Home Twin fetal facial expressions at 30–33+6 weeks of gestation
Article Publicly Available

Twin fetal facial expressions at 30–33+6 weeks of gestation

  • Emiko Nitta , Kenji Kanenishi , Nobuhiro Mori , Mohamed Ahmed Mostafa AboEllail and Toshiyuki Hata EMAIL logo
Published/Copyright: October 17, 2019

Abstract

Objective

To assess the characteristics of twin fetal facial expressions at 30–33 + 6 weeks of gestation using four-dimensional (4D) ultrasound to clarify twin fetal brain development and maturation.

Methods

Frequencies of seven fetal facial expressions were studied using 4D ultrasound for 15 minutes in 30 singleton pregnancies and 18 twin pregnancies [four monochorionic diamniotic (MD) and 14 dichorionic diamniotic (DD) twins] at 30–33 + 6 weeks of gestation. Comparison of the frequency in each facial expression was performed between singleton and twin fetuses.

Results

Mouthing was the most frequent facial expression at 30–33 + 6 weeks of gestation, followed by blinking in twin and singleton fetuses. Both facial expressions were significantly more frequent than other expressions (P < 0.05). The frequencies of mouthing and scowling in twin fetuses were significantly lower than those in singleton fetuses, but there were no significant differences in the frequencies of the five other facial expressions between the fetal groups.

Conclusion

Our results suggest that restricted twin fetal behavior before 20 weeks of gestation may still affect the frequencies of twin facial expressions early in the third trimester of pregnancy. Moreover, the frequencies of facial expressions in twins are different from those of singleton fetuses.

Introduction

Degani et al. [1] studied fetal temperament in twins late in the first trimester of pregnancy and found that differences in activity during this period were followed by temperamental differences postnatally. In our previous studies [2], [3], twin fetuses showed less frequent arm movements compared with singleton fetuses at 12–13 weeks, while the frequencies of eight fetal movements (head anteflexion, head retroflexion, body rotation, hand-to-face movement, general movement, arm movement, leg movement, and mouthing) in twins were significantly lower than those in singleton fetuses at 14–19 weeks. The reason for these differences in fetal movements between singleton and twin fetuses in currently unknown. One possible explanation is the restricted space available to twins in-utero. Mulder et al. [4] also found that twin fetuses were less active regarding general movement than singletons during gestation. Therefore, restricted space before 20 weeks in-utero may affect fetal behavioral movements in twin pregnancies. A question raised here is whether these neurobehavioral restrictions in twin fetuses persist in the third trimester of pregnancy.

Four-dimensional (4D) ultrasound observation of fetal behavior has been used to indirectly assess developmental maturation of the fetal brain and central nervous system (CNS) developmental stages of fetal neurobehavior [5], [6]. Behavior is a product of the functioning CNS; therefore, by investigating fetal behavior, it is possible to make a deduction regarding the functioning of the fetal brain [7]. Fetal facial expressions can mirror the fetal brain function and development in the latter half of pregnancy [8], [9]. These expressions suggest the maturation and development of different brain and CNS, which control these actions [9]. There have been numerous studies on 4D ultrasound assessment of fetal facial expressions [10], [11], [12], [13], [14], [15]. To the best of our knowledge, however, there has been no study on 4D ultrasound assessment of twin fetal facial expressions.

In healthy appropriate-for-gestational-age (AGA) fetuses, the frequency of mouthing movement assessed by 4D ultrasound was significantly higher than those of six other facial expressions (yawning, smiling, tongue expulsion, scowling, sucking, and blinking) early in the third trimester of pregnancy [12], [15]. However, mouthing was the most frequent facial expression, but there was no significant difference between mouthing and blinking in fetal growth restriction (FGR) [16]. There has been no investigation on 4D ultrasound assessment of the frequency of twin fetal facial expressions in the third trimester of pregnancy.

According to a 4D ultrasound study, the coordination of different parts of the fetal brain controlling facial expressions became apparent after 36 weeks of gestation in healthy AGA fetuses [17]. However, the control of facial expressions by the brain in FGR fetuses may be most evident compared with AGA fetuses at 32–35 weeks of gestation due to the acceleration of neurological maturation and development [18]. A question raised here is whether the control of facial expressions by the brain in twin fetuses is apparent compared with singleton fetuses early in the third trimester of pregnancy, because twin fetal weight is lower than that of singleton fetuses.

In the present study, we evaluated the characteristics of twin fetal facial expressions at 30–33+6 weeks of gestation using 4D ultrasound to clarify twin fetal brain development and maturation.

