Abstract
The oldest medical school of modern civilization, in Salerno, Italy, prioritized the study of philosophy, logic, and reasoning. We first retrace the history of how clinical reasoning and its perceived importance has evolved, culminating ultimately in the 2015 National Academies report on diagnostic error in healthcare. The report clearly emphasized the fundamental role of clinical reasoning in diagnosis, and the critical need to optimize the cognitive elements of diagnosis to prevent diagnostic errors in the future. The dual processing paradigm, envisioning both intuitive and rational pathways, is central to current understandings of clinical reasoning. The importance of knowledge, the impact of cognitive biases, the influence of context, and many other ‘adjacent’ factors also impact the likelihood of arriving at the correct diagnosis. Medical education needs to re-prioritize cognition over content, and teach clinical reasoning interprofessionally. Emphasizing rationality and recognizing cognitive and affective bias are key. A host of interventions have been proposed: patient engagement, second opinions, reflection, improving teamwork, and using AI are all well justified and worthy of trials.
The medical school at Salerno
Much of modern medicine is focused on the attainment of declarative knowledge, or knowing what (typically the essential facts of anatomy, physiology, biochemistry and pathophysiology) while placing less emphasis on procedural knowledge or knowing how (how to think, reason, and make decisions about clinical data). In contrast, at the Schola Medica Salernitana, the medical school at Salerno in Southern Italy, founded in the 9th century and considered the oldest medical school of modern civilization, prospective medical students initially undertook three years of training in philosophy, logic, and dialectical reasoning (scientia logicalis), followed by five years of medical studies, in order to receive their medical certificate. Thus, an early emphasis on logic and reasoning was established to emphasise empirical observation and action based on rational thought. Medical students at Salerno would use these skills, what are now sometimes referred to as ‘soft’ skills, to identify the nature of the illness based on symptoms, environmental factors, and patient history, an early form of what now would be considered evidence-based medicine, and engage in rigorous discussion with colleagues to optimise their decisions [1] (Figure 1).

A miniature depicting the Schola Medica Salernitana from a copy of Avicenna’s Canons (from Wikipedia).
Cognition may be defined as the mental action or process of acquiring and using knowledge and understanding through thought, experience, and the senses. Cognition is essential to the practice of medicine and the two have co-existed for thousands of years. The study of human cognition is Psychology. It is difficult to think of an area of clinical practice that does not depend on cognitive factors, yet these are not generally discussed in recent times in health professions education (or in pre-medical education at universities). Indeed, most healthcare providers have limited exposure to the whole realm of cognition and what it means for their behavior. Unquestionably, cognition plays a critical role in clinical medicine. This is especially true for the process of establishing a diagnosis, and efforts to improve clinical reasoning are now front and center in the quest to reduce the harm associated with diagnostic errors (Table 1).
Cognitive factors relevant to the clinical practice of medicine.
