Home Did Shy Trump Supporters Bias the 2016 Polls? Evidence from a Nationally-representative List Experiment
Article
Licensed
Unlicensed Requires Authentication

Did Shy Trump Supporters Bias the 2016 Polls? Evidence from a Nationally-representative List Experiment

  • Alexander Coppock ORCID logo EMAIL logo
Published/Copyright: June 26, 2017
Become an author with De Gruyter Brill

Abstract

Explanations for the failure to predict Donald Trump’s win in the 2016 Presidential election sometimes include the “Shy Trump Supporter” hypothesis, according to which some Trump supporters succumb to social desirability bias and hide their vote preference from pollsters. I evaluate this hypothesis by comparing direct question and list experimental estimates of Trump support in a nationally representative survey of 5290 American adults fielded from September 2 to September 13, 2016. Of these, 32.5% report supporting Trump’s candidacy. A list experiment conducted on the same respondents yields an estimate 29.6%, suggesting that Trump’s poll numbers were not artificially deflated by social desirability bias as the list experiment estimate is actually lower than direct question estimate. I further investigate differences across measurement modes for relevant demographic and political subgroups and find no evidence in support of the “Shy Trump Supporter” hypothesis.

Appendix

Table A1:

Comparing Direct Question and List Experimental Estimates of Trump Support.

Subgroup N Unadjusted estimates
Adjusted estimates
Direct question List experiment Difference Direct question List experiment Difference
Entire sample 5290 32.5 (0.8) 29.6 (3.4) 3.0 (3.4) 33.6 (0.7) 33.0 (2.6) 0.6 (2.6)
Strong democrat 726 4.5 (0.8) −4.3 (7.5) 8.8 (7.6) 5.0 (0.6) 0.8 (2.4) 4.2 (2.4)
Not very strong democrat 1140 9.2 (1.2) 5.0 (7.0) 4.2 (6.9) 8.0 (0.7) 2.5 (3.2) 5.5 (3.2)
Lean democrat 409 13.0 (2.1) 8.4 (11.0) 4.6 (11.1) 13.0 (1.4) 7.6 (5.9) 5.5 (6.1)
Independent 1131 20.1 (1.5) 24.3 (8.0) −4.2 (8.1) 18.8 (1.4) 26.5 (6.9) −7.7 (6.9)
Lean republican 351 73.4 (2.7) 55.9 (11.0) 17.6 (11.1) 59.8 (2.4) 48.5 (12.5) 11.3 (12.3)
Not very strong republican 990 68.1 (1.9) 72.1 (8.2) −4.0 (8.0) 72.4 (1.3) 73.2 (6.9) −0.8 (6.9)
Strong republican 543 90.4 (1.3) 80.5 (9.7) 9.9 (9.7) 84.1 (1.3) 89.6 (6.5) −5.5 (6.6)
Less than high school 157 30.7 (4.2) −4.3 (18.4) 35.1 (18.4) 33.6 (4.2) 15.3 (9.1) 18.3 (9.7)
High school or some college 2793 34.4 (1.1) 35.8 (4.7) −1.4 (4.6) 36.2 (1.0) 36.7 (4.2) −0.5 (4.2)
College 1465 30.1 (1.4) 27.4 (5.0) 2.7 (5.0) 32.5 (1.3) 35.5 (4.7) −3.0 (4.6)
Graduate school 875 25.8 (1.7) 10.9 (6.7) 14.9 (6.5) 27.1 (1.6) 20.5 (4.7) 6.6 (4.5)
Below 20th income percentile 1038 30.7 (1.8) 15.6 (8.2) 15.1 (7.8) 33.3 (1.2) 31.0 (4.0) 2.4 (3.9)
20th–40th Income percentile 1361 36.6 (1.7) 38.4 (6.3) −1.8 (6.2) 35.6 (1.1) 34.6 (2.8) 0.9 (2.7)
40th–60th Income percentile 936 34.0 (2.1) 19.7 (7.3) 14.3 (7.3) 34.0 (1.3) 34.8 (3.3) −0.8 (3.1)
60th–80th Income percentile 1151 33.9 (1.8) 40.7 (7.4) −6.8 (7.4) 35.4 (1.4) 39.7 (5.0) −4.3 (4.9)
Above 80th income percentile 804 27.2 (2.0) 30.5 (8.8) −3.4 (9.0) 27.5 (1.4) 21.4 (4.7) 6.1 (4.7)
Men 2332 36.9 (1.3) 33.5 (5.3) 3.5 (5.2) 38.0 (1.1) 33.5 (3.7) 4.5 (3.7)
Women 2958 28.4 (1.1) 26.4 (4.4) 2.0 (4.5) 30.1 (1.0) 32.7 (4.6) −2.6 (4.6)
White 3544 39.4 (1.1) 38.0 (4.1) 1.4 (4.1) 39.9 (0.9) 40.5 (3.3) −0.6 (3.3)
Black 446 10.3 (1.7) 16.6 (10.9) −6.3 (11.0) 11.2 (1.7) 22.0 (9.0) −10.7 (9.0)
Hispanic 804 24.4 (2.0) 2.8 (9.7) 21.6 (9.4) 26.2 (1.9) 9.0 (5.5) 17.2 (5.4)
Other race 496 20.3 (2.2) 24.0 (11.6) −3.8 (11.2) 20.7 (2.2) 28.5 (10.1) −7.8 (10.0)
Unlikely voter 1420 24.2 (1.4) 20.7 (7.1) 3.5 (7.1) 24.3 (1.3) 24.9 (5.9) −0.5 (5.9)
Likely voter 3870 36.5 (1.0) 33.6 (3.7) 2.9 (3.7) 37.0 (0.9) 36.0 (2.9) 1.0 (2.8)
  1. All estimates incorporate sampling weights.