Subjects and methods

This study was conducted from April 2016 to December 2018. Pregnant Japanese women, who were at 30–33+6 weeks’ gestation and scheduled to undergo routine ultrasound examinations at Kagawa University Hospital, were asked to participate in a 15-min examination of fetal facial expressions. Only healthy non-smokers with singleton, dichorionic diamniotic (DD) or monochorionic diamniotic (MD) twin pregnancies were included in this study. Pregnancies with a high maternal or fetal risk (hypertensive disorders of pregnancy, gestational diabetes, thyroid disease, FGR, polyhydramnios, oligohydramnios, and chromosomal abnormalities) were excluded from the study. The gestational age was calculated from the first day of the last menstrual period, and confirmed by the first- or early second-trimester two-dimensional (2D) sonographic examinations.

A sonographic mid-trimester anomaly scan according to the guidelines of the International Society of Obstetrics and Gynecology was used to exclude the presence of fetal abnormalities, while growth was evaluated using Japanese growth charts [19]. All pregnancies were only examined once. If the fetal face was not clearly displayed because it was covered by body parts or due to inappropriate fetal position, the trial was repeated on another day. The growth of all fetuses was normal, and there were no abnormalities on conducting 4D ultrasound examination. There was no neonate with congenital anomalies or genetic disorders.

All examinations were performed using Voluson E8 (GE Healthcare Japan, Tokyo, Japan) with a curved array trans-abdominal transducer (1–4 MHz). Examinations and data analysis were performed by two experienced examiners (E.N. and M.A.M.A.). The study was conducted following approval by the Ethics Committee of the Kagawa University Graduate School of Medicine. All participants provided informed consent after a full explanation of the aim of the study.

Sixty fetuses of 48 pregnant women (30 with a singleton pregnancy and 18 with twin pregnancies) were eligible to be enrolled in the study. Twin pregnancies included 18 cases (14 DD pregnancies and four MD twin fetuses). Out of the 14 DD cases, 10 cases (20 fetuses) showed clear visualization of both fetuses, while the remaining four cases involved only one fetus that could be clearly visualized (four fetuses). MD cases involved two twin cases (four fetuses) that could be clearly visualized, while the two remaining MD cases involved one fetus only each that could be clearly observed. Therefore, twin fetuses included 24 DD and six MD fetuses. Table 1 shows the clinical characteristics of subjects included in the study.

Table 1:

Clinical characteristics of subjects.

SubjectsnMaternal age (y.o.) mean (SD)Para median (range)Examination week (weeks) mean (SD)Birth weight (g) mean (SD)Sex (male/female)Apgar scoreUApH mean (SD)NICU admission, %Neonatal abnormality, %
1 min median (range)5 min median (range)
Singleton pregnancy3031.6 (5.6)1 (0–4)31.8 (1.3)3133.5 (281.9)17/138 (7–9)9 (7–10)7.300 (0.072)3.30
Twin pregnancy1830.4 (5.1)0 (0–3)31.9 (1.3)2490.4 (344.7)9/218 (5–9)9 (7–10)7.322 (0.062)33.30
SignificanceNSNSNSP<0.0005P=0.039NSNSNSP=0.003NS
  1. NICU, neonatal intensive care unit; NS, not significant; SD, standard deviation; UApH, umbilical artery blood pH; y.o., years old.

Seven fetal facial expressions (mouthing, yawning, smiling, tongue expulsion, scowling, sucking, and blinking) were assessed using 4D ultrasound. The descriptions of these expressions were precisely explained in our previous study [15].

The frequencies of total movement are expressed as the median and range values. The unpaired t-test was used to assess differences in maternal age, gestational age at examination, birth weight, and umbilical artery pH (UApH). A comparison of the sex ratio, incidence of neonatal intensive care unit (NICU) admission, and incidence of neonatal abnormalities between the two groups was made using the chi-square (χ2) test. The Mann-Whitney U test was used to examine differences in parity, Apgar scores, and differences in the frequencies of facial expressions. Facial expression frequencies in singleton or twin fetuses were compared using the Kruskal-Wallis one-way analysis of variance by ranks test. Statistical software SPSS, version 23, for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) was used for statistical analysis, with significance set at a P-value <0.05.

