Home Acquisition of doubly articulated stops among Ga-speaking children
Article
Licensed
Unlicensed Requires Authentication

Acquisition of doubly articulated stops among Ga-speaking children

  • Felix Kpogo EMAIL logo , Virginia C. Mueller Gathercole and Jonathan Nsiah Tetteh
Published/Copyright: October 14, 2021

Abstract

This study investigates the acquisition of labio-velar stops by Ga-speaking children in Ghana. Such stops were elicited in initial, intervocalic, and pre-lateral positions through a picture naming task. Sixty Ga-dominant and Ga-English children at 5-, 61/2-, and 8-years of age were tested. All age groups showed some difficulty with the doubly articulated stops, but this was relative to voicing, phonological environment, age, and input. Performance on the voiceless labio-velar stop was better than on the voiced labio-velar stop, and better in intervocalic position than in word-initial and pre-lateral positions. Older children performed better than younger children and Ga-dominant children better than Ga-English children. Performance was better when children did not receive a prompt than when they did. Analysis of modifications reveals frequent processes of simplification to labial singleton stops, some voicing changes, and the occurrence of processes of vowel insertion between the stops and /l/. These findings are discussed in terms of universals of speech sound acquisition, the role of input, and the influence of language-specific factors on children’s performance.

Abstract in Ga

Nɛkɛ shigbɛlɛmɔ nɛɛ kwɛ bɔni Ga gbekɛbii ni eye kɛjɛ afii enumɔ kɛyashi afii kpaanyɔ ni wieɔ Ga pɛ yɛ shia kɛ amɛteŋ mɛi ni wieɔ Blɔfo kɛ Ga fɛɛ yɛ shia kaseɔ kɔnsonanti gbɛɛmɔi ni ji: /kp/ kɛ /gb/ ni jeɔ kpo yɛ gbɛhei srɔtoi etɛ (ni ji wiemɔkuli shishijee, vaolii ateŋ kɛ /l/ gbɛɛmɔ hiɛ). Ga gbekɛbii ni wieɔ Ga pɛ lɛ jɛ shiai ni awieɔ Ga titri yɛ jɛmɛ ni gbekɛbii ni wieɔ Ga kɛ Blɔfo lɛ jɛ shiai ni awieɔ Blɔfo kɛ Ga fɛɛ. Gbekɛbii ni ahala kɛfee shigbɛlɛmɔ nɛɛ ayifalɛ ji nyɔŋmai ekpaa (60); abi ni amɛtsɛ nibii kɛ nifeemɔi komɛi ni atɛŋ amɛhe mfoniri lɛ agbɛi. Ayɔse


Corresponding author: Felix Kpogo, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA, E-mail:

Acknowledgments

This work is a revised version of an MA thesis conducted at Florida International University (FIU) by the first author, under the supervision of the second author. We wish to express our gratitude to the other members of the MA committee, Mehmet Yavaʂ and Tometro Hopkins. We also thank the African & Africa Diaspora Studies Program at FIU, authorities of St. Mary’s Anglican and Osu Presbyterian Preparatory Schools, and the teachers, parents, and children who supported and participated in this research.

Appendix A: Number of children reaching 90 and 75% criteria with prompt

Voicing Phonological environment Ga-dominant (GD) Ga-English (GE)
5 61/2 8 5 61/2 8
90% criterion Voiceless Word-initial 4 7 7 1 4 2
Intervocalic 8 10 9 10 10 10
Pre-lateral 1 4 2 1 0 2
Voiced Word-initial 1 1 3 0 0 1
Intervocalic 7 9 9 9 9 9
Pre-lateral 0 1 1 0 0 0
75% criterion Voiceless Word-initial 7 8 7 2 4 3
Intervocalic 9 10 9 10 10 10
Pre-lateral 5 6 8 3 5 3
Voiced Word-initial 2 3 4 0 0 1
Intervocalic 9 9 9 9 10 10
Pre-lateral 0 4 2 0 0 1

Appendix B: Modifications in intervocalic environments

Stop modification Ga-dominant (GD) Ga-English (GE) Total GD Total GE Totals
Age 5 6 1/2 8 5 6 1/2 8
Number of errors:
5 4 3 4 4 1 7 2 9
Simplification kp > p Errors 3 3 1 (33.3)

