Home The normative order of sensing: enacting the tasting sheet in tasting training sessions
Article
Licensed
Unlicensed Requires Authentication

The normative order of sensing: enacting the tasting sheet in tasting training sessions

  • Lorenza Mondada

    Lorenza Mondada is professor of linguistics at the University of Basel. Her research deals with social interaction in ordinary, professional and institutional settings, within an ethnomethodological and conversation analytic perspective. Her specific focus is on video analysis and multimodality, integrating language and embodiment in the study of human action. She has published extensively in Journal of Pragmatics, Discourse Studies, Language in Society, Research on Language and Social Interaction, Journal of Sociolinguistics and hasco-edited several books (for Cambridge University Press, De Gruyter, Benjamins, Routledge), as well as developed her approach of sensoriality in the book Sensing in Social Interaction (Cambridge University Press, 2021).

    ORCID logo EMAIL logo
Published/Copyright: November 9, 2023

Abstract

Tasting sessions are a social activity in which the senses and the sensorial features of the tasted objects are the main focus of the participants, who do not only experience taste but also aim at precisely describing it. For doing that, they use pre-formatted tasting sheets and pre-existing standardized repertoires of descriptors. This paper investigates the relations between bodies and sensations, linguistic expressions and the normativity of lexical repertoires. While the literature has insisted on the sensorial lexicon of diverse languages and its specialization within expert domains, the very way in which the sensing body, language, and the normativity of standardized categories are precisely articulated in tasting practices remains understudied. Using an ethnomethodological and conversation analytic approach, this paper demonstrates how participants in training tasting sessions achieve practically, bodily and materially the association between the sample tasted, the sensing body and the use of lexical repertoires. Artefacts and tools like tasting sheets and lexical lists are situatedly mobilized in a way that enhances the senses but also socializes and standardizes them. On the basis of cheese tasting sessions video-recorded during training workshops of professional tasters in Italy and Italian-speaking Switzerland, the paper demonstrates how the normative order of sensing is achieved through the imbrication of the use of tasting sheets within the sensory examination of samples – thus showing how body, materiality, language, and artefacts are normatively constrained, practically managed and bodily aligned in the tasting session.


Corresponding author: Lorenza Mondada, Department of Linguistics, University of Basel, Maiengasse 51, CH 4056, Basel, Switzerland, E-mail:

Award Identifier / Grant number: 100012_175969

About the author

Lorenza Mondada

Lorenza Mondada is professor of linguistics at the University of Basel. Her research deals with social interaction in ordinary, professional and institutional settings, within an ethnomethodological and conversation analytic perspective. Her specific focus is on video analysis and multimodality, integrating language and embodiment in the study of human action. She has published extensively in Journal of Pragmatics, Discourse Studies, Language in Society, Research on Language and Social Interaction, Journal of Sociolinguistics and hasco-edited several books (for Cambridge University Press, De Gruyter, Benjamins, Routledge), as well as developed her approach of sensoriality in the book Sensing in Social Interaction (Cambridge University Press, 2021).

Appendix: Transcription conventions

Talk has been transcribed following Jefferson’s conventions (2004]) and embodiment following Mondada’s conventions (2018]).

A1: General features

Overview of basic transcript elements:

RIC: Speaker identification (pseudonym or category)
bea Participant doing the embodied action (identified when (s)he is not the speaker; for the speaker, the embodied annotation is placed on the lines(s) immediately following the transcribed turn, without any identification in the margin)
???: Speaker identification unsure
=> Phenomena in focus are highlighted in various ways,
-> with arrows in the left margin

Organization of multi-linear transcripts:

  1. The first line of the transcript is dedicated to the original language and presented in bold black.

  2. The second line is dedicated to translation, and is presented in italics.

  3. The third and following lines describe embodied conduct. They are in normal font.

A2: Conventions for transcribing talk

Talk is transcribed using the conventions developed by Gail Jefferson (2004).

wo[rds ] Onset and end of overlap (simultaneous talk), at the start of
[word]s an utterance or later
word= Latching (no intervening beat of silence) between TCUs or
=word between turns by different speakers
(0.6) Elapsed time (pause or silence); Length is timed relative to the delivery of the preceding talk or based on absolute measures.
(.) Micro-pause (less than 0.2 s)
no:, no: Lengthening/stretching of the sound before the colon
(: = 0.1 s)
>word word< Accelerando/rushed/compressed talk: increase in tempo relative to surrounding talk
<word word> Rallentando/stretched-out talk: slowing down in tempo relative to surrounding talk
wo- word Hyphens mark an abrupt ending through oral or glottal
´ cut-off (glottal closure) before a word is complete.
(word) Transcriber’s uncertain hearing
(war)/(wer) Transcriber’s alternative hearings
( ) Unintelligible stretch of talk
((laughs)) Description, not transcription: Additional transcriber comments and descriptions are provided in double parentheses.