Basic mental processes: How the brain processes information is fundamental to the diagnostic process. Thus, we need an understanding of basic cognitive processes such as perception, attention, information processing, memory, recall, and others. More advanced processes such as logic, reasoning and decision making are needed to integrate and make sense of what we perceive. Still higher orders of thinking are required to successfully apply a variety of cognitive bias mitigation strategies to the complicated and complex diagnostic process. |
|
Attention and detection: In particular, are important characteristics of cognition. Our brains are generally more attuned towards detecting the presence of things rather than their absence [2].This tendency may well have been adaptive – survival depending to a greater extent, say, on the detection of predators than the absence of gatherable food. The value of constructing a differential diagnosis that is likely to identify the correct diagnosis from a set of possible competing diagnoses has received widespread support [3], [4], [5], [6], [7]. Essentially, this involves engaging a deliberate process to consider a diagnosis which may ‘not be there.’ It is a systematic process classified as a higher order of thinking. [8] In the absence of a cognitive forcing strategy such as making a differential diagnosis, the analytic process of System 2 may be prematurely derailed by a variety of judgment and decision-making biases, e.g. anchoring, availability, confirmation bias and many others. |
|
Cognitive control and executive function in decision making: One of the crucial areas for decision making that underlies diagnostic performance is the degree of cognitive control and executive function achieved by the frontal lobes of the brain. Studies in cognitive science, and neuroscience have shown that fatigue, sleep deprivation and other homeostatic insults may modify the local metabolic environment leading to neurotoxicity and impaired function and performance in decision making [9]. Cognitive load, interruptions, distractions, stressors, and adverse work conditions are other known variables that modulate brain function. |
|
The trade-off between speed and accuracy (SATO): Time pressure is ubiquitous in medicine today, predisposing to the well-known SATO (Speed Accuracy Trade Off) effect, originally described in industrial psychology [10]. SATO says that the faster people have to work, the more likely they are to make mistakes. In emergency medicine, this was characterised as RACQITO (Resource Availability Continuous Quality Improvement Trade-Off) [11]. The cognitive benefits of slowing down have been demonstrated in a number of studies across a wide range of activities. |
|
Development of cognitive forcing strategies: Has resulted in our ability to recognise potentially vulnerable areas of human performance and produce a change for the better. Examples are abundant ranging from simple mnemonics to alert sounds or visual signals (connecting seat-belts, prompting attention when a truck is reversing, to smoke detector alarms), to constructing a differential diagnosis, to observing checklists for the safe operation of ventilator bundles, and many other applications. Every time an automated teller machine is used, it has a built-in forcing function to ensure that the owner’s card is not mistakenly left in the machine. These ubiquitous strategies simply predict common errors and force corrective, remedial steps to avoid or mitigate the error before it happens [12]. They have proved essential in the increasingly sophisticated environment of modern medicine |
|
Identification of cognitive and affective bias as major threats to rationality: These biases can be extremely influential on decision making – they are predominantly implicit, operating below the level of conscious awareness, and capable of skewing affect and judgment. Explicit training in awareness of all forms of bias is essential for clinical practice. Cognitive psychologists have identified cognitive biases as the major threat to rationality [13]. |
|
Improving communication: The study of communication in psychology explores how individuals communicate verbally and nonverbally, focusing on the social and psychological aspects of communication. This is especially important in times of pandemics when many visual cues may be obscured by personal protective equipment. A deeper understanding of how cognitive factors influence the dynamics of communication can improve patient-clinician interactions and reduce misunderstandings that lead to diagnostic errors. |
The history of how cognition has risen to become the central challenge in efforts to improve diagnosis retraces the origins of the diagnostic error movement itself. Although the early years of the patient safety movement focused largely on system-related aspects of performance, the work of psychologist James Reason, and his famous ‘Swiss Cheese’ model of accident causation, emphasized the critical role that cognitive and human factor elements play in accident causation. His classic book Human Error [14], published in 1990, recounted the work of Herbert Simon on human decision-making [15], and how heuristics and biases can influence cognition, the work of Kahneman and Tversky [16], 17]. Simon received the Nobel Prize for his work in behavioral economics in 1978 and Kahneman received his in 2002 (Tversky died in 1996 and Nobel Prizes are not awarded posthumously).
The new interest in cognition and decision-making was revolutionary, and was especially relevant to patient safety, where the consequence of faulty-decision making translated directly to patient harm and lives lost. The patient safety movement solidified a relationship between Psychology and Medicine that didn’t exist before, and modern efforts to improve diagnosis are squarely focused on issues at this interface. Cognition is relevant to essentially every aspect of diagnosis and medical practice generally.
Impact of the Halifax series
Each country had its own initiatives for the emergence of their respective patient safety movements. A major feature in Canada was a series of 10 annual conferences from 2001 to 2010, held at different venues across the country, in which a variety of safety issues were explored, one of which was diagnostic failure. The series was initially developed by two emergency physicians, Pat Croskerry and Sam Campbell, and an anesthesiologist Jan Davies. Diagnostic error emerged as a topic of interest in 2005 [3], 18] although its magnitude was not fully appreciated at that time and it was not an immediate focus. In fact, medication errors were seen as a greater threat to patient safety getting 70 mentions in the 1999 IOM report “To Err is Human”, compared with 2 for diagnostic error [19], 20].This was not altogether surprising. Medication errors were proximal in nature i.e. typically visible, tangible, and measurable, whereas diagnostic errors, due to their inherently cognitive nature, were invisible.