Results

When clinical characteristics were compared between singleton and twin cases, there were no significant differences between singleton and twin pregnancies regarding maternal age, parity, examination week, Apgar score, UApH, or incidence of neonatal abnormalities. The birth weight was significantly higher in singleton cases than in twins (P<0.0005). There was a significant difference in the male/female ratio between singleton and twin pregnancies. NICU admission was significantly more frequent in twin cases than in singleton fetuses (P=0.003) (Table 1).

Mouthing and blinking were the most frequent facial expressions at 30–33+6 weeks of gestation in twin (Figure 1) and singleton fetuses (Figure 2). Both mouthing and blinking were significantly more frequent than the other facial expressions. When the frequency of each facial expression was compared between singleton and twin fetuses, we found that mouthing was more frequent in singleton (median, 4; range, 0–23) than in twin (median, 2; range, 0–8) fetuses (P=0.038) (Figure 3). Scowling was more frequent in singleton (median, 0; range, 0–3) than in twin (median, 0; range, 0–2) fetuses (P=0.01) (Figure 4). The frequencies of the other five facial expressions did not show any significant differences between the two groups (Table 2). There were also no significant differences between DD and MD twin fetuses.

Figure 1: Pattern of facial expressions in twin fetuses at 30–33+6 weeks.
Figure 1:

Pattern of facial expressions in twin fetuses at 30–33+6 weeks.

Figure 2: Pattern of facial expressions in singleton fetuses at 30–33+6 weeks.
Figure 2:

Pattern of facial expressions in singleton fetuses at 30–33+6 weeks.

Figure 3: Comparison of the frequencies of mouthing between twin and singleton fetuses.
Figure 3:

Comparison of the frequencies of mouthing between twin and singleton fetuses.

Figure 4: Comparison of the frequencies of scowling between twin and singleton fetuses.
Figure 4:

Comparison of the frequencies of scowling between twin and singleton fetuses.

Table 2:

Comparison of the frequencies of facial expressions between singleton and twin fetuses.

SubjectsnMouthing median (range)Yawning median (range)Smiling median (range)Tongue expulsion median (range)Scowling median (range)Sucking median (range)Blinking median (range)
Singleton304 (0–23)1 (0–5)1 (0–6)0 (0–4)0 (0–3)0 (0–2)4.5 (0–17)
Twin302.5 (0–8)1 (0–3)1 (0–5)0 (0–3)0 (0–2)0 (0–1)2.5 (0–17)
SignificanceP=0.038NSNSNSP=0.01NSNS
  1. NS, not significant.

Discussion

In the current study, facial expression frequencies showed that mouthing and blinking are the most frequent facial expressions in both singleton as well as twin fetal groups. These findings are consistent with those reported in our previous study [15]. When facial expressions were compared between singleton and twin fetuses, mouthing was less frequent in twins than in singletons. The reason for this difference is unknown. One possible cause might be accelerated brain maturation found in twin fetuses. It is known from previous reports that mouthing movements decrease with the advancement of gestation as a sign of brain maturation and development [10]. Reissland et al. [20] found that fetuses of smoker mothers displayed less mouthing movements than those of non-smokers probably due to the effect of stress. Smoking represented the stress factor that contributed to accelerated brain maturation in that study [20]. In the case of twin fetuses, the limitation of the available space in-utero represents a stressful situation, which might be responsible for the accelerated brain maturation as indicated by the less frequent mouthing movement compared with those in singleton fetuses at the same gestational age group. Amiel-Tison et al. [21] also reported that neurologic and physical maturity in twins after birth may be accelerated compared with those of singletons.

Regarding scowling, our results demonstrated its lower frequency in twin fetuses than in singletons. Scowling represents a state of fetal expression of pain or discomfort in-utero [22]. Twin pregnancy is a special experience where both fetuses experience early twin-to-twin contact [23]. Moreover, researchers found that fetuses developed habituation to stimuli as functional development of the brain reach advanced stages [24]. Therefore, the adaptations to touch stimulation by the co-twin and stressful situation of the crowded uterus might explain the lower frequency of scowling movements in twins in the third trimester compared with singleton fetuses. Such adaptation might reflect accelerated brain development in twin fetuses.

Although mouthing and scowling in twin fetuses are significantly different from those in singleton fetuses, the other facial expressions did not show any difference. This may be due to partial acceleration of the fetal brain development affecting some parts of the brain in twin fetuses while the others are not affected, resulting in selective acceleration of some brain areas as reflected by the changes in frequencies of two out of the seven studied facial expressions. This might be a result of maternal immune activation which happens when environmental factors in-utero are not favorable due to crowding of the uterus by the co-twin [25].

