Abstract
To assess primary emotions in subcortical brain regions across all mammals, the affective neuroscience personality scales (ANPS) were created for use in research involving human subjects. Later revisions of the scales have been validated across many countries and are used in diverse fields of psychology. The ANPS revisions culminated in the most recent ANPS 3.1. In this study, we evaluated the psychometric properties of the ANPS 3.1 and its two abbreviated versions (affective neuroscience personality scales – brief (BANPS) and affective neuroscience personality scales – short version (ANPS-S)) in a Slovenian community sample of 502 young adults. We simultaneously examined several kinds of validity evidence across the three versions: construct validity, internal reliability, and convergent validity. Our findings revealed acceptable construct validity of the six-factor model of the BANPS that was superior to the ANPS-S and particularly to the ANPS 3.1. The latter exhibited incremental fit issues noted in previous versions. However, we revealed sufficient internal reliability and convergent validity of the scale scores against the Big Five personality traits, and the frequency of the participants’ recently felt positive and negative emotions across all three ANPS versions. Testing the measurement invariance of BANPS across sex suggested full metric invariance and partial scalar invariance which allows direct score comparisons between males and females.
1 Introduction
Personality models based on the lexical approach currently dominate the field of personality research, with the five-factor model (FFM; Costa & McCrae, 1985, 1992) and the Big Five (Goldberg, 1990) among the most prominent ones. Although they stem from different research traditions, both provide a comprehensive framework to describe the human personality and subsume a large number of specific traits into Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness. The models have been demonstrated useful and highly applicable for personality assessment in general populations and in clinical samples (Costa & Widiger, 2002). Cross-cultural research has further suggested that the five personality constructs are robust across many different cultures and language communities (McCrae & Costa, 1997; McCrae et al., 2005; Saucier & Goldberg, 2006). However, a lack of a strong theoretical grounding for the emergence of these hypothesized personality traits might be the most considerable drawback of the FFM (Boyle, 2008) or the Big Five. Likewise, convincing evidence for the neurobiological basis of the proposed biological grounding of the five personality traits (Costa & McCrae, 1992; Eaves et al., 1989; Plomin, 1990) was deficient three decades ago (e.g., Eysenck, 1994). While the Big Five still lacks a comprehensive theoretical grounding based on neurobiology (Montag & Davis (2018), various studies have identified biological markers of the five traits, such as the brain structure (DeYoung et al., 2010, 2021) and molecular genetic underpinnings (McCrae et al., 2010; Power & Pluess, 2015).
1.1 The Primary Emotional Systems and the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS)
Over the past few decades, there has been a substantial increase in the understanding of the neurobiological underpinnings of personality, specifically of its emotional foundation (Davis & Panksepp, 2018; DeYoung et al., 2021; Panksepp, 1998; Yarkoni, 2015). One of the leading contributions was the Affective Neuroscience Theory (Panksepp, 1998). The author and his colleagues performed experiments on animals (vertebrates, mainly mammals) that involved pharmacological manipulations, deep electric brain stimulation, and lesion studies combined with detailed subject observation. They identified seven primary emotional systems that are grounded in the subcortical regions of all mammals (the systems are capitalized to prevent confusion with similar terms for emotions used in the common vernacular). These systems are SEEKING, LUST, CARE, PLAY, ANGER, FEAR, and SADNESS. Each of them has a separate neural pathway during activation (e.g., SEEKING involving the nucleus accumbens, ventral tegmental area, mesolimbic and mesocortical outputs, lateral hypothalamus, and the periaqueductal gray; Montag & Davis, 2018; Panksepp, 2011), specific neuromodulators that activate and regulate it (e.g., SEEKING – dopamine, ANGER – substance P, FEAR – glutamate, CARE – oxytocin), and expression in observable behaviors (e.g., distress call vocalizations – SADNESS, fleeing – FEAR, biting – ANGER) that causally follow the activation of each system (Panksepp, 2006). While SEEKING, LUST, CARE, and PLAY represent the positive primary emotional systems, ANGER, FEAR, and SADNESS represent the negative emotional systems (Davis & Panksepp, 2011).
The primary emotional systems are thought to have developed as “survival guides” and have been crucial in allowing mammals to navigate environments filled with evolutionary pressures that have endangered them throughout the course of their evolution (Panksepp, 1998). The SEEKING system drives animals to search the environment for the resources required to fulfill their survival needs (a drive accompanied by feelings of enthusiasm, curiosity, and motivation). The ANGER system enables animals to defend themselves and their resources from others. When activated, the system evokes feelings of irritation, anger, frustration, and rage. The FEAR system helps animals escape and avoid physical threats that could result in bodily harm or death. The system associates with anxiety, worry, and fear. The LUST system drives animals to search for and identify potential mates and thus reproduce. In the course of their later evolution, mammals acquired a more social orientation which resulted in an affective system that drives them to nurture their offspring (CARE). The system is associated with a need for nurturing, feelings of empathy and affection, and preference to care for others, as well as with a powerful separation distress that fosters caregiver-offspring contacts and is accompanied by distress vocalizations, feelings of sadness, and panic in the offspring (SADNESS). Finally, a system that promotes social bonding and learning, especially between young animals, is accompanied by feelings of joy and happiness (PLAY). This neuro-evolutionary psychobiological point of view offers an insight into the functional characteristics of motivational/emotional systems in human phylogenesis (Cory & Gardner 2002; Darwin 1965; McLean, 1990) and has the potential to contribute to a better understanding of human personality, as well as to promote treatment of mental disorders (Coenen et al., 2011; Davis & Panksepp, 2011).
To investigate individual differences in these evolutionarily ancient drives, Davis et al. (2003) constructed a self-report measure – the 110-item ANPS. They capture six of the primary emotional systems, with the LUST system omitted due to concerns regarding social desirability bias which might threaten the validity of the respective scale. However, the authors added the spirituality scale as an additional dimension for its documented relevance in the clinical treatment of addiction problems (Jarusiewicz, 2000). Since its original version, the ANPS has been subject to several revisions, with the 112-item version 2.4 (Davis & Panksepp, 2011) being the most prominent one in current use. It contains slight adjustments over the ANPS intended to enhance the psychometric properties of the scales. Indeed, the ANPS and their variants have suffered from several psychometric issues, including occasional difficulties to support the factorial structure according to the most commonly cited fit indexes (Hu & Bentler, 1999). To improve upon some of these drawbacks, several short versions of the instrument, targeting the original six primary emotional systems, have been created. These include the 33-item BANPS (Barrett et al., 2013), the 36-item ANPS-S (Pingault et al., 2012a), and the ANPS-AR (Montag & Davis, 2018) which accounts for observer ratings.
It is also of note that the ANPS, all designed as trait measures of Panksepp’s primary emotions (Montag et al., 2021), have been biologically validated. Behavioral genetic studies with twins yielded moderate (e.g., for ANGER) to high (e.g., PLAY) heritability estimates for the primary emotional systems as assessed by the ANPS (Montag et al., 2016a,b). Molecular genetic research further identified links of several genetic markers (e.g., DARPP-32, OXTR, DA, COMT) with the negative ANPS systems (Felten et al., 2011; Harro et al., 2019; Montag et al., 2011; Reuter et al, 2009). Likewise, brain imaging studies found associations of brain activity in discrete areas with specific scores of the “ANPS negative primary systems” (e.g., Deris et al., 2017; Unterrainer et al., 2017), and Reuter et al. (2009) reported that higher ANGER scores relate to lower gray matter density in the left amygdala.
The ANPS and its later versions have been translated and validated in several countries and various languages, including French (ANPS 2.4: Pahlavan et al., 2008; Pingault et al., 2012b; ANPS-S: Pingault et al., 2012a), Spanish (ANPS 2.4: Abella et al., 2011), Turkish (ANPS: Özkarar-Gradwohl et al., 2014), Norwegian (ANPS 2.4., BANPS, ANPS-S: Pedersen et al., 2014), Polish (ANPS 2.4: Cwojdzińska & Rybakowski, 2015), Italian (ANPS: Pascazio et al., 2015; ANPS 2.4: Giacolini et al., 2017), Brazilian Portuguese (BANPS: Esposito et al., 2016), German (ANPS: Reuter et al., 2017), Japanese (ANPS 2.4: Narita et al., 2017), Persian (BANPS: Amiri & Azad-Marzabadi, 2017), Mandarin Chinese (ANPS 2.4: Sindermann et al., 2018), Portuguese (BANPS: Gurfinkel et al., 2018), and Serbian (ANPS 2.4: Montag et al., 2019). Montag et al. (2021) introduced the latest revision of the instrument – the ANPS 3.1 and invited researchers to start using it. It differs from the previous versions in that it features a six-point rating scale and includes an experimental social dominance scale while retaining the spirituality scale. The potential significance of the social dominance scale for the assessment of human personality was proposed due to the identification of the trait in other animal species and the associated neuromodulators such as testosterone and cortisol (van der Westhuizen & Solms, 2015).
Following this suggestion, the current study aimed to validate the ANPS 3.1 in Slovenia where none of the ANPS versions has been translated/adapted to date. As the 3.1 version also includes items of the short ANPS versions, we simultaneously targeted the validation of the BANPS and ANPS-S.
1.2 Primary Emotional Systems, Personality Traits, Emotional Experiences, and Clinical Correlates
From the very beginnings of the development of the ANPS, the primary emotional systems that arise from subcortical brain regions have been thought to, at least in part, represent the neurobiological substrate of the five robust personality traits (Davis et al., 2003; Davis & Panksepp, 2011) and a likely evolutionary foundation of human personality (Davis & Panksepp, 2011; Montag & Panksepp, 2017). Specifically, Panksepp’s primary emotional systems have been considered as bottom-up emotional underpinnings of the lexically and statistically derived higher-order traits of the FFM or the Big Five (Davis & Panksepp, 2018; Marengo et al., 2021; Montag et al., 2021).