At the same time, the meetings of the National Patient Safety Foundation dramatically increased interest in patient safety in the United States, building on “To Err is Human” [19]. Juxtaposed to the emerging interest in cognition and clinical reasoning, the relevance to the problem of diagnostic errors was glaring. It was this mix that led to the creation of the diagnostic error in medicine conference series, the formation of The Society to Improve Diagnosis in Medicine, and ultimately to the landmark report on Improving Diagnosis in Health Care from the National Academies of Science, Engineering, and Medicine (NASEM) [21]. The chronology and key intermediaries that culminated in publishing this remarkable document are illustrated in Figure 2.

Chronology of events and organizations relevant to producing the NASEM report improving diagnosis in health care. TWA – Trans World Airlines; ASRS – Aviation Safety and Reporting System; IHI – Institute for Healthcare Improvement; NPSF – National Patient Safety Foundation; IOM – Institute of Medicine; DEM – Diagnostic Error in Medicine; SIDM – Society to Improve Diagnosis in Medicine; CIDM – Community Improving Diagnosis in Medicine.
Impact of improving diagnosis in health care
Improving Diagnosis in Health Care, for the first time in the patient safety movement, called out the central role of clinical reasoning as the basis for safe diagnosis: “Timely, accurate, and patient-centered diagnosis relies on proficiency in clinical reasoning, which is often regarded as the clinician’s quintessential competency.” The report emphasized a co-equal role of system-related and cognitive contributions to diagnostic error, and included clear direction in its conclusions and recommendations to improve clinical reasoning.
The report solidified the central role of Kahneman and Tversky’s ‘dual processing’ framework and relied extensively on the work of Croskerry and colleagues to illustrate how System 1 and System 2 processes constituted the essential elements of diagnosis throughout clinical practice (Figure 3) [22], [23], [24]. The particulars of this model, and its many implications for clinical decision making, lie at the heart of understanding the role cognition plays in every healthcare interaction today.
![Figure 3:
The dual process model of clinical reasoning. The model runs from left to right, describing two systems (1 and 2), and their respective processes (type 1 and type 2). The patient initially presents with symptoms or signs that may form a pattern that is immediately recognised and the diagnosis can be made in a fraction of a second, activating type 1 processing. The four channels in system 1 characterise the four principal origins of type 1 described in more detail in Croskerry et al. [22] If the illness presentation is not recognised, system 2 may be activated typically resulting in a slower response that may take minutes to hours to days. If system 1 is activated and system 2 doesn’t approve, it can override it (executive override) signifying an executive function of the cognitive control that is achieved by the prefrontal cortex. The T represents a toggle function, ability to move back and forth between the two systems. System 2 can be overridden by a system 1 response. Overall, there is a strong tendency to default to system 1 where less cognitive effort is required – referred to as cognitive miserliness.](/document/doi/10.1515/dx-2025-0106/asset/graphic/j_dx-2025-0106_fig_003.jpg)
The dual process model of clinical reasoning. The model runs from left to right, describing two systems (1 and 2), and their respective processes (type 1 and type 2). The patient initially presents with symptoms or signs that may form a pattern that is immediately recognised and the diagnosis can be made in a fraction of a second, activating type 1 processing. The four channels in system 1 characterise the four principal origins of type 1 described in more detail in Croskerry et al. [22] If the illness presentation is not recognised, system 2 may be activated typically resulting in a slower response that may take minutes to hours to days. If system 1 is activated and system 2 doesn’t approve, it can override it (executive override) signifying an executive function of the cognitive control that is achieved by the prefrontal cortex. The T represents a toggle function, ability to move back and forth between the two systems. System 2 can be overridden by a system 1 response. Overall, there is a strong tendency to default to system 1 where less cognitive effort is required – referred to as cognitive miserliness.