  2. Bootstrapped standard errors are in parentheses.

  3. Adjusted direction question estimates are predictions from a logistic regression.

  4. Adjusted list experiment estimates are predictions from Imai’s (2011) NLS regression model.

References

Aronow, P. M., A. Coppock, F. W. Crawford and D. P. Green (2015) “Combining List Experiment and Direct Question Estimates of Sensitive Behavior Prevalence,” Journal of Survey Statistics and Methodology, 3:43–66.10.1093/jssam/smu023Search in Google Scholar

Blair, G. and K. Imai (2012) “Statistical Analysis of List Experiments,” Political Analysis, 20:47–77.10.1093/pan/mpr048Search in Google Scholar

Curtice, J. (1997) “So How Well Did They Do? The Polls in the 1997 Election,” Journal of the Market Research Society, 39:449–462.10.1177/147078539703900303Search in Google Scholar

Dropp, K. (2015) “Why Does Donald Trump Perform Better in Online Versus Live Telephone Polling?”.Search in Google Scholar

Dropp, K. (2016) “Yes, There Are Shy Trump Voters. No, They Won’t Swing the Election,” URL https://morningconsult.com/2016/11/03/yes-shy-trump-voters-no-wont-swing-election/.Search in Google Scholar

Durand, C., A. Blais and S. Vachon (2001) “Review: A Late Campaign Swing or a Failure of the Polls? The Case of the 1998 Quebec Election,” The Public Opinion Quarterly, 65:108–123.10.1086/320041Search in Google Scholar

Frye, T., S. Gehlbach, K. L. Marquardt and O. J. Reuter (2016) “Is Putin’s Popularity Real?” Post-Soviet Affairs, 1–15.10.1080/1060586X.2016.1144334Search in Google Scholar

Glynn, A. N. (2013) “What Can We Learn with Statistical Truth Serum? Design and Analysis of the List Experiment,” Public Opinion Quarterly, 77:159–172.10.1093/poq/nfs070Search in Google Scholar

Hopkins, D. J. (2009) “No More Wilder Effect, Never a Whitman Effect: When and Why Polls Mislead about Black and Female Candidates,” The Journal of Politics, 71:769–781.10.1017/S0022381609090707Search in Google Scholar

Imai, K. (2011) “Multivariate Regression Analysis for the Item Count Technique,” Journal of the American Statistical Association, 106:407–416.10.1198/jasa.2011.ap10415Search in Google Scholar

Kuklinski, J. H., P. M. Sniderman, K. Knight, T. Piazza, P. E. Tetlock, G. R. Lawrence and B. Mellers (1997) “Racial Prejudice and Attitudes Toward Affirmative Action,” American Journal of Political Science, 41:402–419.10.2307/2111770Search in Google Scholar

LaBrie, J. W. and M. Earleywine (2000) “Sexual Risk Behaviors and Alcohol: Higher Base Rates Revealed Using the Unmatched-Count Technique,” Journal of Sex Research, 37:321–326.10.1080/00224490009552054Search in Google Scholar

Lax, J. R., J. H. Phillips and A. F. Stollwerk (2016) “Are Survey Respondents Lying About Their Support for Same-Sex Marriage? Lessons from a List Experiment,” Public Opinion Quarterly, 80:510–533.10.1093/poq/nfv056Search in Google Scholar

Lyall, J., G. Blair and K. Imai (2013) “Explaining Support for Combatants duringWartime: A Survey Experiment in Afghanistan,” American Political Science Review, 107:679–705.10.1017/S0003055413000403Search in Google Scholar

Mellon, J. and C. Prosser (2017) “Missing Nonvoters and Misweighted Samples: Explaining the 2015 Great British Polling Miss,” Public Opinion Quarterly, forthcoming.10.1093/poq/nfx015Search in Google Scholar

Miller, J. (1984) A New Survey Technique for Studying Deviant Behavior, Phd thesis., George Washington University.Search in Google Scholar

Payne, J. G. (2010) “The Bradley Effect: Mediated Reality of Race and Politics in the 2008 US Presidential Election,” American Behavioral Scientist, 54: 417–435.10.1177/0002764210381713Search in Google Scholar

Powell, R. J. (2013) “Social Desirability Bias in Polling on Same-sex Marriage Ballot Measures,” American Politics Research, 41:1052–1070.10.1177/1532673X13484791Search in Google Scholar

Streb, M. J., B. Burrell, B. Frederick and M. A. Genovese (2008) “Social Desirability Effects and Support for a Female American President,” Public Opinion Quarterly, 72:76–89.10.1093/poq/nfm035Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2017-06-26
Published in Print: 2017-10-26

©2017 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 25.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/spp-2016-0005/html
Scroll to top button