Albeit convincing neuropsychological evidence for the links of neural circuitry to the ANPS scores is still insufficient (Montag et al., 2021), self-report data obtained through measures of personality traits and the ANPS (asking individuals more directly about feelings and their expressions in behavior than personality inventories) show consistent and substantial relations of the six primary emotional systems with four robust personality traits across different countries. Specifically, SEEKING associates with openness, PLAY with extraversion, both low ANGER and high CARE with agreeableness, and FEAR, SADNESS, and ANGER with neuroticism (Abella et al., 2011; Barrett et al., 2013; Davis & Panksepp, 2011; Montag et al., 2019; Montag & Panksepp, 2017; Pahlavan et al., 2008) (Figure 1). Montag and Panksepp (2017), for example, reported the following associations between the theoretically related ANPS and the FFM traits: SEEKING and openness (r = 0.47), both FEAR and SADNESS and neuroticism (r = 0.75 and r = 0.68, respectively), CARE and agreeableness (r = 0.50), ANGER and both neuroticism (r = 0.65) and agreeableness (r = −0.48), and PLAY and extraversion (r = 0.46). A recent meta-analysis of 13 studies using the NEO and the Big Five inventories (Marengo et al., 2021) also suggested that ANGER associates with both agreeableness (r = −0.47) and neuroticism (r = 0.42), CARE with agreeableness (r = 0.39), FEAR and SADNESS with neuroticism (r = 0.73 and r = 0.67, respectively), PLAY with extraversion (r = 0.64), and SEEKING with openness (r = 0.47). These mainly differential relationships support the convergent validity of the ANPS scores. In contrast, modest or nonsignificant associations of the ANPS scores with conscientiousness across the studies are consistent with the divergent validity of the ANPS. While the ANPS items that refer to the six primary emotional systems scales were designed to capture individual differences in conscious expression of subcortical primary emotions, conscientiousness represents the most cerebrally-focused personality dimension of the FFM/Big Five measures and thus, links poorly to the primary emotions (Marengo et al., 2021).

The theoretically congruent associations between the primary emotions, Big Five traits, and positive and negative experiences.
Other important personality theories have been evaluated in regard to the ANPS (see Montag et al., 2021 for a review), such as Cloninger’s biosocial theory. Cwojdzińska and Rybakowski (2015), for example, found novelty seeking associated with PLAY, low harm avoidance with SEEKING, CARE with both reward dependence and persistence, FEAR and SADNESS with both harm avoidance and reward dependence, and ANGER with low persistence. Validity evidence of the ANPS was also documented with scores on questionnaires stemming from the subjective well-being tradition (e.g., positive and negative affect), or instruments targeting specific emotions (e.g., anxiety, anger). Employing such self-report measures with young adults from a general population, Pingault et al. (2012b) revealed relations of the ANPS scores referring to positive (particularly PLAY) and negative primary emotional systems (especially FEAR and SADNESS) with positive and negative affectivity scores. Further support for the validity of the ANPS comes from clinical studies. Pedersen et al. (2014) validated three versions of the ANPS in a large sample of persons with various personality disorders (PDs) and found similar results on construct validity and internal reliability to studies in community samples. Compared to a large control group, depressed patients reported higher levels of SADNESS and FEAR, as well as lower levels of SEEKING and PLAY, but the controls who scored higher on depression showed a similar pattern of associations with the ANPS scores and reported higher levels of ANGER (Montag et al., 2017). Along with SADNESS most strongly predicting depression, the scale scores were associated with somatization, anxiety, substance abuse disorder (Fuchshuber et al., 2019a), and childhood trauma (Fuchshuber et al., 2019b).
1.3 The Present Study
Our goal was to simultaneously validate three versions of the ANPS in Slovenia, i.e., the most recent ANPS 3.1 that has not been validated yet, and two of the short versions, namely the BANPS and the ANPS-S. Since both short versions include the six primary emotional scales, we considered only these six scales for the ANPS 3.1. Specifically, we aimed to assess the construct validity of the three ANPS versions (their factorial structure and intercorrelations among the scales), the correlations between the corresponding scales across the three versions, the internal consistency of the scales, and the convergent validity of the scale scores against the Big Five personality traits and the experiences of positive and negative emotions. Likewise, we examined the associations of the scales forming general scores of positive affect and negative affect. To support the convergent validity of the three ANPS versions, PLAY should be associated with extraversion, ANGER with neuroticism and (negatively) with agreeableness, SEEKING with openness, and both FEAR and SADNESS with neuroticism. Likewise, the ANPS general positive affect and general negative affect, as well as each of their correspondent scale scores should share variance with the frequency of individuals’ positive emotional experiences and negative emotional experiences, respectively.
Based on the obtained psychometric properties, we further aimed to opt for the relatively most appropriate version of the ANPS in the Slovenian social-cultural context and ascertain whether the mean scale scores can be directly compared between males and females by testing the measurement invariance across sex. Except for the ANPS-S (Orri et al., 2018), the measurement invariance has not been evaluated in previous ANPS studies, which established consistent, but modest sex differences, with women scoring higher on FEAR, SADNESS, and CARE (Pahlavan et al., 2008; Pedersen et al., 2014; Pingault et al., 2012b); other score differences (i.e., for ANGER and PLAY) were sample-specific. However, a lack of evidence on measurement invariance may cast doubts on the outcomes of group comparisons because we cannot be certain that the target constructs are measured in the same way across different groups (e.g., men and women).
2 Method
2.1 Participants and Procedure
We recruited participants through an online survey platform. E-mails were sent to all three Slovenian public universities and the study was promoted on social media. The only requirement to take part was 18 years of age. Potential participants were first informed about the aim of the study, the approximate time needed to complete the survey and their rights (e.g., anonymity, consensual participation, possibility to terminate participation at any time, data analysis only on a group level). The introductory page also mentioned a raffle that was employed in the study (one participant would win a 25€ gift card) as a modest incentive for completing the entire survey. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Arts, University of Ljubljana (No. 248-2021).
Individuals who intended to participate gave their consent by clicking “next page” to enter the survey. Out of 843 potential participants who provided informed consent (M age = 29.85 years, SD = 12.54; 39.5% men and 60.5% women; 51.6% students, 35% employed full time, 1.8% employed part time, 3.7% unemployed, and 8.9% indicated “other” status). The highest attained level of education was as follows: high school (44.2%), Bachelor’s degree (22.5%), Master’s degree (18%), and 15.3% of the remaining individuals reported either a nine-year elementary school, secondary technical school, or a PhD. We performed the analyses only for the participants with complete data. Of the 843 initial participants, 502 completed the survey and constituted our final sample. It included 308 women (60.7%) and 194 men (38.6%). The age of the final sample ranged from 18 to 30 years (M = 28.99 years, SD = 12.59); 274 (54.6%) were students, 15.4 (30.7%) were employed full time, 9 (1.8%) were employed part time, 17 (3.4%) were unemployed, and 48 (9.6%) specified “other” as their current employment status. Most participants completed a four-year high school (231 or 46%), followed by those who had a Bachelor’s degree (106 or 21.1%), a Master’s degree (94 or 18.7%), and finished either a nine-year elementary school, secondary technical school, or a PhD (17 or 14.2%). The group comparison analysis between the initial sample and the final sample did not reveal statistically significant differences across the respective demographic characteristics (age: F(1, 1345) = 1.510, p = 0.22; employment status: χ 2 = 15.49, df = 16, p = 0.49; level of education: χ 2 = 41.028, df = 13, p = 0.26). Note that the structure of the final sample by sex was nearly identical to the structure of the initial sample.
2.2 Measures
The survey consisted of demographic questions (age, gender, employment status, and level of completed education) and three self-report questionnaires. To avoid a potential bias in the order of presentation, the rank order of appearance of the questionnaires was automatically rotated.
2.2.1 ANPS
We employed three versions of the ANPS – the ANPS 3.1 (Montag et al., 2021), BANPS (Barrett et al., 2013), and ANPS-S (Pingault et al., 2012a). Both the 33-item BANPS and the 36-item ANPS-S include items that are retained in the 112-item ANPS 3.1. To avoid replication and response fatigue, we employed only the ANPS 3.1 and five BANPS items that do not overlap with those of the ANPS 3.1, whereas the ANPS-S items are fully subsumed by the ANPS 3.1. However, the items of the three ANPS versions are rated along different rating scales, and therefore, we used the ANPS 3.1 rating scale across all items (ranging from 0 – strongly disagree to 5 – strongly agree). The authors of the present study translated all the items of the ANPS 3.1 and the additional items of the BANPS into Slovenian. The translations were examined by a native-speaking professor of the English language. A few inconsistencies were clarified for semantic differences and the translated items were revised accordingly.
With regard to The ANPS 3.1 (ANPS 3.1; Montag et al., 2021), we only used 84 items that form six scales of the primary emotional systems (14 items per each scale). Three of these scales refer to positive emotional systems of SEEKING (e.g., “Seeking an answer is as enjoyable as finding the solution.”), PLAY (e.g., “I am known as one who keeps work fun.”), and CARE (e.g., “I feel softhearted towards stray animals.”). They constitute a measure of general positive affect. The other three primary scales of FEAR (e.g., “I sometimes cannot stop worrying about my problems.”), ANGER (e.g. “My friends would probably describe me as hotheaded.”), and SADNESS (e.g., “I often feel lonely.”) refer to negative emotional systems and represent a measure of general negative affect. The ANPS 3.1 was first published in a comprehensive review of the ANPS studies (Montag et al., 2021), but the scales have not been validated yet. In the present study, the internal reliability (in terms of alpha coefficients; Table 3) of the scales ranged from 0.79 for SEEKING to 0.90 for FEAR.
The Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales – Brief (BANPS; Barrett et al., 2013) includes 28 items of the ANPS (Davis et al., 2003) and five new ones that were added to improve the internal reliability of the scales and provide a clearer six-factor structure. Similar to the ANPS 3.1, the BANPS of SEEKING (six items), PLAY (six items), and CARE (four items) form a measure of general positive affect and the scales of FEAR (five items), ANGER (six items) and SADNESS (six items) constitute a measure of general negative affect. In the BANPS validation study (Barrett et al., 2013), fit indexes were as follows: SRMSR 0.0645, RMSEA 0.0649 (0.0619 –0.0679), while the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) and comparative fit index (CFI) were not reported; alpha coefficients ranged from 0.65 (CARE) to 0.84 (SADNESS). The internal reliability of the scales in the present study (Table 4) ranged from 0.67 (CARE) to 0.89 (SADNESS).
The Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales – Short Version (ANPS-S; Pingault et al., 2012a) also contains a reduced number of items from the ANPS (Davis et al., 2003) that form the six primary scales (six items per scale). In the validation study (Pingault et al., 2012a), the reported fit indexes were standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) 0.06, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) 0.04, and CFI 0.90, with alpha coefficients from 0.61 (SEEKING) to 0.79 (ANGER). As mentioned in the introduction, convergent validity was determined by associations between CARE and empathic skills, SEEKING and systemizing propensity, FEAR and trait anxiety, SADNESS and depression, and ANGER and trait anger (Pingault et al., 2012b). In the present study, the alpha coefficients (Table 5) ranged from 0.69 for SEEKING to 0.82 for SADNESS.
The Big-Five Inventory (BFI; John et al., 1991) is a 44-item measure of five robust personality traits. It is based on trait adjectives that were derived through factor analysis as follows: extraversion (eight items), agreeableness (nine items), conscientiousness (nine items), neuroticism (eight items), and openness (ten items). The items are designed as short phrases that begin with “I see myself as someone who is: e.g., talkative; reserved; not easily upset” and are rated on a five-point scale (1 – disagree strongly, 5 – agree strongly). The Slovenian version of the BFI (Avsec & Sočan, 2007) showed adequate construct validity, including internal consistency of the scales (alpha coefficients) between 0.77 (Extraversion) and 0.85 (Conscientiousness). Rank-order stability coefficients over a 2-year period were also high in a sample of emerging adults (0.82, 0.63, 0.73, 0.72, and 0.78 for extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness, respectively) and the trait scores differentially predicted emotional, psychological, and social well-being (Zupančič & Kavčič, 2017). In the present study, the internal reliability coefficients (Table 3) ranged from 0.77 for agreeableness to 0.87 for neuroticism.
The Scale of Positive and Negative Experiences (SPANE; Diener et al., 2009) is a 12-item measure that evaluates one’s positive (six items) and negative (six items) emotional experiences. Participants indicate how often they felt a certain way (e.g., positive, joyful, angry) over the past 4 weeks on a five-point rating scale (1 – very rarely or never, 5 – very often or always). The validation study suggested internal reliability of 0.84 and 0.80 (Diener et al., 2009). Evaluations of the model in a Slovenian sample showed a good fit: RMSEA = 0.04, SRMR = 0.034, CFI = 0.98 (Tomažič, 2021), with internal reliability (alphas) of 0.90 (positive experiences) and 0.84 (negative experiences). The alpha values in the present study (Table 3) were 0.73 for positive experiences and 0.84 for negative experiences. The predictive validity of the SPANE has been examined and supported in relation to life satisfaction, depression, and overall subjective well-being (Jovanović, 2015). Along with life satisfaction, satisfactory concurrent validity of the scale was established with other subjective well-being measures, such as optimism and pessimism (Espejo et al., 2020).
2.3 Data Analyses
To examine the construct validity of the three ANPS versions, we performed confirmatory factor analyses in R (R Core Team, 2021). Model fit was assessed according to the following indexes: CFI (Bentler, 1990), TLI (Tucker & Lewis, 1973), RMSEA (Steiger & Lind, 1980), and SRMR (Hu & Bentler, 1999). We relied on the cut-off values most often cited in the literature as suggesting good model fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999): CFI and TLI of 0.90 (or above), RMSEA between 0.05 and 0.08, and SRMR below 0.08. The robust maximum likelihood estimator was used to evaluate the model fit of the measures. Using IBM SPSS Version 25 (IBM, 2017), internal consistency of the scales was estimated by alpha coefficients, whereas correlations between the corresponding scale scores across the three ANPS versions, intercorrelations among the scale scores within each ANPS version, and convergent validity of the scales across the versions were estimated by the Pearson product-moment coefficients of correlation. Based on the results of these analyses, we selected the relatively most appropriate ANPS version and tested the factorial model for measurement invariance across sex through a multigroup confirmatory factor analysis. We examined configural, metric, and scalar invariance, using ∆CFI as a measure of invariance; ∆CFI smaller or equal to 0.01 was considered to indicate measurement invariance at each level (Cheung & Rensvold, 2002). If the model fit worsened substantially at the next step of invariance testing (i.e., ΔCFI > −0.01), we reviewed the modification indexes to examine noninvariant items. We then removed the equality of constraint in items that contributed most to an inadequate model fit. Partial invariance was achieved if we were able to decrease the differences between CFIs and RMSEAs within the accepted cut-offs and the parameter remained unconstrained in the following models of measurement invariance assessment. Lastly, we ran independent samples t-tests (two-sided) to examine sex differences and estimated the effect sizes of the differences by Hedge’s g.
3 Results
In this section, we first present the results of construct validity analyses for the ANPS versions under study and the correlations between the corresponding six scales across the versions. Then, we display intercorrelations among the six scale scores for each of the ANPS versions, followed by the internal consistency and the convergent validity evidence of the scale scores. Lastly, we present the measurement invariance analysis across sex and mean sex differences for the ANPS version, which showed the most acceptable psychometric properties overall.
3.1 Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the ANPS 3.1, the BANPS, and the ANPS-S
3.1.1 Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the ANPS 3.1, the BANPS, and the ANPS-S
Table 1 shows the goodness of fit statistics, whereas the obtained factor loadings are included in the supplement material (Tables S1, S2, and S3 for the ANPS 3.1, BANPS, and ANPS-S, respectively). According to the most cited cut-off values (Hu & Bentler, 1999), neither of the three ANPS versions suggested a clear model fit. While the RMSEA and SRMR were both within the values indicative of adequate fit (0.06 and 0.08, respectively), the TLI and CFI were below the standard cut-off values of 0.90 across the questionnaires. The BANPS and ANPS-S, however, suggested a relatively better fit to a six-factor model than the ANPS 3.1, while the BANPS demonstrated even a slightly better fit comparatively. Results regarding the BANPS were very similar to those of the original validation study in the US sample (Barrett et al., 2013; see the Method section for details), with both RMSEA and SRMR suggesting adequate fit, but the TLI and CLI slightly under the recommended values by Hu and Bentler (1999).
Fit indexes for the three ANPS versions in the Slovenian sample
Version of the ANPS | χ 2 | Df | RMSEA (90% CI) | TLI | CFI | SRMR |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
ANPS 3.1 | 11922.011 | 4443 | 0.060 (0.058–0.062) | 0.622 | 0.632 | 0.079 |
BANPS | 1304.151 | 480 | 0.065 (0.061–0.068) | 0.853 | 0.867 | 0.061 |
ANPS-S | 1813.075 | 579 | 0.068 (0.065–0.072) | 0.793 | 0.809 | 0.080 |
Note. RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; TLI = Tucker-Lewis Index; CFI = comparative fit index; SRMR = standardized root mean square residual. All χ 2 values were statistically significant (p < 0.001).
Regarding the scale structure obtained by the CFA, the item factor loadings for the BANPS were superior to those for the ANPS 3.1 and the ANPS-S. Except a very low loading (i.e., 0.10) of item 21 (57 in the ANPS 3.1) on the SEEKING scale, all other BANPS items loaded sufficiently (over 0.40) on the corresponding scales, ranging from 0.415 to 0.864 (Table S2). In contrast, the ANPS 3.1 had poor item loadings (below 0.40) for each of the proposed six scales, i.e., from one (FEAR) to seven (SADNESS) items per scale (Table S1). The ANPS-S had higher item loadings (Table S3; note that it also contains less items than version 3.1 and three more items than the BANPS). Three scales (SEEKING, PLAY, and SADNESS) of the ANPS-S were poorly loaded by one of the proposed items and three items suggested low loadings for the CARE scale. In sum, the ANPS 3.1 item 57 (“I am usually not interested in solving problems and puzzles just for the sake of solving them.”) also contained in the BANPS (21), but not in the ANPS-S, as well as the ANPS 3.1 items 27 (“Caring for a sick person would be a burden for me.”) and 54 (“I often think about people I have loved who are no longer with me.”) which are also included in the ANPS-S (but not in the BANPS) showed low loadings in both of the respective versions. In contrast, all other items shared between the ANPS 3.1 and the BANPS, or the ANPS 3.1 and the ANPS-S had higher loadings (over 0.40) in both respective versions.
3.2 Correlations Across the Three ANPS Versions in the Slovenian Sample
As shown in Table 2, all the correlations between the corresponding scale scores of the three ANPS versions were high. Within this range of effect size, the associations were relatively weaker between the CARE scales, particularly between the BANPS and the ANPS-S. It is of note once again that the CARE scale of the ANPS 3.1 and the ANPS-S had low loadings for two and three items in our sample, respectively. However, a similar pattern of correlations between the corresponding scales of multiple ANPS versions was revealed in a large Norwegian clinical sample (Pedersen et al., 2014).
Correlations between the ANPS 3.1, the BANPS, and the ANPS-S across the corresponding scale scores
SEEKING | FEAR | CARE | ANGER | PLAY | SADNESS | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
ANPS 3.1 with: | ||||||
BANPS | 0.85 | 0.92 | 0.78 | 0.95 | 0.91 | 0.93 |
ANPS-S | 0.88 | 0.91 | 0.84 | 0.93 | 0.94 | 0.88 |
BANPS with: | ||||||
ANPS-S | 0.83 | 0.86 | 0.67 | 0.94 | 0.84 | 0.82 |
Note. All correlations are significant at p < 0.001.
3.3 Intercorrelations, Internal Reliability, and Convergent Validity
The multi-trait-multi-method (MTMM) matrix for each of the three ANPS versions (Tables 3, 4, and 5 for the ANPS 3.1, BANPS, and ANPS-S, respectively) contains intercorrelations between the six primary scale scores and between the general positive affect and general negative affect scores, correlations between the scale scores and each of the two General Affect scores, as well as evidence for internal reliability and convergent validity of all these scale scores.
