholar
Mehlhorn, Grit. 2001. Produktion und Perzeption von Hutkonturen im Deutschen. Linguistische Arbeitsberichte 77. 31–57.Search in Google Scholar
Milsark, Gary. 1974. Existential sentences in English. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Molnár, Valéria & Inger Rosengren. 1997. Zu Jacobs’ Explikation der I-Topikalisierung. Linguistische Berichte 169. 211–247.Search in Google Scholar
Müller, Gereon. 1998. Incomplete category fronting. A derivational approach to remnant movement in German. Dordrecht: Kluwer.10.1007/978-94-017-1864-6Search in Google Scholar
Ott, Dennis. 2010. Varieties of VP-fronting. Cambridge, MA: Ms Harvard University. lingbuzz/001024.Search in Google Scholar
Ott, Dennis. 2018. VP-fronting: Movement vs. dislocation. The Linguistic Review 35(2). 243–282.10.1515/tlr-2017-0024Search in Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet. 1980. The phonology and phonetics of English intonation. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology dissertation.Search in Google Scholar
Quirk, Randolph, Sidney Greenbaum, Geoffrey Leech & Jan Svartvik. 1985. A comprehensive grammar of the English language. London and New York: Longman.Search in Google Scholar
Rochemont, Michael. 2013. Discourse new, F-marking, and normal stress. Lingua 136. 38–62.10.1016/j.lingua.2013.07.016Search in Google Scholar
Rosengren, Inger. 1997. The thetic/categorical distinction revisited once more. Linguistics 35. 439–479.10.1515/ling.1997.35.3.439Search in Google Scholar
Sasse, Hans-Jürgen. 1987. The thetic/categorical distinction revisited. Linguistics 25. 511–580.10.1515/ling.1987.25.3.511Search in Google Scholar
Sasse, Hans-Jürgen. 1996. Theticity. Arbeitsbericht Nr. 27, Institut für Sprachwissenschaft, Universität Köln.10.1515/9783110892222.255Search in Google Scholar
Steube, Anita. 2001. Grammatik und Pragmatik von Hutkonturen. Linguistische Arbeitsberichte 77. 7–29.Search in Google Scholar
Uhmann, Susanne. 1991. Fokusphonologie. Eine Analyse deutscher Intonationskonturen im Rahmen der nicht-linearen Phonologie. Tübingen: Niemeyer.Search in Google Scholar
von Heusinger, Klaus. 2002. Specificity and definiteness in sentence and discourse structure. Journal of Semantics 19(3). 245–274.10.1093/jos/19.3.245Search in Google Scholar
Wasow, Thomas. 2002. Postverbal behavior. Stanford: CSLI Publications.Search in Google Scholar
Webelhuth, Gert. 1990. Diagnostics for structure. In G. Grewendorf & W. Sternefeld (eds.), Scrambling and barriers, 40–75. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/la.5.04webSearch in Google Scholar
Webelhuth, Gert & Hans den Besten. 1987. Remnant topicalization and the constituent structure of VP in the Germanic SOV languages. Paper presented at the Generative Linguistics of the Old World conference. Venice, 1987.Search in Google Scholar
Wunderlich, Dieter. 1991. Intonation and contrast. Journal of Semantics 8. 239–251.10.1093/jos/8.3.239Search in Google Scholar
Wurmbrand, Susi. 2001. AGREE: The other VP-internal subject hypothesis. In K. Megerdoomian & L. Bar-El (eds.), Proceedings of the 20th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics. 635–648. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Search in Google Scholar
Wurmbrand, Susi. 2004. No TP fronting meets nearly headless nick. Storrs: Ms., University of Connecticut. http://ling.auf.net/lingbuzz/000146.Search in Google Scholar
Wurmbrand, Susi. 2006. Licensing case. Journal of Germanic Linguistics 18(3). 175–236.10.1017/S1470542706000079Search in Google Scholar
Zwart, Jan-Wouter. 2007. Uncharted territory? Toward a noncartographic account of Germanic syntax. Groninger Arbeiten Zur Germanistischen Linguistik 45. 55–75.Search in Google Scholar
© 2019 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston
Articles in the same Issue
- Frontmatter
- research-article
- 3rd person clitic combinations across Catalan varieties: Consequences of the nature of the dative clitic
- Revisiting unselected embedded questions in the light of Classical Greek wh-clauses
- The syntax, information structure, and prosody of German ‘VP’-fronting
- Ellipsis resolution in Persian complex predicates
- corrigendum
- Corrigendum to: Arabic stress in strict CV, with no moras, no syllables, no feet and no extrametricality
Articles in the same Issue
- Frontmatter
- research-article
- 3rd person clitic combinations across Catalan varieties: Consequences of the nature of the dative clitic
- Revisiting unselected embedded questions in the light of Classical Greek wh-clauses
- The syntax, information structure, and prosody of German ‘VP’-fronting
- Ellipsis resolution in Persian complex predicates
- corrigendum
- Corrigendum to: Arabic stress in strict CV, with no moras, no syllables, no feet and no extrametricality