A3: Conventions for transcribing embodiment

Embodied actions are transcribed using the conventions developed by Lorenza Mondada (cf. Mondada 2018). For a full version (updated 2022) and a tutorial see: https://www.lorenzamondada.net/multimodal-transcription.

* * Descriptions of embodied actions are delimited
+ + between two identical symbols (one symbol per participant) and are synchronized with corresponding stretches of talk.
∗---> The action described continues across subsequent lines until
--->∗ the same symbol is reached.
>> The action described begins before the excerpt’s beginning.
--->> The action described continues after the excerpt’s end.
.…. Action’s preparation
---- Action’s full extension is reached and maintained.
,„„ Action’s retraction
fig The exact moment at which a screen shot has been taken is
# indicated with a specific sign showing its position within turn at talk and with a figure number on a separate line.

References

Brillat-Savarin, Jean Anthelme. 1825. Physiologie du goût. Paris: Delaunay.Search in Google Scholar

Butler, Ella. 2019. Mouth work: Bodily action in sensory science. Food, Culture & Society 22(2). 224–236. https://doi.org/10.1080/15528014.2019.1573044.Search in Google Scholar

Classen, Constance. 1993. Worlds of sense: Exploring the senses in history and across cultures. London: Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Cooren, François. 2004. Textual agency: How texts do things in organizational settings. Organization 11(3). 373–393. https://doi.org/10.1177/1350508404041998.Search in Google Scholar

Cooren, François. 2009. The haunting question of textual agency: Derrida and Garfinkel on iterability and eventfulness. Research on Language and Social Interaction 42(1). 42–67. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351810802671735.Search in Google Scholar

Fele, Giolo. 2019. Olfactory objects: Recognizing, describing and assessing smells during professional tasting sessions. In Dennis Day & Johannes Wagner (eds.), Objects, bodies and work practice, 250–284. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.10.21832/9781788924535-013Search in Google Scholar

Garfinkel, Harold. 2002. Ethnomethodology’s program: Working out Durkheim’s aphorism. Lanham: Rowman and Littlefield.Search in Google Scholar

Goodwin, Charles. 2017. Co-operative action. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/9781139016735Search in Google Scholar

Hennion, Antoine. 2007. Those things that hold us together: Taste and sociology. Cultural Sociology 1(1). 97–114. https://doi.org/10.1177/1749975507073923.Search in Google Scholar

Heritage, John. 1984. A change-of-state token and aspects of its sequential placement. In Max Atkinson & John Heritage (eds.), Structures of social action, 299–345. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511665868.020Search in Google Scholar

Howes, D. 2003. Sensual relations: Engaging the senses in culture and social theory. Ann Arbor: Michigan University Press.10.3998/mpub.11852Search in Google Scholar

Jackson, Ronald S. 2017. Wine tasting. A professional handbook. London: Academic Press.Search in Google Scholar

Jefferson, Gail. 2004. Glossary of transcript symbols with an introduction. In Gene H. Lerner (ed.), Conversation analysis: Studies from the first generation, 13–31. Amsterdam: Benjamins.10.1075/pbns.125.02jefSearch in Google Scholar

Johnson, Hug. 1989. Vintage: The story of wine. New York: Simon and Schuster.Search in Google Scholar

Kissmann, Ulrike T. & Joost van Loon (eds.). 2019. Discussing new materialism. Wiesbaden: Springer.10.1007/978-3-658-22300-7Search in Google Scholar

Korsmeyer, Carolyn. 1999. Making sense of taste: Food and philosophy. New York: Cornell University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Latour, Bruno. 1996. On interobjectivity. Mind, Culture and Activity 3(4). 228–245. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327884mca0304_2.Search in Google Scholar

Latour, Bruno. 2004. How to talk about the body? The normative dimension of science studies. Body & Society 10(2-3). 205–229. https://doi.org/10.1177/1357034x04042943.Search in Google Scholar

Lawless, Lydia J. R. & Gail V. Civille. 2013. Developing lexicons. Journal of Sensory Studies 28. 270–281. https://doi.org/10.1111/joss.12050.Search in Google Scholar

Lehrer, Adrienne. 1983 [2009]. Wine and conversation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Liberman, Ken. 2022. The taste of coffee. New York: SUNY.10.1515/9781438488981Search in Google Scholar

