Home Monitoring the swimzone while finding south: sustained orientation in multiactivity among beach lifeguards
Article
Licensed
Unlicensed Requires Authentication

Monitoring the swimzone while finding south: sustained orientation in multiactivity among beach lifeguards

  • Simon Harrison

    Simon Harrison received his PhD in English Studies from the Université de Bordeaux III and is currently Assistant Professor of Applied Linguistics at the University of Nottingham Ningbo China. His research interests include gestural forms associated with negation and, more broadly, gestural recurrency in discourse. He is author of the forthcoming book The Impulse to Gesture: Where Language, Minds, and Bodies Intersect (Cambridge University Press).

    EMAIL logo
    and Robert F. Williams

    Robert F. Williams received his PhD in Cognitive Science from the University of California, San Diego, and is currently Associate Dean of the Faculty and Associate Professor of Education at Lawrence University. His research examines how people construct meaning in instructional situations, in collaborative problem-solving, and in creative activity, focusing on the role of gesture in building shared understandings.

Published/Copyright: September 19, 2017

Abstract

Lifeguards stationed opposite their swimzone on a beach in southwest France huddle around a diagram in the sand; the Head Lifeguard points to the sun then looks at the swimzone. What is going on here? Our paper examines two excerpts from this interaction to explore how lifeguards manage an instruction activity that arises in addition to the task of monitoring the swimzone. Building on frame analysis and multiactivity in social interaction, we focus on the role of gaze behavior in maintaining a sustained orientation to the swimzone as a distinct activity in this setting. Multimodal, sequential analyses of extracts from the video data show that orientation to the lifeguarding task is sustained primarily by body orientation and gaze patterns that routinely return to the swimzone. This is supported when sustained orientation away from the swimzone leads to the momentary suspension of the instruction activity and consequent re-organization of the interaction, illustrating the normative and visible nature of managing multiactivity. These gaze behaviors and interactive patterns constitute practices of professional vision among beach lifeguards.

About the authors

Simon Harrison

Simon Harrison received his PhD in English Studies from the Université de Bordeaux III and is currently Assistant Professor of Applied Linguistics at the University of Nottingham Ningbo China. His research interests include gestural forms associated with negation and, more broadly, gestural recurrency in discourse. He is author of the forthcoming book The Impulse to Gesture: Where Language, Minds, and Bodies Intersect (Cambridge University Press).

Robert F. Williams

Robert F. Williams received his PhD in Cognitive Science from the University of California, San Diego, and is currently Associate Dean of the Faculty and Associate Professor of Education at Lawrence University. His research examines how people construct meaning in instructional situations, in collaborative problem-solving, and in creative activity, focusing on the role of gesture in building shared understandings.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Wendy Leeds-Hurwitz, Tom Clark, Geoff Hall, and Daryl Johnson for their valuable input to earlier versions of this research, as well as three anonymous reviewers and editorial work at TEXT & TALK. We are indebted to the lifeguards who took part in this study and to the managers of the beaches at the town hall, and we thank Magali Kerbellec, Marion Sadoux, and Aliyah Morgenstern for help with questions relating to French.

Appendix: transcription conventions

----

Gaze direction. Indicated in tier underneath speech. Direction of gaze integrated between the dashes (e.g. “---sand--” is gaze towards the sand; also see abbreviations in Table 1).

--/--

Forward slash in-between dashes indicates shift in gaze (e.g. “---sand--/--sun--” would mean a shift in gaze from the sand towards the sand).

*words*

Coordination of gestural action is temporally anchored in the speech with asterisks then described in-between same signs in a tier below speech.

+words+

Coordination of action other than gestures (such as clearing the sand) follows the same convention but with plus signs.

°words°

Speech between these symbols is with lowered voice (e.g. for parenthetical comment).

[words]

Square parentheses indicates overlapping speech.

?

Question intonation

er:::

Colons indicate elongated sound.

(0.7)

Pause length noted to tenths of a second; pauses between turns indicated on separate line.

HL

Speakers are indicated at beginning of line: HL (Head Lifeguard), CM (Lifeguard operating camera), L3 (Third Lifeguard)

References

Deborah, Tannen (ed.). 1993. Framing in discourse. New York, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Deppermann, Arnulf. 2014. Multimodal participation in simultaneous joint projects: Interpersonal and intrapersonal coordination in paramedic emergency drills. In P. Haddington, T. Keisanen, L. Mondada & M. Nevile (eds.), Multiactivity in social interaction: Beyond multitasking, 247–281. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.10.1075/z.187.09depSearch in Google Scholar

Emery, Nathan J. 2000. The eyes have it: The neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 24. 581–604.10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00025-7Search in Google Scholar

Goffman, Erving. 1974. Frame analysis. An essay on the organization of experience. Boston: Northeastern University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Goodwin, Charles. 1994. Professional vision. American Anthropologist 96(3). 606–633.10.1525/aa.1994.96.3.02a00100Search in Google Scholar