“Improving Diagnosis in Health Care” has had enormous impact on patient safety by leading to the emergence of a new field, focused on diagnostic error. Addressing diagnostic error requires attention to both the system-related and the cognitive elements of diagnosis, and thus a major imperative in this new area was the need to understand and improve diagnostic reasoning. Although landmark work on diagnostic reasoning had already been published [25], [26], [27], [28], [29], the NASEM report and the ensuing funding that it enabled has led to an explosion of interest and research on the cognitive aspects of diagnosis, and how it can be improved to avoid error. Clinicians, cognitive scientists, researchers, patients, and educators have all become interested and engaged in work to improve diagnostic reasoning, and their publications are appearing regularly in DIAGNOSIS and other leading patient safety journals.
Adjacent issues
Though not addressed in any detail in the report per se, the interest in improving diagnosis has stimulated work in several related areas:
The importance of knowledge: The best clinicians have both a comprehensive knowledge base and well-honed skills in clinical reasoning. Both are critical; in some respects it is certainly true that “knowledge is king” [30], but in practice most diagnostic errors reflect defects in clinical reasoning, not knowledge deficits [3], 31]. Interventions to improve knowledge [32], and interventions to improve clinical reasoning [33], 34] both have potential to improve diagnostic decision-making.
Affective bias: Clinical reasoning is inherently influenced by the heuristics and biases that are part of human cognition, and more recently emotional influences have been acknowledged as part of these subconscious tendencies, described as affective bias [35]. Examples of how affective bias can underlie diagnostic errors are appearing regularly in the literature.
Context: The context in which diagnosis takes place is another relevant factor to whether clinical reasoning will succeed or fail [36], 37]. Diagnosis is ‘situated’ in terms of where it takes place, who is involved, what resources are at hand, how much time is available, etc. Variables regarding the patient come into play as well, including how they describe their symptoms, how they emphasize particular elements (or not), how their disease manifests in them individually, and whether that disease is just becoming manifest or is well developed.
Contextual variables in the workplace also influence clinical reasoning. Some of these factors support optimal outcomes, including having enough time to think, having access to decision support tools, and to colleagues for second opinions. Other contextual elements are clearly detrimental, including time pressure and cognitive load, stress, fatigue, sleep deprivation, poor ergonomic conditions, and others.
The most conspicuous actor in the workplace in regard to its influence on diagnosis is the electronic health record (the EHR). Since their widespread adoption in the 2000s, EHR’s have transformed the diagnostic process. Many features have been beneficial: notes are legible; consults, imaging reports, and lab tests are easily found and are in chronological order; patient input is enhanced through their use of patient portals, etc.
However, multiple downsides have become evident in using EHR’s [38], [39], [40]. A study using cognitive task analysis to understand the impact of the EHR found that it did not support clinical reasoning, it interfered with the clinician’s ability to work collaboratively, and in impeded getting ‘the big picture’ of the patient and his\her problems [38]. The EHR acts to transform an individual skill set into a ‘joint cognitive system’, a mixture of human and machine. By suppressing intrinsic motivation, an inherent feature of System 1 which embraces the ‘’higher pleasures of work’ (meaning-making, exploration, inspiration and creativity), physician burn-out may increase, to the detriment of quality patient care [41].
Calibration refers to the extent to which an individual has a sense of whether their diagnoses are right or wrong, or how certain they are about this conclusion. Improving calibration, especially through the role of feedback [42] has emerged as a novel approach to improving diagnosis accuracy in practice [43].
Diagnostic excellence has been newly defined as the goal of work to improve diagnosis, and the elements of excellence have been defined [44]: Diagnosis should be accurate, timely, efficient, patient-centered, and SAFE.
Accident investigation: Root cause analysis has become a standard approach to investigating and learning from safety breakdowns throughout healthcare. The existing guidelines on conducting RCA have now been supplemented by clear guidelines on how to find and understand the cognitive contributions to diagnostic error [45]. This allows a more distal understanding of factors that may have initially triggered the error.