The MTMM matrix: Intercorrelations between the primary emotions scales of the ANPS 3.1, correlations of the scales with the general positive affect and with the general negative affect scores, internal consistency (in parenthesis) across the measures, and convergence of the ANPS 3.1 scales with the Big Five traits and with the positive and negative experiences scores
SEEK | FEAR | CARE | ANGER | PLAY | SAD | GPA | GNA | SP-PA | SP-NA | E | A | C | N | O | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
SEEK | (0.79) | ||||||||||||||
FEAR | −0.20** | (0.91) | |||||||||||||
CARE | 0.32** | 0.04 | (0.79) | ||||||||||||
ANGER | −0.13** | 0.42** | −0.03 | (0.88) | |||||||||||
PLAY | 0.37** | −0.23** | 0.34** | 0.02 | (0.86) | ||||||||||
SAD | −0.13** | 0.70** | 0.24** | 0.34** | −0.06 | (0.81) | |||||||||
GPA | 0.72** | −0.17** | 0.74** | −0.06 | 0.79** | 0.02 | (0.87) | ||||||||
GNA | −0.20** | 0.88** | 0.09* | 0.74** | −0.11* | 0.82** | −0.09* | (0.93) | |||||||
SP-PE | 0.31* | −0.23** | 0.28** | −0.17** | 0.36** | −0.20** | 0.43** | −0.35** | (0.90) | ||||||
SP-NE | −0.19** | 0.60** | −0.10* | 0.41** | −0.25** | 0.53** | −0.25** | 0.63** | −0.53** | (0.84) | |||||
E | 0.42** | −0.40** | 0.32** | 0.07 | 0.57** | −0.23** | 0.58** | −0.24** | 0.39** | −0.32** | (0.82) | ||||
A | 0.31** | −0.29** | 0.48** | −0.49** | 0.35** | −0.10* | 0.51** | −0.37** | 0.31** | −0.32** | 0.26** | (0.77) | |||
C | 0.17** | −0.10** | 0.16** | −0.14** | −0.05 | −0.13** | 0.11* | −0.15** | 0.21** | 0.16** | 0.29 ** | 0.26** | (0.81) | ||
N | −0.30** | 0.81** | −0.04 | 0.55** | −0.24** | 0.61** | −0.25** | 0.81** | −0.41** | 0.60** | −0.39** | −0.43** | −0.31** | (0.87) | |
O | 0.57** | −0.10* | 0.17** | −0.01 | 0.31** | −0.10** | 0.44** | −0.09 | 0.15** | −0.05 | 0.37** | 0.15** | 0.09* | − 0.14** | (0.82) |
Note. The proposed convergent relations are presented in bold. SEEK = SEEKING, SAD = SADNESS, GPA = general positive affect, GNA = general negative affect, SP-PE (SPANE positive experiences), SP-NE (SPANE negative experiences), E = extraversion, A = agreeableness, C = conscientiousness, N = neuroticism, O = openness. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.
The MTMM matrix: Intercorrelations between the six primary emotions scales of the BANPS, correlations of the scales with the general positive affect and general negative affect BANPS scores, internal consistency (in parenthesis) of the scales, and convergence of the BANPS with the Big Five traits, and Positive and Negative Experiences
SEEK | FEAR | CARE | ANGER | PLAY | SAD | GPA | GNA | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
SEEK | (0.72) | |||||||
FEAR | −0.11* | (0.84) | ||||||
CARE | 0.18** | 0.05 | (0.67) | |||||
ANGER | −0.04 | −0.32** | 0.01 | (0.82) | ||||
PLAY | 0.30** | −0.17** | 0.39** | 0.09 | (0.83) | |||
SAD | −0.12** | 0.67** | −0.02 | 0.29** | −0.18** | (0.89) | ||
GPA | 0.68** | −0.12** | 0.66** | 0.02 | 0.81** | −0.16** | (0.81) | |
GNA | −0.12** | 0.82** | 0.01 | 0.69** | −0.12* | 0.84** | −0.11** | (0.89) |
SP-PE | 0.20** | −0.34** | 0.32** | −0.18** | 0.36** | −0.48** | 0.40** | −0.43** |
SP-NE | −0.13** | 0.56** | −0.10* | 0.37** | −0.24** | 0.65** | −0.23** | 0.67** |
E | 0.31** | −0.34** | 0.40** | 0.13** | 0.57** | −0.38** | 0.58** | −0.25** |
A | 0.21** | 0.26** | 0.42** | −0.43** | 0.31 ** | 0.22** | 0.41** | −0.39** |
C | 0.06 | −0.19** | 0.19** | −0.18** | −0.01 | −0.33** | 0.09* | −0.30** |
N | −0.18** | 0.74** | −0.06 | 0.50** | 0.20** | 0.67** | −0.21** | 0.80** |
O | 0.61** | −0.05 | 0.17** | −0.01 | 0.33** | 0.05 | 0.52** | −0.05 |
Note. The matrix is reduced as all of the coefficients for the SPANE and the Big Five (BFI) scores are shown in Table 3. The proposed convergent relations are presented in bold. SEEK = SEEKING, SAD = SADNESS, GPA = General Positive Affect (the composite of the BANPS positive emotions scale scores), GNA = general negative affect (the composite of the BANPS negative emotions scores), SP-PE (SPANE positive experiences score), SP-NE (SPANE negative experiences score), E = extraversion, A = agreeableness, C = conscientiousness, N = neuroticism, O = openness. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.
The MTMM matrix: Intercorrelations between the six primary emotions scales of the ANPS-S, correlations of the scales with the general positive affect and general negative affect BANPS scores, internal consistency (in parenthesis) of the scales, and convergence of the ANPS-S scales with the Big Five traits, and positive and negative experiences
SEEK | FEAR | CARE | ANGER | PLAY | SAD | GPA | GNA | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
SEEK | (0.69) | |||||||
FEAR | −0.14* | (0.85) | ||||||
CARE | 0.27** | −0.06 | (0.76) | |||||
ANGER | −0.04 | 0.32** | −0.05 | (0.86) | ||||
PLAY | 0.37** | −0.22** | 0.30** | 0.04 | (0.73) | |||
SAD | −0.11* | 0.61** | −0.01 | 0.26** | −0.15** | (0.82) | ||
GPA | 0.70** | −0.19** | 0.76** | −0.02 | 0.74** | −0.11** | (0.80) | |
GNA | −0.12** | 0.83** | 0.05 | 0.70** | −0.14* | 0.80** | −0.24** | (0.88) |
SP-PE | 0.25** | −0.15** | 0.27** | −0.14** | 0.32** | −0.34** | 0.40** | −0.35** |
SP-NE | −0.11* | 0.54** | −0.16* | 0.33** | −0.26** | 0.61** | −0.12** | 0.63** |
E | 0.41** | −0.40** | 0.33** | 0.15** | 0.49** | −0.34** | 0.55** | −0.24** |
A | 0.24** | −0.30** | 0.49** | −0.41** | 0.37** | −0.21** | 0.51** | −0.37** |
C | 0.09* | −0.19** | 0.24** | −0.18** | −0.01 | −0.30** | 0.14** | −0.15** |
N | −0.20** | 0.80** | −0.15** | 0.48** | −0.24** | 0.64** | −0.26** | 0.81** |
O | 0.59** | 0.14** | 0.14** | 0.00 | 0.29** | −0.08 | 0.40** | −0.09 |
Note. The matrix is reduced as all of the coefficients for the SPANE and the Big Five (BFI) scores are shown in Table 3. The proposed convergent relations are presented in bold. SEEK = SEEKING, SAD = SADNESS, GPA = positive affect (composite of positive ANPS-S dimensions), GNA = negative affect (composite of negative ANPS-S dimensions), SP-PE (SPANE positive experiences), SP-NE (SPANE negative experiences), E = extraversion, A = agreeableness, C = conscientiousness, N = neuroticism, O = openness. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.
Except for the FEAR and the SADNESS scales, sharing a substantial amount of variance across the three versions, the correlations between the primary scales were low (some of them non-significant) and a few were moderate. The association between the two general affect scores was also low, suggesting the dissimilarity of the constructs. Furthermore, the correlations of each of the three positive emotions scales with the general positive affect and of each of the three negative emotions scales with the general negative affect were high. The internal reliability of the primary scale scores was acceptable, with a somewhat lower alpha coefficient for the BANPS CARE scale (0.67) and the highest one for the ANPS 3.1 FEAR scale (0.91). The alphas for the general affect scores were 0.80 and above across the versions.
The associations of the primary emotions scale scores with the frequency of recent positive and negative emotional experiences (over the last 4 weeks) and with the theoretically congruent Big Five traits provide evidence for the convergent validity of the scales across the three ANPS versions. Overall, the convergence of general negative affect with negative experiences appeared stronger (rs over 0.60) than of general positive affect with positive experiences (moderate effect sizes), whereas the significant associations of general positive affect with negative experiences and of general negative affect with positive experiences were all negative and low or moderate. The correlations of separate negative and positive primary emotional scale scores with negative emotional experiences (from 0.33 for the ANPS-S ANGER to 0.65 for the BANPS SADNESS) and positive emotional experiences (from 0.20 for the BANPS SEEKING to 0.36 for the BANPS/3.1 PLAY), respectively, were obtained within a similar range. In addition, there were a few non-significant correlations between the presumably unrelated or negatively related scale scores.
In relation to the Big Five, PLAY showed the highest correlations with extraversion, i.e., 0.57 (BANPS/3.1) and 0.49 (ANPS-S), ANGER correlated with both neuroticism (from 0.49 for ANPS-S to 0.55 for ANPS 3.1) and with agreeableness (from −0.41 for ANPS-S to −0.49 for 3.1), SEEKING with openness (from 0.57 for 3.1 to 0.61 for BANPS), and both FEAR and SADNESS with Neuroticism (from 0.74 for BANPS to 0.81 for 3.1 and from 0.61 for 3.1 to 0.67 for BANPS, respectively). In terms of effect sizes, all these associations were at least moderate. Likewise, the general affect scores and the four robust personality traits were substantially related as indicated by moderate (e.g., between ANPS-S positive affect and openness) to high correlations (up to 0.80 and 0.81 of BANPS and 3.1/ANPS-S negative affect with neuroticism, respectively).