A limitation of this study is the small number of subjects, weakening the ability to draw a strong conclusion from the obtained results. This was due to the inability to obtain clear views of fetal faces in the case of a crowded uterus with twins of a relatively large size at such a late gestational age. A second limitation is that although our team has shown good inter- and intra-observer reproducibility in fetal behavior studies [26], fetal facial expressions still lack a universally accepted coding system. Therefore, studies with a larger sample size or multicentric studies using a coding system are needed to determine the real possible impacts of restricted spaces in-utero on functional development of the fetal brain and CNS in twin compared with singleton fetuses. The third limitation is that there is a significant difference in fetal sex between the two groups. Regarding the sex difference in fetal behavior and facial expressions between male and female fetuses, our previous study using 4D ultrasound showed that there is no sex difference in fetal behavior and facial expressions in the third trimester of pregnancy [27]. Therefore, a 4D ultrasound study examining fetal facial expressions may not need to consider the factor of fetal sex.


Corresponding author: Toshiyuki Hata, MD, PhD, Professor Emeritus, Department of Perinatology and Gynecology, Kagawa University Graduate School of Medicine, 1750-1 Ikenobe, Miki, Kagawa 761-0793, Japan; and Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Miyake Clinic, 369-8 Ohfuku, Minami-ku, Okayama 701-0204, Japan, Tel.: +81-(0)87-891-2174, Fax: +81-(0)87-891-2175

  1. Author contributions: All the authors have accepted responsibility for the entire content of this submitted manuscript and approved submission.

  2. Research funding: None declared.

  3. Employment or leadership: None declared.

  4. Honorarium: None declared.

  5. Competing interests: The funding organization(s) played no role in the study design; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the report for publication.

  6. Conflict of interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

1. Degani S, Leibovitz Z, Shapiro I, Ohel G. Twins’ temperament: early prenatal sonographic assessment and postnatal correlation. J Perinatol 2009;29:337–42.10.1038/jp.2008.238Search in Google Scholar

2. AboEllail MAM, Kanenishi K, Mori N, Noguchi J, Marumo G, Hata T. Ultrasound study of fetal movements in singleton and twin pregnancies at 12–19 weeks. J Perinat Med 2018;46:832–8.10.1515/jpm-2017-0158Search in Google Scholar

3. Mori N, Kanenishi K, AboEllail MAM, Nitta E, Noguchi J, Marumo G, et al. Singleton and twin fetal movements before 20 weeks of gestation. Donald School J Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol 2018;12:99–103.10.5005/jp-journals-10009-1558Search in Google Scholar

4. Mulder EJH, Derks JB, de Laat MWM, Visser GHA. Fetal behavior in normal dichorionic twin pregnancy. Early Hum Dev 2012;88:129–34.10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.07.011Search in Google Scholar

5. Prechtl HF. State of the art of a new functional assessment of young nervous system: an early predictor of cerebral palsy. Early Hum Dev 1997;50:1–11.10.1016/S0378-3782(97)00088-1Search in Google Scholar

6. Nijhuis JG. Fetal behavior. Neurobiol Aging 2003;24:S41–6.10.1016/S0197-4580(03)00054-XSearch in Google Scholar

7. Hepper PG. Fetal behavior: why so skeptical? Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol 1996;8:145–8.10.1046/j.1469-0705.1996.08030145.xSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

8. AboEllail MAM, Hata T. Fetal face and four-dimensional ultrasound. In: Kurjak A, Chervenak FA, editors. Donald School Textbook of Ultrasound in Obstetrics & Gynecology 4th ed. New Delhi: Jaypee Brothers Medical Publishers(P) Ltd., 2017:791–9.10.5005/jp/books/13058_48Search in Google Scholar

9. AboEllail MAM, Hata T. Fetal face as important indicator of fetal brain function. J Perinat Med 2017;45:729–36.10.1515/jpm-2016-0377Search in Google Scholar PubMed

10. Kurjak A, Stanojević M, Andonotopo W, Scazzocchio-Duenas E, Azumendi G, Carrera JM. Fetal behavior assessed in all three trimesters of normal pregnancy by four-dimensional ultrasonography. Croat Med J 2005;46:772–80.Search in Google Scholar

11. Yigiter AB, Kavak ZN. Normal standards of fetal behavior assessed by four-dimensional sonography. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med 2006;19:707–21.10.1080/14767050600924129Search in Google Scholar PubMed