Next steps
Progress in appreciating the importance of cognition in diagnosis has been rapid and remarkable. There are two critical next steps in this journey:
(1) Improve education. Educational programs in the health professions need to ensure their graduates appreciate the role of cognition in diagnosis and in practice more generally [46], 47]. It requires the attainment of rationality so that the best possible decisions are made about patients given the available resources. This needs to include training on how to optimize clinical reasoning, and how to prevent, or if need be, recognize and catch diagnostic errors before they cause harm. This training can and should be done in an interprofessional manner, so that doctors, nurses [48], nurse practitioners, physician assistants, pharmacists, and everyone who is involved in diagnosis understands that diagnosis is best done as a team, and that everyone has a role to play [49].
Although pioneering programs have been developed that emphasize cognition over content [50], recent reports indicate that that only a small fraction of education programs have incorporated training on cognition in clinical reasoning [51]. This is something that every program should be doing. A key stumbling block, however, is the need for faculty development programs to equip educators with the skills and tools to provide this curriculum i.e. the curriculum needs to provide comprehensive training in cognition and how it relates to clinical practice. Given that cognitive and affective processes are considered by cognitive psychologists to be the major threat to rationality, appropriate training needs to address these issues in undergraduate medical education and continuing into postgraduate training. The requirements for attaining rationality and sound judgment, along with an awareness of how thinking can be confounded and misled, will need special attention. The need to improve education on cognition is an acute problem and remains largely unaddressed. If it is not fixed it will affect successive generations of trainees.
(2) Trial interventions. With the cognitive issues that can undermine diagnosis now well defined, it is time to begin testing interventions that might reduce the likelihood of harm. Many interventions have been suggested: Improving teamwork, engaging patients in the diagnostic process, using decision support tools, improving reflection [52], calibration, metacognition, slowing down, and so on. Each of these is reasonable and addresses one or more of the cognitive shortcomings known to be problematic. Early evidence has identified beneficial effects in several areas, including getting second opinions [53] and group-based input [54], 55], using decision support [56], and improving teamwork [57].
The explosion of interest in artificial intelligence includes many applications addressed at improving diagnosis, and refocuses attention on the central importance of knowledge as a pilar of diagnosis [58]. Systems to improve visual diagnosis in radiology and pathology are the most advanced and the most successful so far. The new large language models are being trialed for general diagnosis as well, with variable but generally impressive results. However, it is worth noting that advanced decision support for differential diagnosis has been available for several decades now, with disappointing uptake by clinicians in practice. As these new artificial intelligence modalities are refined and validated, exposure will need to begin during training [59], and they will need to be incorporated more widely in actual practice. Moreover, medical expertise will need to take on new and critical roles in the oversight of AI usage and the detection of AI bias and errors.
Conclusions
Cognitive processes have a wide application in clinical practice, and thanks to the 2015 report on Improving Diagnosis in Health Care, the imperative to improve the reliability of diagnosis is now widely recognized. Understanding the role of cognition in clinical reasoning, and how this can go astray, is central to efforts to reduce diagnostic errors. The Schola Medica Salernitana, the Salerno School of Medicine, likely got it right: Knowing HOW is just as critical as knowing THAT; both types of knowledge are essential to the diagnostic process.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Dr. Henk Schmidt for his independent critical review.
-
Research ethics: Not applicable.
-
Informed consent: Not applicable.
-
Author contributions: Both authors have accepted responsibility for the entire content of this manuscript and approved its submission.
-
Use of Large Language Models, AI and Machine Learning Tools: None declared.
-
Conflict of interest: The authors state no conflict of interest.
-
Research funding: None declared.
-
Data availability: Not applicable.