3.4 Measurement Invariance of the BANPS Across Sex
To the best of our knowledge, only Orri et al. (2018) reported on the measurement invariance of an ANPS measure (i.e., the ANPS-S) across sex. As the BANPS suggested relatively best validity evidence among the three versions under our study, we examined whether the six scales of the BANPS primary emotional systems are measured in the same way in men and women. To assess the measurement invariance, we tested the hypotheses regarding equality of factor loadings (metric invariance) and equality of item intercepts (scalar invariance). The configural models were adequate (Table 6) and served as baseline models to evaluate the change in overall model fit after imposing specific parameter constraints of subsequent metric and scalar invariance models. Imposing the constraint of equal loadings (metric invariance) did not statistically significantly change the fit of the six-factor model in the BANPS (ΔCFI = −0.03). Nevertheless, when we tested the hypothesis regarding equal indicator intercepts (full scalar invariance), there was a significant change in overall model fit (ΔCFI = −0.015), and only partial scalar invariance was shown by releasing several items.
Measurement invariance statistics for the BANPS across sex groups in the Slovenian sample
Measurement invariance model | Estimated parameters | χ 2 (df) | CFI | RMSEA [90% CI] | ΔCFI | ΔRMSEA |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
BANPS | ||||||
Configural (no equality constraints) | 228 | 2,075 (960) | 0.842 | 0.068 [0.064, 0.072] | ||
Metric invariance (loadings) | 234 | 2,127 (987) | 0.838 | 0.068 [0.064, 0.072] | −0.004 | 0.000 |
Scalar invariance (loadings, thresholds) | 240 | 2,264 (1,014) | 0.823 | 0.070 [0.066, 0.074] | −0.015 | 0.002 |
Scalar partial invariance (loadings, thresholds) | 240 | 2,209 (1,012) | 0.830 | 0.069 [0.065, 0.073] | −0.008 | 0.001 |
Note. In the context of each iteration of the ANPS version, the table provides chi-square statistics along with degrees of freedom (df). Additionally, it offers the comparative fit index (CFI) and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), along with 90% confidence intervals (90% CI) for each respective model. The terms ΔCFI and ΔRMSEA denote the discrepancy between the model under evaluation and the previous (less restricted) model.
The process of determining partial scalar invariance was as follows: Upon first inspection of the modification indexes, we found that freeing intercepts for the item BANPS10 (“I often have the feeling that I am going to cry.”) (MI = 28.5) would significantly improve model fit. After doing so, CFI change was still above 0.01. The modification indexes suggested that the intercept for the item BANPS7 (“I rarely become sad.”) was the next source of model misfit. After freeing the intercepts for this item (MI = 24.6), we reevaluated the CFI and RMSEA change of the partial scalar model with the metric model and found a CFI and RMSEA change of 0.008 and 0.001, respectively. Both items BANPS10 and BANPS7 are the constituents of the SADNESS scale. Muthén and Asparouhov (2014) proposed that the results can be considered trustworthy when no more than 25% of the parameters are non-invariant. Accordingly, our results suggest complete configural and metric invariance of the BANPS, as well as partial scalar invariance across sex. It means that the BANPS in the Slovenian sample allows for mean-level comparisons between men and women despite slight differences in the intercepts of observed indicators. The results of the t-tests (Supplement, Table S4) suggest statistically significant sex differences of modest effect sizes in the levels of FEAR, CARE, and SADNESS, with higher scores for women than men across the three scales.
4 Discussion
The purpose of the present study was to examine the psychometric properties of three Slovenian adaptations of the ANPS, i.e., the ANPS 3.1, the BANPS, and the ANPS-S. We collected evidence on their score structure, convergent validity, and internal reliability in a Slovenian sample of emerging adults for the first time. For the version suggesting the relatively best validity evidence in our sample, we further assessed measurement invariance across sex and compared the scale scores between groups of men and women.
4.1 The Score Structure of the Three ANPS Versions
Using confirmatory factor analyses, we first tested the proposed six-factor structure of the primary emotional systems scales for each of the ANPS versions separately. Since the ANPS 3.1 includes two additional scales, we omitted these scale-items from data collection to compare the six-factor models across the versions. The BANPS showed the relatively best construct validity based on the fit indexes. While the RMSEA and SRMR were within the acceptable range across the three measures, the TLI and CFI were fairly low for the ANPS 3.1, and the two indexes for both BANPS and ANPS-S were slightly outside of the common cut-off criteria of 0.90. However, they were notably higher than for the ANPS 3.1.
An overview of the factor loadings (Tables S1–S3 in the Supplementary material) also suggests an advantage of the BANPS over the other two versions. Except for the item 21 (equal to the item 57 in the ANPS 3.1: “I am usually not interested in solving problems and puzzles just for the sake of solving them.”), all other BANPS factor loadings were above 0.40. The lower factor loading of the item 21 is likely due to double negations, which might have caused confusion in the Slovenian language. We recommend a rewording of this item into an affirmative statement in future iterations of the measure in Slovenia, which might improve model fit. In contrast, the factor loadings of six ANPS-S items (of 36) were under 0.40 (a half of them from the CARE scale) and so were 19 items of the ANPS 3.1 (of 84 used in our survey), with half of them belonging to the SADNESS scale. We may conclude that the CARE scale of the ANPS-S and the SADNESS scale of version 3.1 would need additional revision in the Slovenian cultural context.
Our results regarding the construct validity of different ANPS versions are similar to the only other study examining the ANPS 2.4 (as opposed to version 3.1 in our study), the BANPS, and the ANPS-S simultaneously (Pedersen et al., 2014). In a sample of patients with PDs, these authors also reported an advantage of the BANPS over both the ANPS-S and the ANPS 2.4. However, the BANPS fit indexes in their study appear slightly better than ours, perhaps partly due to a potentially more pronounced emotionality in the clinical sample relative to our community sample.
To our knowledge, the present study marks the first psychometric validation of the ANPS 3.1. Considering a reduced number of items that presumably reflect six factors (instead of 112 items of the full instrument that includes additional Spirituality and Social Dominance factors), we obtained a poorer model fit of the 3.1 version compared to each of the two short versions. The lower values of the TLI and CFI, metrics for incremental fit, are likely attributable to the increased complexity of the model which is a consequence of the larger number of items (84). Larger measures tend to be more susceptible to local misspecification issues due to heightened model complexity. In parallel to Pedersen et al. (2014) and Pingault et al. (2012b) who highlighted deficiencies in the two incremental fit indexes for the ANPS 2.4, caution is needed when employing the 84-item ANPS 3.1 in Slovenia. However, the full 3.1 version is yet to be tested for the proposed eight-factor model. While acknowledging its limitations, it is also of note that the reliability and convergent validity evidence suggest good psychometric properties of the six primary scales of the instrument. We recommend an improvement of the ANPS 3.1 items of the six scales and utilization of shorter measures to assess the basic emotions, at least in Slovenia.
4.2 Correlations Between the Corresponding Scales Across the Three Versions and Scale Intercorrelations Within Each Version
Correlations between the corresponding scale scores of the three ANPS versions were all high (predominantly over 0.80), suggesting a strong overlap between the scale scores that were supposed to measure the same primary emotional systems expression. This was not surprising because all of the ANPS-S items and a majority of the BANPS items are subsumed in version 3.1.
Mostly low and a few moderate intercorrelations (with one exception) between different scale scores within each of the ANPS versions under our study are in line with the theory (Panksepp, 1998, 2011) that diverse primary emotions evolved as distinct responses to environmental challenges, as well as with the previous research outcomes (e.g., Davis et al., 2003; Pedersen et al., 2014; Pingault et al., 2012a). An inspection of the number and the magnitude of these divergent correlations between the scale scores within each ANPS version (Tables 3–5) again suggests an advantage of both short versions over version 3.1. However, we revealed high correlations between the FEAR and the SADNESS scale scores across the ANPS versions. This finding has been noted since the first iterations of ANPS development (Davis et al., 2003; see also Pahlavan et al., 2008; Pedersen et al., 2014; Pingault et al., 2012b) and has been discussed by Montag et al. (2022) who proposed several explanations. First, the elevated correlations may stem from the comorbidity of anxiety, a characteristic shared by both fear and sadness. Second, the shared sensitivity to pain may contribute to the considerable FEAR-SADNESS overlap, aligning with the shared neurobiological wiring of pain activation in the brain (MacDonald & Leary, 2005; Panksepp, 1998). Third, the close association between physical and physiological pain systems and the high comorbidity between anxiety and mood disorders (e.g., 80% of adults with depression reported significant anxiety symptoms; Gorman, 1996) may further explain the clear FEAR-SADNESS correlations. To address this issue in the ANPS improvements, excluding cognitive aspects from the SADNESS scale and adding items related to flight tendencies to the FEAR scale could mitigate their overlap.
Across the ANPS versions, we also found substantial associations between each of the primary positive emotional systems scores and each of the primary negative emotional systems scores with the composite scores of general positive affect and general negative affect, respectively, adding to the validity of the superordinate scores.
4.3 Reliability and Convergent Validity Evidence
All of the primary emotional systems scale scores and the composite General Affect scores had acceptable internal reliability for each of the ANPS versions, with relatively the lowest estimates for the BANPS CARE scale and the ANPS-S SEEKING scale. In general, the alpha coefficients appear somewhat higher for the 3.1 version, possibly due to a greater number of items per scale. To improve the (still acceptable) reliability of the respective scales of the two short forms, we do not suggest an increase in the number of items, but a detailed reliability analysis which, however, exceeds the aims of this paper. For example, a slightly lower reliability of the SEEKING scale was noticed in our study and had also been reported previously (Pingault et al., 2012a). This may suggest that the scale contains items that measure two somewhat different aspects of seeking behavior, i.e., curiosity and enjoyment in problem-solving.