12. Yan F, Dai SY, Akther N, Kuno A, Yanagihara T, Hata T. Four-dimensional sonographic assessment of fetal facial expression early in the third trimester. Int J Gynecol Obstet 2006;94:108–13.10.1016/j.ijgo.2006.05.004Search in Google Scholar PubMed

13. Kanenishi K, Hanaoka U, Noguchi J, Marumo G, Hata T. 4D ultrasound evaluation of fetal facial expressions during the latter stages of the second trimester. Int J Gynecol Obstet 2013;121:257–60.10.1016/j.ijgo.2013.01.018Search in Google Scholar PubMed

14. Sato M, Kanenishi K, Hanaoka U, Noguchi J, Marumo G, Hata T. 4D ultrasound study of fetal facial expressions at 20–24 weeks of gestation. Int J Gynecol Obstet 2014;126:275–9.10.1016/j.ijgo.2014.03.036Search in Google Scholar PubMed

15. AboEllail MAM, Kanenishi K, Mori N, Mohamed OAK, Hata T. 4D ultrasound study of fetal facial expressions in third trimester of pregnancy. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med 2018;31:1856–64.10.1080/14767058.2017.1330880Search in Google Scholar PubMed

16. Mori N, AboEllail MAM, Tenkumo C, Kanenishi K, Nishimoto N, Hata T. Fetal facial expressions in small-for-gestational-age and growth-restricted fetuses. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med 2019;32:1426–32.10.1080/14767058.2017.1410788Search in Google Scholar PubMed

17. AboEllail MAM, Kanenishi K, Mori N, Hata T. Coordination of fetal facial expressions after 36 weeks of gestation. Donald School J Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol 2018;12:156–61.10.5005/jp-journals-10009-1567Search in Google Scholar

18. Mori N, Kanenishi K, AboEllail MAM, Nitta E, Hata T. Neurological development may be accelerated in growth-restricted fetuses: a 4D ultrasound study. J Perinat Med 2019;47:429–33.10.1515/jpm-2018-0379Search in Google Scholar PubMed

19. Kuno A, Akiyama M, Yanagihara T, Hata T. Comparison of fetal growth in singleton, twin, and triplet pregnancies. Hum Reprod 1999;14:1352–60.10.1093/humrep/14.5.1352Search in Google Scholar PubMed

20. Reissland N, Francis B, Kumarendran K, Mason J. Ultrasound observations of subtle movements: a pilot study comparing foetuses of smoking and nonsmoking mothers. Acta Paediatrica 2015;104:596–603.10.1111/apa.13001Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

21. Amiel-Tison C, Maillard F, Lebrum F, Faucher P, Vitry F, Hottinger O, et al. Acceleration of neurologic and physical maturity in multiple pregnancies. Presented at European Congress of Perinatal Medicine, Helsinki, June, 1994.Search in Google Scholar

22. Bernardes LS, Ottolia JF, Cecchini M, Filho AGA, Teixeira MJ, Francisco RPV, et al. On the feasibility of accessing acute pain-related facial expressions in the human fetus and its potential implications: a case report. Pain Rep 2018;3:e673.10.1097/PR9.0000000000000673Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

23. Hata T, Sasaki M, Yanagihara T. Difference in the frequency of types of inter-twin contact at 10–13 weeks’ gestation: preliminary four-dimensional sonographic study. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med 2012;25:226–30.10.3109/14767058.2011.568551Search in Google Scholar PubMed

24. Morokuma S, Fukushima K, Kawai N, Tomonaga M, Satoh S, Nakano H. Fetal habituation correlates with functional brain development. Behav Brain Res 2004;153:459–63.10.1016/j.bbr.2004.01.002Search in Google Scholar PubMed

25. Estes ML, McAllister AK. Maternal immune activation: implications for neuropsychiatric disorders. Science 2016;19:772–7.10.1126/science.aag3194Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

26. Sajapala S, AboEllail MAM, Kanenishi K, Mori N, Marumo G, Hata T. 4D ultrasound study of fetal movement early in the second trimester of pregnancy. J Perinat Med 2017;45:737–43.10.1515/jpm-2016-0250Search in Google Scholar PubMed

27. Hata T, Hanaoka U, AboEllail MAM, Uematsu R, Noguchi J, Kusaka T, et al. Is there a sex difference in fetal behavior? A comparison of KANET test between male and female fetuses. J Perinat Med 2016;44:585–8.10.1515/jpm-2015-0387Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Received: 2019-04-10
Accepted: 2019-10-02
Published Online: 2019-10-17
Published in Print: 2019-11-26

©2019 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 26.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/jpm-2019-0127/html
Scroll to top button