Goodwin, Charles. 2000. Action and embodiment within situated human interaction. Journal of Pragmatics 32(10). 1489–1522.10.1016/S0378-2166(99)00096-XSearch in Google Scholar

Goodwin, Charles. 2007. Environmentally coupled gestures. In Susan Duncan, Justine Cassell & Elena Levy (eds.), Gesture and the dynamic dimension of language. Essays in honor of David McNeill. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.10.1075/gs.1.18gooSearch in Google Scholar

Gordon, Cynthia. 2008. A(p)parent play: Blending frames and reframing in family talk. Language in Society 37. 319–349. doi:10.1017/S0047404508080536.Search in Google Scholar

Haddington, Pentti, Tiina Keisanen, Lorenza Mondada & Maurice Nevile. 2014. eds. Multiactivity in social interaction: Beyond multitasking. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.10.1075/z.187Search in Google Scholar

Heritage, John. 1984. Garfinkel and ethnomethodology. Cambridge: Polity Press.Search in Google Scholar

Kendon, Adam. 1967. Some functions of gaze direction in two-person conversation. Acta Psychologica 26. 22–63. http://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(67)90005-4 (accessed 8 September 2017).10.1016/0001-6918(67)90005-4Search in Google Scholar

Langton, Stephen R.H. 2000. The mutual influence of gaze and head orientation in the analysis of social attention direction. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 53A(3). 825–845.10.1080/713755908Search in Google Scholar

Mondada, Lorenza. 2011. The interactional production of multiple spatialities within a participatory democracy meeting. Social Semiotics 21(2). 283–308.10.1080/10350330.2011.548650Search in Google Scholar

Mondada, Lorenza. 2014. The temporal orders of multiactivity. Operating and demonstrating in the surgical theatre. In Pentti Haddington, Tiina Keisanen, Lorenza Mondada & Maurice Nevile (eds.), Multiactivity in social interaction: Beyond multitasking, 33–75. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.10.1075/z.187.02monSearch in Google Scholar

Mondada, Lorenza. 2016. Challenges of multimodality: Language and the body in social interaction. Journal of Sociolinguistics 20(3). 336–366. http://doi.org/10.1111/josl.1_12177 (accessed 8 September 2017).10.1111/josl.1_12177Search in Google Scholar

Nishizaka, Aug. 2014. Sustained orientation to one activity in multiactivity during prenatal ultrasound examinations. In Pentti Haddington, Tiina Keisanen, Lorenza Mondada & Maurice Nevile (eds.), Multiactivity in social interaction: Beyond multitasking, 79–107. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.10.1075/z.187.03nisSearch in Google Scholar

Nishizaka, Aug & Masafumi Sunaga. 2015. Conversing while massaging: Multidimensional asymmetries of multiple activities in interaction. Research on Language and Social Interaction 48(2). 200–229.10.1080/08351813.2015.1025506Search in Google Scholar

Page, Jenny, Victoria Bates, Geoff Long, Peter Dawes & Mike Tipton. 2011. Beach lifeguards: Visual search patterns, detection rates and the influence of experience. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics 31. 216–224.10.1111/j.1475-1313.2011.00824.xSearch in Google Scholar

Page, Jenny & Tom Griffiths. 2014. Scanning and surveillance: Swimming pools, beaches and open sea. In Bierens. In J.L.M. Joost (ed.), Handbook on drowning: Prevention, rescue, treatment, 323–329. Germany: Springer.10.1007/978-3-642-04253-9_47Search in Google Scholar

Raymond, Geoffrey & Gene H. Lerner. 2014. A body and its involvements: Adjusting action for dual involvements. In Pentti Haddington, Tiina Keisanen, Lorenza Mondada & Maurice Nevile (eds.), Multiactivity in social interaction: Beyond multitasking, 227–245. Amsterdam, Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.10.1075/z.187.08raySearch in Google Scholar

Rossano, Federico. 2013. Gaze in Conversation. In Jack Sidnell & Tanya Stivers (eds.), The handbook of conversation analysis, 308–329. Malden: Blackwell Publishing Ltd.10.1002/9781118325001.ch15Search in Google Scholar

Tannen, Deborah & Cynthia Wallat. 1993. Interactive frames and knowledge schemas in interaction: Examples from a medical examination/interview. In Deborah Tannen (ed.), Framing in discourse, 57–76. New York: Random House.Search in Google Scholar

Williams, Robert F. & Simon Harrison. 2014. Distributed cognition and gesture: Propagating a functional system through impromptu teaching. In J. L. Polman, E. A. Kyza, D. K. O’Neil, I. Tabak, W. R. Penuel, A. S. Jurow, K. O’Connor, T. Lee & L. D’Amico (eds.), Learning and becoming in practice: The international conference of the learning sciences (ICLS), Vol. 3, 1655–1656. Boulder, CO: International Society of the Learning Sciences.Search in Google Scholar

Williams, Robert F. & Simon Harrison. in preparation. Distributed cognition and gesture in impromptu teaching.Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2017-9-19
Published in Print: 2017-11-27

© 2017 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 23.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/text-2017-0023/html
Scroll to top button