Convergent validity evidence was demonstrated by the correlations of self-perceived expression of the primary emotions with four of the Big Five personality traits, as well as by the associations of the positive and negative scale scores (including the respective composite general affect scores) with the self-reported positive and negative emotional experiences, respectively. However, the convergence between negative emotions (FEAR, ANGER, SADNESS, and general negative affect) and recent negative emotional experiences was notably stronger (moderate to high correlations) than those between the positive emotions (SEEKING, PLAY, CARE, and general positive affect) and recent positive emotional experiences (modest to moderate effect size). This result is likely due to a greater similarity between the items assessing negative experiences and the negative primary emotional systems. The experiences of sadness, fear, and anger are all contained in the SPANE (Diener et al., 2009), while the positive experiences related directly to CARE, PLAY, and SEEKING are not. The ANPS and their composite scores are also considered trait-like measures of emotion (Montag et al., 2021), while the SPANE scores represent a more “state-like” measure that is less stable over time as it refers to the frequency of emotions felt in the past four weeks.
Consistent with the ANT hypothesis that four of the robust personality traits might be based on the activity of subcortical affective systems (Davis & Panksepp, 2018; Montag & Davis, 2018), we found evidence for substantial and differential links between the ANPS (versions 3.1, B, and S) and the respective Big Five scale scores. Our results concur with those observed in other studies using different versions of the ANPS and different personality measures (e.g., Giacolini et al., 2017; Montag et al., 2019; Montag & Panksepp, 2017), including the meta-analysis (Marengo et al., 2021). Precisely, we established associations of SEEKING with openness, PLAY with extraversion, FEAR/ANGER/SADNESS with neuroticism, CARE with agreeableness, and low ANGER with agreeableness. Activation of the SEEKING system is marked by heightened motivation and enthusiasm, fostering a proclivity for novelty and exploration (Davis & Panksepp, 2011; Panksepp, 1998). This aligns with openness, as seen in individuals of an exploratory nature and attracted to novelty (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Heightened ANGER levels often accompany marker traits associated with low agreeableness, manifested in increased aggression and conflict (Bettencourt et al., 2006). The CARE system, characterized by a tendency to nurture and attend to the needs of others aligns (Panksepp, 1998) with characteristics of agreeableness (e.g., altruism, cooperation, and consideration; John et al., 2008). Although to a lesser extent, we found CARE positively related to extraversion, too (see also Marengo et al., 2021), presumably because both traits directly refer to the interpersonal aspect of personality. Elevated FEAR levels correlate with an active behavioral inhibition system (Gray, 1987), leading to heightened anxiety and avoidance – the key components of neuroticism (Barlow et al., 2014). Increased SADNESS and grief levels are also captured by neuroticism, along with a heightened irritability and anger proneness (John et al., 2008), exhibited in heightened levels of ANGER. The PLAY system which activates feelings of joy and happiness (Davis & Panksepp, 2011; Panksepp, 1998) represents the biological basis of extraversion, the trait described largely by an inclination towards positive emotionality and social interaction (John et al., 2008).
In addition to these convergences, we found extraversion moderately associated with SEEKING (also reported by Marengo et al., 2021), presumably due to their shared neurochemistry involved in reward processing (Smillie, 2013). Similar to previous findings (e.g., Barrett et al., 2013; Marengo et al., 2021), we found FEAR and SADNESS more strongly related to Neuroticism than ANGER. This might be due to a conceptual and anatomical overlap between the SADNESS and the FEAR systems and their relation to pain (MacDonald & Leary, 2005; Panksepp, 1998), while the ANGER system has been shown to have distinct neurochemistry (Panksepp, 1998). In support of the convergent validity of the three ANPS versions, we further revealed considerably lower shares of common variance between the primary emotional systems and conscientiousness, the trait which reflects the most cerebrally focused personality trait among the Big Five.
4.4 Generalizability Evidence of the BANPS Across Sex and Mean-Score Sex Differences
Taken together, the BANPS showed relatively better psychometric properties (in particular construct validity) among the three ANPS versions employed in the Slovenian sample. Therefore, we further assessed its measurement invariance across sex to establish whether the six-factor model is sufficiently equivalent between men and women to claim that the constructs have the same meaning in both groups which would allow us to test for the sex differences in the mean scale scores. To our knowledge, measurement invariance of the BANPS across different groups has not been reported yet, whereas Orri et al. (2018) examined it for the ANPS-S, both across sex and over a 4-year time interval. Much like their results, the configural model in our study showed a good fit for the RMSEA and SRMR, while the CFI score was slightly lower. The results of multiple-group CFAs supported the hypothesis of metric invariance of the BANPS across sex (the latent factors were equally associated with the items in both groups). Testing for scalar invariance suggested partial scalar invariance. As less than 25% of the intercepts was nonvariant (Muthén & Asparouhov, 2014) and due to the common consensus that equality of factor loadings is more crucial for establishing measurement invariance than equality of item intercepts (Meade & Kroustalis, 2006), our results still allow valid comparisons of the mean scale scores across sex.
The consequent group comparison analysis showed modest sex differences, with women reporting higher levels of FEAR, CARE, and SADNESS. Similarly, studies using the ANPS-S (Orri et al., 2018; Pingault et al., 2012a) and the ANPS 2.4 (Abella et al., 2011; Orri et al., 2018; Pingault et al., 2012b) established the strongest differences (“favoring” women) in these scale scores. These outcomes are in line with demonstrations of a greater inclination for nurturing behaviors among women (Davis et al., 2003; Derntl et al., 2010), as well as a higher prevalence of depressive and anxious feelings in women than men (McLean & Anderson, 2009; Parker & Brotchie, 2010). However, sex differences in the primary emotions also seem to be subject to cross-cultural differences (Özkarar-Gradwohl et al., 2014).
4.5 Limitations, Conclusions, and Future Directions
There are several limitations to this study. Similar to previous ANPS validation studies (Barrett et al., 2013; Davis et al., 2003; Pingault et al., 2012a), the present study featured a community sample consisting of predominantly emerging adults (most of them students) and young adults. Consequently, we were not able to definitively prove that the structure of the six primary emotional systems remains stable across subsequent developmental periods in adulthood. Regardless we assume the likelihood of significant change in mean levels of emotional expression and/or significant changes in rank-order stability of individuals’ traits with age is low. We aimed to compare the validity evidence among three versions of the ANPS that include massively overlapping items. Therefore, we only applied one questionnaire with 89 items (those of version 3.1 referring to the six primary emotional systems and five items of the BANPS that differ from both versions 3.1 and S). We did so to avoid response fatigue and potential confusion among the respondents, which could have contributed to greater measurement error. In line with the ANPS 3.1 scoring, we also used a six-point rating scale across the items (note that the ANPS-S and the BANPS include a five-point scale). Administering the short ANPS forms as separate questionnaires and original rating scales may have, at least to some extent, affected the score structure as well.
Despite some issues with the incremental fit indexes of the BANPS, the troublesome factor loading of item 21 (likely due to the double negation), and relatively low (although still falling within the lower ranges of acceptability) (George & Mallery, 2003) internal consistency of the CARE scale, we consider the Slovenian adaptation of this short form for further research in the respective social-cultural context acceptable. In contrast, several modifications are warranted to the ANPS-S and, in particular, to the ANPS 3.1 (after validating its full version with two additional factors).
Regarding the future of the ANP scales, they would benefit from ameliorations focused on their psychometric properties (e.g., reducing the overlap between FEAR and SADNESS scales, increasing the reliability of the CARE and SEEKING scales in the short ANPS forms), shortenings of the full forms, incorporation of the LUST scale, and creations of alternative “state” instead of trait-measures (Montag et al., 2022). Examples of the LUST scale have already been constructed (Fuchshuber et al., 2022), but the full ANPS 3.1 that additionally includes factors of spirituality and social dominance still awaits validation. Nonetheless, an increasing number of studies have recently employed the ANPS measures across a variety of domains, as mentioned in the Introduction. This might be partly due to an advantage of the ANPS over other measures of personality traits. Most notably, the primary emotions are neurobiologically grounded and are thus likely to provide the evolutionary foundation of the robust personality traits (Davis & Panksepp, 2018; Marengo et al., 2021; Montag et al., 2021). Consequently, they pave the way for studies, which might help elucidate human personality on a neuroanatomical, neurochemical, neurophysiological, and molecular genetic level. The ANPS has also received attention from the growing movement of neuropsychoanalysis (Panksepp & Solms, 2012; Solms & Panksepp, 2012) and their employment suggested results that are consistent with certain psychoanalytic hypotheses, such as those of drive theory (the primary emotions are thought to represent the instinctual drives) and attachment theory (the SADNESS and the CARE systems are assumed to provide foundations of attachment) (Solms & Turnbull, 2011). The psychometrically improved ANPS measures can thus offer valuable assessment tools in diverse areas of research on the human mind.
Acknowledgements
The authors gratefully acknowledge all the participants who took part in the study and extend special thanks to Mr. Martin Cregeen for his assistance in the translation process as well as Prof. Gregor Sočan for providing valuable methodological advice.
-
Funding information: This study was supported by the Slovenian Research and Innovation Agency (research core funding No. P5-0062).
-
Author contributions: TG and MZ contributed to the conceptualization and study design. TG collected and analyzed the data. TG drafted the manuscript, which was then proofread and revised by MZ. The authors applied the “first-last-author-emphasis” (FLAE) norm approach for the sequence of authors.
-
Conflict of interest: The authors state no conflict of interest.
-
Data availability statement: The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available in the Open Science Framework repository https://osf.io/rs8w6/.