1
1 1 7 2 9
Percent (60.0) (75.0) (25.0) (25.0) (58.3) (22.2) (42.9)
No. children 3 2 1 1 6 2 8
gb > b Errors 1 2 1 1 3 4
Percent (20.0) (50.0) (100.0) (8.3) (33.3) (19.0)
No. children 1 2 1 1 3 4
Voicing change kp > gb Errors 1 (25.0)

1
1 (11.1)

1
1 (4.8)

1
Percent
No. children
gb > kp Errors 1 (20.0)

1
1 (25.0)

1
2 (66.7)

1
3 4 (33.3)

3
3 7
Percent (75.0) (33.3) (33.3)
No. children 1 1 4
TOTAL 5 4 3 4 3 1 12 9 21

References

Amarteifio, Victor. 1998. Bediako [The adventurer], 2nd edn. Accra: Amaabooks Limited. (Translated into Ga by Charlotte Ogbedee Laryea).Search in Google Scholar

Ardila, Alfredo, Monica Rosselli, Esmeralda Matute & Soledad Guajardo. 2005. The influence of the parents’ educational level on the development of executive functions. Developmental Neuropsychology 28(1). 539–560. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2801_5.Search in Google Scholar

Beckman, Mary Esther, Yoneyama Kiyoko & Jan Edwards. 2003. Language-specific and language-universal aspects of lingual obstruent production in Japanese-acquiring children. Journal of the Phonetic Society of Japan 7. 18–28.Search in Google Scholar

Bernhardt, Barbara May & Joseph P. Stemberger. 2018. Tap and trill clusters in typical and protracted phonological development: Conclusion. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 32(5–6). 563–575. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2017.1370496.Search in Google Scholar

Bialystok, Ellen. 2001. Bilingual development: Language, literacy and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511605963Search in Google Scholar

Bibiebome, Eric Ziem. 2011. Language shift and maintenance among the Ga. Legon, Accra: University of Ghana MPhil thesis.Search in Google Scholar

Brown, Roger. 1973. A first language: The early stages. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.10.4159/harvard.9780674732469Search in Google Scholar

Bureau of Ghana Languages. 1986. Language guide: Ga version. Accra: BGL Publication.Search in Google Scholar

Calvo, Alejandro & Ellen Bialystok. 2014. Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition 130(3). 278–288. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.015.Search in Google Scholar

Cheung, Pamela S. P. 1990. The acquisition of Cantonese phonology in Hong Kong: A cross-sectional study. Unpublished undergraduate thesis. London: University College London.Search in Google Scholar

Chin, Steven B. & Daniel A. Dinnsen. 1992. Consonant clusters in disordered speech: Constraints and correspondence patterns. Journal of Child Language 19. 259–285. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900011417.Search in Google Scholar

Chirlian, Nicola S. & Christopher F. Sharpley. 1982. Children’s articulation development: Some regional differences. Australian Journal of Human Communication Disorders 10. 23–30. https://doi.org/10.3109/asl2.1982.10.issue-2.03.Search in Google Scholar

Connell, Bruce. 1994. The structure of labial-velar stops. Journal of Phonetics 22(4). 441–476. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0095-4470(19)30295-5.Search in Google Scholar

Dakubu, Mary Esther. 1971. A problem in the reconstruction of the Ga-Adangme consonant system. Journal of African Languages 10(3). 73–78.Search in Google Scholar

Dakubu, Mary Esther. 2002. Ga phonology. Monograph 6. Legon: Institute of African Studies, University of Ghana.Search in Google Scholar

da Silva, Karl Monica, Carla Ferrante, John Van Borsel & Monica Medeiros de Britto Pereira. 2012. Phonological acquisition of Brazilian Portuguese in children from Rio de Janeiro. Jornal Da Sociedade Brasileira De Fonoaudiologia 24(3). 248–254. https://doi.org/10.1590/s2179-64912012000300010.Search in Google Scholar

Danis, Nick. 2017. Markedness and complex stops: Evidence from simplification processes. Proceedings of World Congress of African Linguistics 8. 1–16.Search in Google Scholar

Davidson, Lisa. 2016. Variability in the implementation of voicing in American English obstruents. Journal of Phonetics 54. 35–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2015.09.003.Search in Google Scholar

Davis, Barbara & Lisa Bedore. 2013. An emergence approach to speech acquisition: Doing and knowing. New York, NY: Psychology Press.10.4324/9780203375303Search in Google Scholar

Demuth, Katherine. 1989. Problems in the acquisition of grammatical tone. Papers & Reports on Child Language Development 28. 81–88.Search in Google Scholar