References
1. de Devitiis, E, Cappabianca, P, de Divitiis, O. The “schola medic salernitana”: the forerunner of the modern university medical schools. Neurosurgey 2004;55:722–44.10.1227/01.NEU.0000139458.36781.31Search in Google Scholar PubMed
2. Barnett, B. Why nothing matters. Mamaroneck, NY, USA: Aeon; 2025.Search in Google Scholar
3. Graber, ML, Franklin, N, Gordon, R. Diagnostic error in internal medicine. Arch Intern Med 2005;165:1493–9. https://doi.org/10.1001/archinte.165.13.1493.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
4. Mandin, H, Jones, A, Woloschuk, W, Harasym, P. Helping students learn to think like experts when solving clinical problems. Acad Med 1997;72:173–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001888-199703000-00009.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
5. Elstein, A. Clinical reasoning in medicine. In: Higgs, JJM, editor. Clinical reasoning in the health professions. Oxford, England: Butterworth-Heinemann Ltd; 1995:49–59 pp.Search in Google Scholar
6. Meyer, A, Payne, V, Meeks, D, Rao, R, Singh, H. Physicians’ diagnostic accuracy, confidence, and resource requests. JAMA Intern Med 2013;173:1952–8. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.10081.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
7. Kostopoulou, O, O’udhoff, J, Nath, R. Prompting GP trainees to generate alternative diagnoses improves diagnosttic accuracy. BMJ Qual Saf 2017;26:558–64.Search in Google Scholar
8. Cook, C, Decary, S. Higher order thinking about differential diagnosis. Braz J Phys Ther 2019;24:1–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bjpt.2019.01.010.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
9. Wiehler, A, Branzoli, F, Adanyugeh, I, Mochel, F, Pessiglione, M. A neuro-metabolic account of why daylong cognitive work alters the control of economic decisions. Curr Biol 2022;32:3564–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2022.07.010.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
10. Fitts, P. Cognitive aspects of information processing: III. Set for speed versus accuracy. J Exp Psychol 1966;71:849–57. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0023232.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
11. Croskerry, P. The cognitive imperative: thinking about how we think. Acad Emerg Med 2000;7:1223–31. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1553-2712.2000.tb00467.x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
12. Croskerry, P. Cognitive bias mitigation: becoming better diagnosticians. In: Croskerry, P, Cosby, K, Graber, M, Singh, H, editors. Diagnosis: interpreting the shadows. Boca Raton, FL: Taylor and Francis Group; CRC Press; 2018.10.1201/9781315116334Search in Google Scholar
13. Stanovich, K. What intelligence tests miss. The psychology of rational thought. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 2009.Search in Google Scholar
14. Reason, J. Human error. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 1990.Search in Google Scholar
15. Simon, H. Invariants of human behavior. Ann Rev Psychol 1990;41:1–19. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.41.1.1.Search in Google Scholar
16. Kahneman, D, Slovic, P, Tversky, A. Judgement under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Cambridge, England: Cambridge Universisty Press; 1982.10.1017/CBO9780511809477Search in Google Scholar
17. Kahneman, D. Thinking fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Strauss and Giroux; 2011.Search in Google Scholar
18. Graber, M. Diagnostic errors in medicine: a case of neglect. Jt Comm J Qual Patient Saf 2005;31:106–13. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1553-7250-05-31015-4.Search in Google Scholar
19. Kohn, L, Corrigan, J, Donaldson, M. To Err is human: building a safer health system. Washington D.C.: National Academy of Medicine; 2000.Search in Google Scholar
20. Wachter, R. Why diagnostic errors don’t get any respect – and what can be done about them. Health Aff 2010;29:1605–10. https://doi.org/10.1377/hlthaff.2009.0513.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
21. National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. Improving diagnosis in health care. Washington DC: National Academies Press; 2015.Search in Google Scholar
22. Croskerry, P, Singhal, G, Mamede, S. Cognitive debiasing 2: impediments to and strategies for change. BMJ Qual Saf 2013;22ii:65–72. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjqs-2012-001713.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