Majid, Asifa & Niclas Burenhuit. 2014. Odors are expressible in language, as long as you speak the right language. Cognition 130(2). 266–270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.004.Search in Google Scholar

Mondada, Lorenza. 2018. Multiple temporalities of language and body in interaction: Challenges for transcribing multimodality. Research on Language and Social Interaction 51(1). 85–106. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2018.1413878.Search in Google Scholar

Mondada, Lorenza. 2019. Rethinking bodies and objects in social interaction: A multimodal and multisensorial approach to tasting. In Ulrike Tikvah Kissmann & Joost van Loon (eds.), Discussing new materialism, 109–134. Berlin: Springer.10.1007/978-3-658-22300-7_6Search in Google Scholar

Mondada, Lorenza. 2020a. Audible sniffs: Smelling-in-interaction. Research on Language and Social Interaction 53(1). 140–163. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2020.1716592.Search in Google Scholar

Mondada, Lorenza. 2020b. Orchestrating multi-sensoriality in tasting sessions: Sensing bodies, normativity, and language. Symbolic Interaction 44. 63–86. https://doi.org/10.1002/symb.472.Search in Google Scholar

Mondada, Lorenza. 2021. Sensing in social interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/9781108650090Search in Google Scholar

Mondada, Lorenza. 2023. Demonstrating and guiding how to smell in tasting sessions: .nhHHHhh and the audible-visible production of sensorial intersubjectivity. Language & Communication 88. 111–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langcom.2022.11.006.Search in Google Scholar

Mondada, Lorenza. in press. The semantics of taste in interaction. Journal of Interactional Linguistics.Search in Google Scholar

Mondada, Lorenza & Giolo Fele. 2020. Descrittori visivi per l’assaggio professionale: Lessico, sensorialità e standardizzazione. Rivista Italiana di Linguistica Applicata XLIX(3). 651–681.Search in Google Scholar

Mondada, Lorenza & Kimmo Svinhufvud. 2016. Writing-in-interaction: Studying writing as a multimodal phenomenon in social interaction. Language and Dialogue 6(1). 1–53. https://doi.org/10.1075/ld.6.1.01mon.Search in Google Scholar

Muniesa, Fabian & Anne-Sophie Trébuchet-Breitwiller. 2010. Becoming a measuring instrument. Journal of Cultural Economy 3(3). 321–337. https://doi.org/10.1080/17530350.2010.506318.Search in Google Scholar

Nevile, Maurice, Pentti Haddington, Trine Heinemann & Mirka Rauniomaa (eds.). 2014. Interacting with objects: Language, materiality, and social activity. Amsterdam: Benjamins.10.1075/z.186Search in Google Scholar

Noble, Ann C., R. A. Arnold, B. M. Masuda, S. D. Pecore, J. O. Schmidt & P. M. Stern. 1984. Progress towards a standardized system of wine aroma terminology. American Journal of Enology and Viticulture 35. 107–109. https://doi.org/10.5344/ajev.1984.35.2.107.Search in Google Scholar

Peynaud, Emile. 1987. The taste of wine. London: MacDonald.Search in Google Scholar

Schegloff, Emanuel A. & Harvey Sacks. 1973. Opening up closings. Semiotica 8(4). 289–327. https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.1973.8.4.289.Search in Google Scholar

Silverstein, Michael. 2006. Old wine, new ethnographic lexicography. Annual Review of Anthropology 35(1). 481–496. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.35.081705.123327.Search in Google Scholar

Sorabji, Richard. 1971. Aristotle on demarcating the five senses. Philosophical Review 80(1). 55–79. https://doi.org/10.2307/2184311.Search in Google Scholar

Streeck, Jürgen, Charles Goodwin & Curtis LeBaron (eds.). 2011. Embodied interaction: Language and body in the material world. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Tekin, Burak. in press. Disciplined bodies.Search in Google Scholar

Vedel, André. 1972. Essai sur la dégustation des vins: Propositions pour servir de base à une méthodologie et un vocabulaire unifiés. Macon: SEIV.Search in Google Scholar

Wiggins, Sally & Jonathan Potter. 2003. Attitudes and evaluative practices: Category vs. item and subjective vs. objective constructions in everyday food assessments. British Journal of Social Psychology 42(4). 513–531. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466603322595257.Search in Google Scholar

Received: 2023-01-05
Accepted: 2023-10-18
Published Online: 2023-11-09
Published in Print: 2025-01-29

© 2023 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 12.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/text-2022-0195/pdf
Scroll to top button