Demuth, Katherine. 1992. Acquisition of Sesotho. In Slobin Dan (ed.), The cross-linguistic study of language acquisition, 557–638. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.10.4324/9781315808208-11Search in Google Scholar

Denny, Margaret & Richard S. McGowan. 2012. Implications of peripheral muscular and anatomical development for the acquisition of lingual control for speech production: A review. Folia Phoniatrica et Logopaedica 64. 105–115. https://doi.org/10.1159/000338611.Search in Google Scholar

Dinnsen, Daniel A. 1992. Variation in developing and fully developed phonetic inventories. In Charles A. Ferguson, Lise Menn & Carol Stoel-Gammon (eds.), Phonological development: Theories, research and implications, 191–210. Timonium, MD: York Press.Search in Google Scholar

Dodd, Barbara, Alison Holm, Hua Zhu & Sharon Crosbie. 2003. Phonological development: A normative study of British English-speaking children. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 17(8). 617–643. https://doi.org/10.1080/0269920031000111348.Search in Google Scholar

Eberhard, David, Gary Simons & Fennig Charles (eds.). 2019 Ethnologue: Languages of Ghana, 22nd edn. Dallas: SIL International.Search in Google Scholar

Freitas, Maria João. 2004. The vowel [ɨ] in the acquisition of European Portuguese. In Jacqueline van Kampen & Sergio Baauw (eds.), Proceedings of GALA 2003, 163–174. Utrecht: LOT.Search in Google Scholar

Gangji, Nazneen, Michelle Pascoe & Mantoa Smouse. 2014. Swahili speech development: Preliminary normative data from typically developing pre-school children in Tanzania. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 50(2). 151–164. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12118.Search in Google Scholar

Gathercole, Virginia C. Mueller. 2007. Miami and North Wales, so far and yet so near: Constructivist account of morpho-syntactic development in bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism 10(3). 224–247. https://doi.org/10.2167/beb442.0.Search in Google Scholar

Gathercole, Virginia C. Mueller. 2016. Factors moderating proficiency in bilinguals. In Elena Nicoladis & Simona Montanari (eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan: Factors moderating language proficiency, 123–140. Washington, DC: Mouton De Gruyter, American Psychological Association.10.1515/9783110341249-009Search in Google Scholar

Gathercole, Virginia C. Mueller. 2018. Language development in bilingual children: Fact, factoid, and fiction. In Maarja Siiner, Francis M. Hult & Tanja Kupisch (eds.), Language policy and language acquisition planning, 237–255. Cham, Switzerland: Springer.10.1007/978-3-319-75963-0_14Search in Google Scholar

Gathercole, Virginia C. Mueller & Hoff Erika. 2007. Input and the acquisition of language: Three questions. In Erika Hoff & Marilyn Shatz (eds.), The handbook of language development, 107–127. N.Y.: Blackwell Publishers.10.1002/9780470757833.ch6Search in Google Scholar

Gathercole, Virginia C. Mueller, Enlli Mȏn Thomas, Emily J. Roberts, Catrin O. Hughes & Emma K. Hughes. 2013. Why assessment needs to take exposure into account: Vocabulary and grammatical abilities in bilingual children. In Virginia C. Mueller Gathercole (ed.), Issues in the assessment of bilinguals, 20–55. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.10.21832/9781783090105-004Search in Google Scholar

Gathercole, Virginia C. Mueller, Rocío Pérez-Tattam, Hans Stadthagen-González & Enlli M. Thomas. 2014. Bilingual construction of two systems: To interact or not to interact? In Enlli Mȏn Thomas & Ineke Mennen (eds.), Advances in the study of bilingualism, 63–89. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.10.21832/9781783091713-007Search in Google Scholar

Ginsborg, Jane. 2006. The effects of socio-economic status on children’s language acquisition and use. In Judy Clegg & Jane Ginsborg (eds.), Language and social disadvantage: Theory into practice, 9–27. Chichester, West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons.Search in Google Scholar

Goldman, Ronald & Macalyne Fristoe. 2000. Goldman-Fristoe test of articulation, 2nd edn. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.10.1037/t15098-000Search in Google Scholar

Hart, Betty & Todd R. Risley. 1995. Meaningful differences in the everyday experiences of young American children. Baltimore: Paul Brookes.Search in Google Scholar

Hoff, Erika. 2003. The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development 74(5). 1368–1378. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00612.Search in Google Scholar