References
Abella, V., Panksepp, J., Manga, D., Bárcena, C., & Iglesias, J. A. (2011). Spanish validation of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 14(2), 926–935. doi: 10.5209/rev_SJOP.2011.v14.n2.38.Search in Google Scholar
Amiri, E., & Azad-Marzabadi (2017). Validation of Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales in normal populations. NeuropsychiatriaiNeuropsychologia/Neuropsychiatry and Neuropsychology, 12(3), 95–100. doi: 10.5114/nan.2017.71665.Search in Google Scholar
Avsec, A., & Sočan, G. (2007). Vprašalnik petih velikih faktorjev BFI [The Big Five inventory BFI]. In A. Avsec (Ed.), Psihodiagnostika osebnosti [The psychodiagnostics of personality] (pp. 171–178). University of Ljubljana.Search in Google Scholar
Barlow, D. H., Ellard, K. K., Sauer-Zavala, S., Bullis, J. R., & Carl, J. R. (2014). The origins of neuroticism. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(5), 481–496. doi: 10.1177/1745691614544528.Search in Google Scholar
Barrett, F. S., Robins, R. W., & Janata, P. (2013). A brief form of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. Psychological Assessment, 25(3), 826–843. doi: 10.1037/a0032576.Search in Google Scholar
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 238–246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238.Search in Google Scholar
Bettencourt, B., Talley, A., Benjamin, A. J., & Valentine, J. (2006). Personality and aggressive behavior under provoking and neutral conditions: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 132(5), 751–777. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.751.Search in Google Scholar
Boyle, G. J. (2008). Critique of the five-factor model of personality. In G. J. Boyle, G. Matthews, & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.), The Sage handbook of personality theory and testing: Volume 1. Personality theories and models (pp. 295–312). Sage. doi: 10.4135/9781849200462.Search in Google Scholar
Carré, A., Chevallier, C., Robel, L., Barry, C., Maria, A. S., Pouga, L., Philippe, A., Pinabel, F., & Berthoz, S. (2015). Tracking social motivation systems deficits: The affective neuroscience view of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 3351–3363. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2498-2.Search in Google Scholar
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9(2), 233–255. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_5.Search in Google Scholar
Coenen, V. A., Schlaepfer, T. E., Maedler, B., & Panksepp, J. (2011). Cross-species affective functions of the medial forebrain bundle—Implications for the treatment of affective pain and depression in humans. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(9), 1971–1981. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.009.Search in Google Scholar
Cory, G. A., & Gardner, R. (Eds.). (2002). The evolutionary neuroethology of Paul MacLean: Convergences and frontiers. Greenwood Publishing Group.Search in Google Scholar
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory manual. Psychological Assessment Resources.Search in Google Scholar
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO personality inventory-revised (NEO PI-R). Psychological Assessment Resources.Search in Google Scholar
Costa, P. T., & Widiger, T. A. (2002). Introduction: Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality. In P. T. Costa, Jr. & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (pp. 3–14). American Psychological Association. doi: 10.1037/10423-001.Search in Google Scholar
Cwojdzińska, A., & Rybakowski, F. (2015). Operationalization of Jaak Panksepp’s concept of brain emotional systems–Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. Neuropsychiatria i Neuropsychologia/Neuropsychiatry and Neuropsychology, 10(3), 102–109.Search in Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1965). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1872).Search in Google Scholar
Davis, K. L., & Panksepp, J. (2011). The brain’s emotional foundations of human personality and the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(9), 1946–1958. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.04.004.Search in Google Scholar
Davis, K. L., & Panksepp, J. (2018). The emotional foundations of personality: A neurobiological and evolutionary approach. WW Norton & Company.Search in Google Scholar
Davis, K. L., Panksepp, J., & Normansell, L. (2003). The Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales: Normative data and implications. Neuropsychoanalysis, 5(1), 57–69. doi: 10.1080/15294145.2003.10773410.Search in Google Scholar
Deris, N., Montag, C., Reuter, M., Weber, B., & Markett, S. (2017). Functional connectivity in the resting brain as biological correlate of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. NeuroImage, 147, 423–431. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.063.Search in Google Scholar
Derntl, B., Finkelmeyer, A., Eickhoff, S., Kellermann, T., Falkenberg, D. I., Schneider, F., & Habel, U. (2010). Multidimensional assessment of empathic abilities: Neural correlates and gender differences. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35(1), 67–82. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.10.006.Search in Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Grazioplene, R. G., & Allen, T. A. (2021). The neurobiology of personality. In O. P. John & R. W. Robins (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 193–216). The Guilford Press.Search in Google Scholar
DeYoung, C. G., Hirsh, J. B., Shane, M. S., Papademetris, X., Rajeevan, N., & Gray, J. R. (2010). Testing predictions from personality neuroscience: Brain structure and the big five. Psychological Science, 21(6), 820-828. doi: 10.1177/0956797610370159.Search in Google Scholar
Diener, E., Wirtz, D., Biswas-Diener, R., Tov, W., Kim-Prieto, C., Choi, D. W., & Oishi, S. (2009). New measures of well-being. Assessing well-being (pp. 247-266). Springer.Search in Google Scholar
Eaves, L. J., Eysenck, H. J., & Martin, N. G. (1989). Genes, culture and personality: An empirical approach. Academic Press.Search in Google Scholar
Espejo, B., Checa, I., Perales-Puchalt, J., & Lisón, J. F. (2020). Validation and measurement invariance of the scale of positive and negative experience (SPANE) in a Spanish general sample. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(22), 8359. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17228359.Search in Google Scholar
Esposito, S. B., Fonseca, M. S. M., Mileo, R. A., & Gurfinkel, Y. (2016). Tradução para o português e adaptação transcultural da Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales-brief. Revista da Faculdade de Ciências Médicas de Sorocaba, 18(Sup1), 81. doi: 10.23925/1984-4840.2018v20i4a8.Search in Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1994). Personality: Biological foundations. In P. A. Vernon (Ed.), The neuropsychology of individual differences. Academic Press.Search in Google Scholar
Felten, A., Montag, C., Markett, S., Walter, N. T., & Reuter, M. (2011). Genetically determined dopamine availability predicts disposition for depression. Brain and Behavior, 1(2), 109–118. doi: 10.1002/brb3.20.Search in Google Scholar
Fuchshuber, J., Hiebler-Ragger, M., Kresse, A., Kapfhammer, H. P., & Unterrainer, H. F. (2019a). Do primary emotions predict psychopathological symptoms? A multigroup path analysis. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 610. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00610.Search in Google Scholar
Fuchshuber, J., Hiebler-Ragger, M., Kresse, A., Kapfhammer, H. P., & Unterrainer, H. F. (2019b). The influence of attachment styles and personality organization on emotional functioning after childhood trauma. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 643. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00643.Search in Google Scholar
Fuchshuber, J., Jauk, E., Hiebler-Ragger, M., & Unterrainer, H. F. (2022). The affective neuroscience of sexuality: Development of a LUST scale. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 16, 853706. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.853706.Search in Google Scholar
George, D., & Mallery, P. (2003). SPSS for Windows step by step: A simple guide and reference. 11.0 update (4th ed.). Allyn & Bacon.Search in Google Scholar
Giacolini, T., Ardizzone, I., Davis, K. L., Ferrara, M., Picconi, L., Terrinoni, A., & Sabatello, U. (2017). Brain emotional systems: The Italian version of the ANPS-Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales 2.4 (reliability and validity). Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 14(4), 263–274.Search in Google Scholar
Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative” description of personality”: The Big-Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(6), 1216–1229. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.59.6.1216.Search in Google Scholar
Gorman, J. M. (1996). Comorbid depression and anxiety spectrum disorders. Depression and Anxiety, 4, 160–168. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)15206394(1996)4:4<160:AID-DA2>3.0.CO;2-J.Search in Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress (Vol. 5). CUP Archive.Search in Google Scholar
Gurfinkel, Y., Mileo, R. A., da Fonseca, M. S., & Esposito, S. B. (2018), Traduçãopara o português e adaptação transcultural da Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales—Brief. Revista da Faculdade de CiênciasMédicas de Sorocaba, 20 (4) 223–229. doi: 10.23925/1984-4840.2018v20i4a8.Search in Google Scholar
Harro, J., Laas, K., Eensoo, D., Kurrikoff, T., Sakala, K., Vaht, T., Parik, J., Maestu J., & Veidebaum, T. (2019). Orexin/hypocretin receptor gene (HCRTR1) variation is associated with aggressive behaviour. Neuropharmacology, 156, 107527. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2019.02.009.Search in Google Scholar
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118.Search in Google Scholar
IBM Corp. Released 2017. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, (Version 25.0). [Computer software]. IBM Corp.Search in Google Scholar
Jarusiewicz, B. (2000). Spirituality and addiction: Relationship to recovery and relapse. Alcoholism Treatment Quarterly, 18(4), 99–109. doi: 10.1300/J020v18n04_08.Search in Google Scholar
John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. L. (1991). The big five inventory. Versions 4a and 54. University of California.Search in Google Scholar
John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative Big Five trait taxonomy. Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research (pp. 114–158). Guilford Press.Search in Google Scholar
Jovanović, V. (2015). Beyond the PANAS: Incremental validity of the Scale of Positive and Negative Experience (SPANE) in relation to well-being. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 487–491. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.015.Search in Google Scholar
Karterud, S., Pedersen, G., Johansen, M., Wilberg, T., Davis, K., & Panksepp, J. (2016). Primary emotional traits in patients with personality disorders. Personality and Mental Health, 10(4), 261–273. doi: 10.1002/pmh.1345.Search in Google Scholar
MacDonald, G., & Leary, M. R. (2005). Why does social exclusion hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain. Psychological Bulletin, 131(2), 202–223. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.202.Search in Google Scholar
Marengo, D., Davis, K. L., Gradwohl, G. Ö., & Montag, C. (2021). A meta-analysis on individual differences in primary emotional systems and Big Five personality traits. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-84366-8.Search in Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal. American Psychologist, 52(5), 509–516. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.52.5.509.Search in Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., Scally, M., Terracciano, A., Abecasis, G. R., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (2010). An alternative to the search for single polymorphisms: Toward molecular personality scales for the five-factor model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(6), 1014–1024.Search in Google Scholar