23. Croskerry, P, Singhal, G, Mamede, S. Cognitive debiasing 1: origins of bias and theory of debiasing. BMJ Qual Saf 2013;22:ii58–64. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjqs-2012-001712.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
24. Croskerry, P. A universal model of diagnostic reasoning. Acad Med 2009;84:1022–8. https://doi.org/10.1097/acm.0b013e3181ace703.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
25. Gruver, RH, Freis, ED. A study of diagnostic errors. Ann Int Med 1957;47:108–20. https://doi.org/10.7326/0003-4819-47-1-108.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
26. Kassirer, JP, Kopelman, RI. Cognitive errors in diagnosis: instantiation, classification, and consequences. Am J Med 1989;86:433–41 https://doi.org/10.1016/0002-9343-89-90342-2.Search in Google Scholar
27. Cabot, RC. A study of mistaken diagnosis; Based on the analysis of 1000 autopsies and a comparison with the clinical findings. JAMA 1910;55:1343–50. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.1910.04330160011005.Search in Google Scholar
28. Elstein, AS, Higgs, JJM. Clinical reasoning in medicine. Clinical reasoning in the health professions. Oxford, England: Butterworth-Heinemann Ltd; 1995:49–59 pp.Search in Google Scholar
29. Voytovich, AE, Rippey, RM, Suffredini, A. Premature conclusions in diagnostic reasoning. J Med Education 1985;60:302–7. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001888-198504000-00004.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
30. Dhaliwal, G. Premature closure? Not so fast. BMJ Qual Saf 2017;26:87–9. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjqs-2016-005267.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
31. Nendaz, M, Perrier, A. Diagnostic errors and flaws in clinical reasoning: mechanisms and prevention in practice. Swiss Med Wkly 2012;142:w13706. https://doi.org/10.4414/smw.2012.13706.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
32. Mamede, S, de Carvalho-Filho, A, de Faria, R, Franci, D, do Patrocino-Tenoria Nunes, M, Ribeiro, L, et al.. Immunising physicians against availability bias in diagnostic reasoning: a randomised controlled experiment. BMJ Qual Saf 2020;29:550–9. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjqs-2019-010079.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
33. Croskerry, P. The rational diagnostician and achieving diagnostic excellence. JAMA 2022;327:317–8. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2021.24988.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
34. Pinnock, R, Ritchie, D, Gallagher, S, Henning, M, Webster, C. The efficacy of mindful practice in improving diagnosis in healthcare: a systematic review and evidence synthesis. Adv Health Sci Educ 2021;26:785–809. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-020-10022-x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
35. Croskerry, P. Diagnostic failure: a cognitive and affective approach. Advances in patient safety: from research to implementation. 2. AHRQ Publication Nos. 050021. Rockville, MD: Agency for Health Care Research and Quality; 2005:241–54 pp.Search in Google Scholar
36. Croskerry, P. Context is everything or how could I have been that stupid? Healthc Q 2009;12:e171–6. https://doi.org/10.12927/hcq.2009.20945.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
37. Schmidt, H, Norman, G, Mamede, S, Magzoub, M. The influence of context on diagnostic reasoning: a narrative synthesis of experimental findings. J Eval Clin Pract 2024;30:1091–101. https://doi.org/10.1111/jep.14023.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
38. Pfaff, M, Eris, O, Weir, C, Anganes, A, Crotty, T, Rahman, M, et al.. Analysis of the cognitive demands of electronic health record use. J Biomed Inf 2021;113:103633. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbi.2020.103633.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
39. James, T, Courtney, W, Mangus, C, Parker, S, Chandanabhumma, P, Cassady, C, et al.. Everything is electronic health record-driven: the role of the electronic health record in the emergency department diagnostic process. JAMIA Open 2025;8:1–8. https://doi.org/10.1093/jamiaopen/ooaf029.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
40. Dixit, R, Boxley, C, Samuel, S, Mohan, V, Ratwani, R, Gold, J. EHR-use issues and diagnostic error: a scoping review and framework. J Patient Saf 2023;19:e25–30. https://doi.org/10.1097/pts.0000000000001081.Search in Google Scholar
41. Weir, C, Taber, P, Taft, T, Reese, T, Jones, B, Del Fiol, G. Feeling and thinking: can theories of human motivation explain how EHR design impacts clinician burnout? JAMIA; 2021:10421046 p.10.1093/jamia/ocaa270Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