Hoff, Erika, Cynthia Core, Silvia Place, Rosario Rumiche, Melissa Señor & Marisol Parra. 2012. Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language 39. 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000910000759.Search in Google Scholar

Ingram, David. 1976. Phonological disability in children: Studies in language disability and remediation 2. London: Edward Arnold.Search in Google Scholar

Jakobson, Roman. 1941/1968. Child language, aphasia and phonological universals. Tr. by Allan R. Keiler. Mouton: The Hague.10.1515/9783111353562Search in Google Scholar

Jonas, Akpanglo-Nartey N. & Rebecca A. Akpanglo-Nartey. 2012. Some endangered languages of Ghana. American Journal of Linguistics 1. 10–18.Search in Google Scholar

Jun, Sun-Ah. 2006. Phonological development of Korean: A case study. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 105. 51–65.Search in Google Scholar

Kehoe, Margaret M. & Conxita Lleo. 2002. Intervocalic consonants in the acquisition of German: Onsets, codas or something else? Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 16(3). 169–182. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200110112213.Search in Google Scholar

Kim, Minjung, Soo-Jin Kim & Carol Stoel-Gammon. 2017. Phonological acquisition of Korean consonants in conversational speech produced by young Korean children. Journal of Child Language 44. 1010–1023. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000916000258.Search in Google Scholar

Kpogo, Felix. in preparation. Evidence from complex stop reduction: Covert contrast and/or neutralization? PhD Qualifying Paper 1. Boston, MA: Boston University.Search in Google Scholar

Kupisch, Tanja. 2003. On the relation between input frequency and acquisition patterns from a cross-linguistic perspective. In Jacqueline van Kampen & Sergio Baauw (eds.), Proceedings of GALA 2003, 281–292. Utrecht: LOT Occasional Series.Search in Google Scholar

Law, Naska C. W. & Lydia K. H. So. 2006. The relationship of phonological development and language dominance in bilingual Cantonese and Putonghua children. International Journal of Bilingualism 10. 405–428. https://doi.org/10.1177/13670069060100040201.Search in Google Scholar

Levelt, Clara C., Niels O. Schiller & Willem J. Levelt. 2000. The acquisition of syllable types. Language Acquisition 8(3). 237–264. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0803_2.Search in Google Scholar

Lin, Susan & Katherine Demuth. 2015. Children’s acquisition of English onset and coda /l/: Articulatory evidence. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 58. 13–27. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_jslhr-s-14-0041.Search in Google Scholar

Macken, Marlys A. 1980. Aspects of the acquisition of stop systems: A cross-linguistic perspective. In Grace H. Yeni-Komshian, James F. Kavanagh & Charles A. Ferguson (eds.), Child phonology, vol. 1: Production, 143–168. New York: Academic Press.10.1016/B978-0-12-770601-6.50013-9Search in Google Scholar

MacLeod, Andrea A. N., Ann T. Sutton, Natacha Trudeau & Elin T. Thordardottir. 2011. The acquisition of consonants in Quebecois French: A cross-sectional study of pre-school aged children. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 13(2). 93–109. https://doi.org/10.3109/17549507.2011.487543.Search in Google Scholar

Maddieson, Ian. 1984. Patterns of sounds. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511753459Search in Google Scholar

Maddieson, Ian & Ladefoged Peter. 1989. Multiply articulated segments and the feature hierarchy. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 72. 116–138.Search in Google Scholar

Mahura, Olebeng Olive & Michelle Pascoe. 2016. The acquisition of Setswana segmental phonology in children aged 3.0–6.0 years: A crosssectional study. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 18(6). 533–549. https://doi.org/10.3109/17549507.2015.1126639.Search in Google Scholar

Maphalala, Zinhle, Michelle Pascoe & Mantoa Smouse. 2014. Phonological development of first language isiXhosa-speaking children aged 3;0–6;0 years: A descriptive cross-sectional study. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 28(3). 176–194. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2013.840860.Search in Google Scholar

McLeod, Sharynne & Kathryn Crowe. 2018. Children’s consonant acquisition in 27 languages: A cross-linguistic review. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 27. 1546–1571. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_ajslp-17-0100.Search in Google Scholar

Mfum-Mensah, Obed. 2005. The impact of colonial and postcolonial Ghanaian language policies on vernacular use in schools in two Northern Ghanaian communities. Comparative Education 41. 71–85.10.1080/03050060500073256Search in Google Scholar