McCrae, R. R., Terracciano, A., & 78 Members of the Personality Profiles of Cultures Project. (2005). Universal features of personality traits from the observer’s perspective: Data from 50 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(3), 547–561. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.547.Search in Google Scholar
McLean, C. P., & Anderson, E. R. (2009). Brave men and timid women? A review of the gender differences in fear and anxiety. Clinical Psychology Review, 29(6), 496–505. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.05.003.Search in Google Scholar
McLean, J. H. (1990). Neolepetopsidae, a new docoglossate limpet family from hydrothermal vents and its relevance to patellogastropod evolution. Journal of Zoology, 222(3), 485–528. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7998.1990.tb04047.x.Search in Google Scholar
Meade, A. W., & Kroustalis, C. M. (2006). Problems with item parceling for confirmatory factor analytic tests of measurement invariance. Organizational Research Methods, 9(3), 369–403. doi: 10.1177/1094428105283384.Search in Google Scholar
Montag, C., & Davis, K. L. (2018). Affective neuroscience theory and personality: An update. Personality Neuroscience, 1, e12. doi: 10.1017/pen.2018.10.Search in Google Scholar
Montag, C., Davis, K. L., Lazarević, L. B., & Knezević, G. (2019). A Serbian version of the ANPS and its link to the five-factor model of personality. Open Psychology, 1(1), 303–316. doi: 10.1515/psych-2018-0019.Search in Google Scholar
Montag, C., Elhai, J. D., & Davis, K. L. (2021). A comprehensive review of studies using the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales in the psychological and psychiatric sciences. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 125, 160–167. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.02.019.Search in Google Scholar
Montag, C., Fiebach, C. J., Kirsch, P., & Reuter, M. (2011). Interaction of 5-HTTLPR and a variation on the oxytocin receptor gene influences negative emotionality. Biological Psychiatry, 69(6), 601–603. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.10.026.Search in Google Scholar
Montag, C., Hahn, E., Reuter, M., Spinath, F. M., Davis, K., & Panksepp, J. (2016a). The role of nature and nurture for individual differences in primary emotional systems: Evidence from a twin study. PloS One, 11(3), e0151405. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0157200.Search in Google Scholar
Montag, C., Hahn, E., Reuter, M., Spinath, F. M., Davis, K., & Panksepp, J. (2016b). Correction: The role of nature and nurture for individual differences in primary emotional systems: Evidence from a twin study. PloS One, 11(6), e0157200. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0157200.Search in Google Scholar
Montag, C., & Panksepp, J. (2017). Primary emotional systems and personality: An evolutionary perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 464. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00464.Search in Google Scholar
Montag, C., Solms, M., Stelzel, C., & Davis, K. L. (2022). The future of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales: A reflection on seven pressing matters. Personality Neuroscience, 5, e10. doi: 10.1017/pen.2022.2.Search in Google Scholar
Montag, C., Widenhorn-Müller, K., Panksepp, J., & Kiefer, M. (2017). Individual differences in Affective Neuroscience Personality Scale (ANPS) primary emotional traits and depressive tendencies. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 73, 136–142. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.11.007.Search in Google Scholar
Muthén, B., & Asparouhov, T. (2014). IRT studies of many groups: The alignment method. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 978. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00978.Search in Google Scholar
Narita K., Hatta, T., Hirao, K., Mitamura, T., Yama, M., & Yokode, M. (2017), The validity and reliability in Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales Japanese edition: An approach to human personality from the affective neuroscience. Japanese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 17, 691–702.Search in Google Scholar
Orri, M., Rouquette, A., Pingault, J. B., Barry, C., Herba, C., Côté, S. M., & Berthoz, S. (2018). Longitudinal and sex measurement invariance of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. Assessment, 25(5), 653–666. doi: 10.1177/1073191116656795.Search in Google Scholar
Özkarar-Gradwohl, F. G., Panksepp, J., İçöz, F. J., Çetinkaya, H., Köksal, F., Davis, K. L., & Scherler N. (2014). The influence of culture on basic affective systems: The comparison of Turkish and American norms on the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. Culture and Brain, 2, 173–192. doi: 10.1007/s40167-014-0021-9.Search in Google Scholar
Pahlavan, F., Mouchiroud, C., Zenasni, F., & Panksepp, J. (2008). French validation of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS). European Review of Applied Psychology, 58(3), 155–163. doi: 10.1016/j.erap.2007.08.004.Search in Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2006). Emotional endophenotypes in evolutionary psychiatry. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 30(5), 774–784. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.01.004.Search in Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2011). Cross-species affective neuroscience decoding of the primal affective experiences of humans and related animals. PloS One, 6(9), e21236. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021236.Search in Google Scholar
Panksepp, J., & Solms, M. (2012). What is neuropsychoanalysis? Clinically relevant studies of the minded brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(1), 6–8. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.005.Search in Google Scholar
Parker, G., & Brotchie, H. (2010). Gender differences in depression. International Review of Psychiatry, 22(5), 429-436. doi: 10.3109/09540261.2010.492391.Search in Google Scholar
Pascazio, L., Bembich, S., Nardone, I. B., Vecchiet, C., Guarino, G., & Clarici, A. (2015). Validation of the Italian translation of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. Psychological Reports, 116(1), 97–115. doi: 10.2466/08.09.PR0.116k13w4.Search in Google Scholar
Pedersen, G., Selsbakk J.M., Theresa W., & Sigmund K. (2014). Testing different versions of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales in a clinical sample. PLoS One, 9(10), Article e109394. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0114449.Search in Google Scholar
Pingault, J. B., Falissard, B., Côté, S., & Berthoz, S. (2012a). A new approach of personality and psychiatric disorders: A short version of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales. PLoS One, 7(7), e41489. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0041489.Search in Google Scholar
Pingault, J. B., Pouga, L., Grezes, J., & Berthoz, S. (2012b). Determination of emotional endophenotypes: A validation of the Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales and further perspectives. Psychological Assessment, 24(2), 375–385. doi: 10.1037/a0025692.Search in Google Scholar
Plomin, R. (1990). The role of inheritance in behavior. Science, 248(4952), 183–188. doi: 10.1126/science.2183351.Search in Google Scholar
Power, R. A., & Pluess, M. (2015). Heritability estimates of the Big Five personality traits based on common genetic variants. Translational Psychiatry, 5(7), e604. doi: 10.1038/tp.2015.96.Search in Google Scholar
R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/Reuter.Search in Google Scholar
Reuter, M., Panksepp, J., Davis, K., & Montag, C. (2017). Affective(ANPS) – Deutsche Version. Hogrefe.Search in Google Scholar
Reuter, M., Weber, B., Fiebach, C. J., Elger, C., & Montag, C. (2009). The biological basis of anger: Associations with the gene coding for DARPP-32 (PPP1R1B) and with amygdala volume. Behavioural Brain Research, 202(2), 179–183. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.032.Search in Google Scholar
Saucier, G., & Goldberg, L. R. (2006). Personnalité, caractère et tempérament: la structure translinguistique des traits. Psychologie Française, 51(3), 265–284. doi: 10.1016/j.psfr.2006.01.005.Search in Google Scholar
Sindermann, C., Luo, R., Zhao, Z., Li Q., Li, M., Kendrick, K.M., Panksepp, J., & Montag, C. (2018). High ANGER and low agreeableness predict vengefulness in German and Chinese participants. Personality and Individual Differences, 121, 184–192. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.09.004.Search in Google Scholar
Smillie, L. D. (2013). Extraversion and reward processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(3), 167–172. doi: 10.1177/0963721412470133.Search in Google Scholar
Solms, M., & Panksepp, J. (2012). The “Id” knows more than the “Ego” admits: Neuropsychoanalytic and primal consciousness perspectives on the interface between affective and cognitive neuroscience. Brain Sciences, 2(2), 147–175. doi: 10.3390/brainsci2020147.Search in Google Scholar
Solms, M., & Turnbull, O. H. (2011). What is neuropsychoanalysis? Neuropsychoanalysis, 13(2), 133–145. doi: 10.1080/15294145.2011.10773670.Search in Google Scholar
Steiger, J. H., & Lind, J. C. (1980, May). Statistically-based tests for the number of common factors. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Psychometric Society, Iowa City, IA.Search in Google Scholar
Tomažič, T. (2021). Priredba štirih lestvic psihičnega blagostanja [An adaptation of four measures of psychological well-being] (Unpublished master’s thesis). University of Ljubljana.Search in Google Scholar
Tucker, L. R., & Lewis, C. (1973). A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika, 38(1), 1–10. doi: 10.1007/BF02291170.Search in Google Scholar
Unterrainer, H. F., Hiebler-Ragger, M., Koschutnig, K., Fuchshuber, J., Tscheschner, S., Url, M., Wagner-Skacel, J., Reininghaus, E. Z., Papousek, I., Weiss, E. M., & Fink, A. (2017). Addiction as an attachment disorder: White matter impairment is linked to increased negative affective states in poly-drug use. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 208. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00208.Search in Google Scholar
van der Westhuizen, D., & Solms, M. (2015). Social dominance and the affective neuroscience personality scales. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 90-111. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.12.005.Search in Google Scholar
Wernicke, J., Li, M., Sha, P., Zhou, M., Sindermann, C., Becker, B., Kendrick, K. & Montag, C. (2019). Individual differences in tendencies to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and emotionality: Empirical evidence in young healthy adults from Germany and China. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 11(2), 167–182. doi: 10.1007/s12402-018-0266-9.Search in Google Scholar
Yarkoni, T. (2015). Neurobiological substrates of personality: A critical overview. In APA handbook of personality and social psychology, Volume 4: Personality processes and individual differences (pp. 61–83). American Psychological Association.Search in Google Scholar
Zupančič, M., & Kavčič, T. (2017). Personality, life events, and three components of subsequent subjective wellbeing in female university students. Social Inquiry into Well-Being, 3(1), 1–14.Search in Google Scholar
© 2024 the author(s), published by De Gruyter
This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Articles in the same Issue
- Research Articles
- Speed and Reliability of Touch vs Mouse Inputs in Visual Foraging
- The Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS) in Slovenia: Validation of the Six Primary Scales Across Three ANPS Versions
- Brief Reports
- Features of Two Embodied Processes in Spatial Perspective-Taking Across the Lifespan
- “I Have Some Serious Doubts About this Vaccine…” – Generic Conspiracy Beliefs Predict the Acceptance of the Covid-19 Vaccination
Articles in the same Issue
- Research Articles
- Speed and Reliability of Touch vs Mouse Inputs in Visual Foraging
- The Affective Neuroscience Personality Scales (ANPS) in Slovenia: Validation of the Six Primary Scales Across Three ANPS Versions
- Brief Reports
- Features of Two Embodied Processes in Spatial Perspective-Taking Across the Lifespan
- “I Have Some Serious Doubts About this Vaccine…” – Generic Conspiracy Beliefs Predict the Acceptance of the Covid-19 Vaccination