Mowrer, Donald E. & Sharon Burger. 1991. A comparative analysis of phonological acquisition of consonants in the speech of 21/2–6-year-old Xhosa- and English-speaking children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 5(2). 139–164. https://doi.org/10.3109/02699209108985510.Search in Google Scholar

Munson, Benjamin, Jan Edwards & Mary Esther Beckman. 2012. Phonological representations in language acquisition: Climbing the ladder of abstraction. In Abigail C. Cohn, Cècile Fougeron & Marie K. Huffman (eds.), The Oxford handbook of laboratory phonology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199575039.013.0012Search in Google Scholar

Nissen, Shawn L. & Robert Allen Fox. 2009. Acoustic and spectral patterns in young children’s stop consonant productions. Journal of Acoustic Society of America 126(3). 1369–1378. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3192350.Search in Google Scholar

Obeng, Samuel Gyasi. 1997. An analysis of the linguistic situation in Ghana. African Languages and Cultures 10. 63–81. https://doi.org/10.1080/09544169708717813.Search in Google Scholar

Ohala, John. 1997. The relation between phonetics and phonology. In William J. Hardcastle & John Laver (eds.), Handbook of phonetic sciences, 674–694. Oxford: Blackwell.10.1002/9781444317251.ch17Search in Google Scholar

Oller, D. Kimbrough & Rebecca E. Eilers (eds.). 2002. Language and literacy in bilingual children. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.10.21832/9781853595721Search in Google Scholar

Owens, Robert E. 1996. Language development: An introduction, 4th edn. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Search in Google Scholar

Paradis, Johanne & Fred Genesee. 1996. Syntactic acquisition: Autonomous or interdependent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition 18. 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0272263100014662.Search in Google Scholar

Pearson, Barbara Zurer & Sylvia C. Fernández. 1994. Patterns of interaction in the lexical growth in two languages of bilingual infants and toddlers. Language Learning 44. 617–653. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-1770.1994.tb00633.x.Search in Google Scholar

Pearson, Barbara Zurer, Sylvia C. Fernández & D. Kimbrough Oller. 1993. Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning 43. 93–120. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-1770.1993.tb00174.x.Search in Google Scholar

Pearson, Barbara Zurer, Sylvia C. Fernández & D. Kimbrough Oller. 1995. Cross-language synonyms in the lexicons of bilingual infants: One language or two? Journal of Child Language 22. 345–368. https://doi.org/10.1017/s030500090000982x.Search in Google Scholar

Peña, Elizabeth D., Lisa M. Bedore & Christine Fiestas. 2013. Development of bilingual semantic norms: Can two be one? In Virginia C. Mueller Gathercole (ed.), Solutions for the assessment of bilinguals, 103–124. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.10.21832/9781783090150-008Search in Google Scholar

Perez, Denisse, Pilar Vivar, Barbara M. Bernhardt, Elvira Mendoza, Carmen Ávila, Gloria Carballo, Dolores Fresneda, Juana Muñoz & Patricio Vergara. 2018. Word-initial rhotic clusters in Spanish-speaking preschoolers in Chile and Granada, Spain. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 32(5–6). 481–505. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2017.1359852.Search in Google Scholar

Place, Silvia & Erika Hoff. 2011. Properties of dual language exposure that influence 2-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development 82. 1834–1849. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01660.x.Search in Google Scholar

Poole, Irene. 1934. Genetic development of articulation of consonants sounds in speech. Elementary English Review 11. 159–161.Search in Google Scholar

Porter, Halvorsen Judith & Barbara Williams Hodson. 2001. Collaborating to obtain phonological acquisition data for local schools. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools 32. 165–171. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2001/015).Search in Google Scholar

Raviv, Tali, Maureen Kessenich & Frederick J. Morrison. 2004. A meditational model of the association between socioeconomic status and three-year-old language abilities: The role of parenting factors. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 19(4). 528–547. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.10.007.Search in Google Scholar

Rvachew, Susan & Ellen Andrews. 2002. The influence of syllable position on children’s production of consonants. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 16(3). 183–198. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200110112222.Search in Google Scholar

Saah, Kofi K. 1986. Language use and attitudes in Ghana. Anthropological Linguistics: Exploring the Languages of the World 28. 367–377.Search in Google Scholar

Sanders, Eric K. 1972. When are speech sounds learned? Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 37(1). 55–63.10.1044/jshd.3701.55Search in Google Scholar

Sarsour, Khaled, Margaret Sheridan, Jutte Douglas, Amani Nuru-Jeter, Hinshaw Stephen & W. Thomas Boyce. 2011. Family socioeconomic status and child executive functions: The roles of language, home environment, and single parenthood. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 17(l). 120–132. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617710001335.Search in Google Scholar

Scobie, James M., Fiona Gibbon, William J. Hardcastle & Paul Fletcher. 2000. Covert contrast as a stage in the acquisition of phonetics and phonology. In Michael Broe & Janet Pierrehumbert (eds.), Papers in laboratory phonology V, 194–207. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Sellers, Melinda. 2007. Attitudes, ambitions, and language of education: Debates on language of instruction in Ghanaian education system. http://homes.chass.utoronto.ca/∼cpercy/courses/eng6365-sellers.htm (retrieved 12 February 2018).Search in Google Scholar

Shariatmadari, David. 2006. Sounds difficult? Why phonological theory needs ease of articulation. SOAS Working Papers in Linguistics 14. 207–226.Search in Google Scholar

Shetty, Parinitha P. & Prabhu Shwetha. 2015. Acquisition of speech sounds in Tulu language. Journal of Child Language Acquisition and Development 3(2). 98–109.Search in Google Scholar

Smit, Ann Bosma. 1993. Phonologic error distributions in the Iowa-Nebraska articulation norms project: Consonant singletons. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36. 931–947. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3605.931.Search in Google Scholar

Smit, Ann Bosma, Linda Hand, Joseph J Freilinger, John E. Bernthal & Ann Bird. 1990. The Iowa articulation norms project and its Nebraska replication. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 55. 779–798. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.5504.779.Search in Google Scholar

So, Lydia K. H. & Barbara J. Dodd. 1995. The acquisition of phonology by Cantonese-speaking children. Journal of Child Language 22. 473–495. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900009922.Search in Google Scholar

Stemberger, Joseph Paul & Barbara M. Bernhardt. 2002. Editorial: Forum on intervocalic consonants in phonological development. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 16(3). 149–154. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200110112196.Search in Google Scholar

Stoel-Gammon, Carol. 1991. Normal and disordered phonology in two-year-olds. Topics in Language Disorders 11. 21–32. https://doi.org/10.1097/00011363-199111040-00005.Search in Google Scholar

Studdert-Kennedy, Michael & Louis Goldstein. 2003. Launching language: The gestural origin of discrete infinity. In Morten H. Christiansen & Simon Kirby (eds.), Language evolution, 235–254. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199244843.003.0013Search in Google Scholar

Tar, Éva. 2018. Word-initial tap-trill clusters: Hungarian. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 32(5–6). 544–562. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2017.1363292.Search in Google Scholar

Templin, Mildred. 1957. Certain language skills in children: Their development and interrelationships. Institute of Child Welfare Monograph 26. Minneapolis: The University of Minnesota Press.10.5749/j.ctttv2stSearch in Google Scholar

To, Carol K. S., Pamela S. Cheung & Sharynne McLeod. 2013. A population study of children’s acquisition of Hong Kong Cantonese consonants, vowels, and tones. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 56(1). 103–122. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0080).Search in Google Scholar

Tuomi, Seppo K., Sandile Gxhilishe & Lusanda Matomela. 2001. The acquisition of Xhosa phonemes. Per Linguam 17(1). 14–23.10.5785/17-1-132Search in Google Scholar

Unsworth, Sharon. 2016. Quantity and quality of language input in bilingual language development. In Elena Nicoladis & Simona Montanari (eds.), Bilingualism across the lifespan: Factors moderating language proficiency, 103–122. Washington, DC: Mouton De Gruyter, American Psychological Association.10.1515/9783110341249-008Search in Google Scholar

Vihman, Marilyn. 1996. Phonological development. Oxford: Blackwell.Search in Google Scholar

Wellman, Beth L., Ida Mae Case, Ida Gaarder Mengert & Dorothy E. Bradbury. 1931. Speech sounds of young children: University of Iowa Studies in Child Welfare, vol. 5, 2nd edn. Iowa City: University of Iowa Press.Search in Google Scholar

Yavaʂ, Mehmet. 1998. Bilingual phonology. In Mehmet Yavaʂ (ed.), Phonological development and disorders, 193–231. London: Singular Publishing Group.Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2021-10-14
Published in Print: 2021-05-26

© 2021 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 28.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/jall-2021-2018/html?lang=en
Scroll to top button