42. Croskerry, P. The feedback sanction. Acad Emerg Med 2000;7:1232–8. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1553-2712.2000.tb00468.x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
43. Zwaan, L, Hautz, WE. Bridging the gap between uncertainty, confidence and diagnostic accuracy: calibration is key. BMJ Qual Saf 2019;28:352–5. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjqs-2018-009078.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
44. Singh, H, Marinez, A, Mushtaq, U, Shahid, U. The safer Dx checklist - ten high priority practices for diagnostic excellence. Available from: https://www.ihi.org/sites/default/files/Safer-Dx-Checklist.pdf.Search in Google Scholar
45. Graber, M, Castro, G, Danforth, M, Tilly, J-L, Croskerry, P, El-Kareh, R, et al.. Root cause analysis of cases involving diagnosis. Diagnosis 2024;11:353–68. https://doi.org/10.1515/dx-2024-0102.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
46. Bowen, JL. Educational strategies to promote clinical diagnostic reasoning. N Engl J Med 2006;355:2217–25. https://doi.org/10.1056/nejmra054782.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
47. Schmidt, H, Mamede, S. How to improve the teaching of clinical reasoning – a narrative review and proposal. Med Educ 2015;49:961–73. https://doi.org/10.1111/medu.12775.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
48. Gleason, K, Harkless, G, Stanley, J, Olson, A, Graber, M. The critical need for nursing education to address the diagnostic process. Nurs Outlook 2021;69:362–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.outlook.2020.12.005.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
49. Graber, M, Holmboe, E, Stanley, J, Danielson, J, Schoenbaum, S, Olson, A. A call to action: next steps to advance diagnosis education in the health professions. Diagnosis 2021;9:166–75. https://doi.org/10.1515/dx-2021-0103.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
50. Schwartzstein, R, Dienstag, J, King, R, Chang, B, Flanagan, J, Besche, H, et al.. The Harvard medical school pathways curriculum: reimagining developmentally appropriate medical education for contemporary learners. Acad Med 2020;95:1687–95. https://doi.org/10.1097/acm.0000000000003270.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
51. Rencic, J, Trowbridge, R, Fagan, M, Szauter, K, Durning, S. Clinical reasoning education at US medical schools: results from a national survey of internal medicine clerkship directors. J Gen Int Med 2017;32:1242–6. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-017-4159-y.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
52. Mamede, S, Schmidt, H. Deliberate reflection and clinical reasoning: founding ideas and empirical findings. Med Educ 2023;57:76–85. https://doi.org/10.1111/medu.14863.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
53. Freund, Y, Goulet, H, Leblanc, J, Bokobza, J, Ray, P, Maignan, M, et al.. Effect of systematic physician cross-checking on reducing adverse events in the emergency department: the CHARMED cluster randomized trial. JAMA Intern Med 2018;178:812–9. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamainternmed.2018.0607.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central
54. Blanchard, M, Herzog, S, Kammer, J, Zoller, N, Kostopoulou, O, Kurvers, R. Collective intelligence increases diagnostic accuracy in a general practice setting. Med Decis Mak 2024;44:451–62. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272989x241241001.Search in Google Scholar
55. Sherbino, J, Sibbald, M, Norman, G, LoGiudice, A, Keuhl, A, Lee, M, et al.. Crowdsourcing a diagnosis? Exploring the accuracy of the size and type of group diagnosis: an experimental study. BMJ Qual Saf 2024;33:790–4. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjqs-2023-016695.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
56. Kostopoulou, O, Porat, T, Corrigan, D, Mahmoud, S, Delaney, B. Diagnostic accuracy of GPs when using an early- intervention decision support system: a high-fidelity simulation. Br J Gen Pract 2017;67:e201–7. https://doi.org/10.3399/bjgp16x688417.Search in Google Scholar
57. Olson, A, Durning, S, Branson, C, Sick, B, Lane, K, Rencic, J. Teamwork in clinical reasoning – cooperative or parallel play? Diagnosis 2020;7:307–12. https://doi.org/10.1515/dx-2020-0020.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
58. Norman, G, Pelaccia, T, Wyer, P, Sherbino, J. Dual process models of clinical reasoning: the central role of knowledge in diagnostic expertise. J Eval Clin Pract 2024;30:788–96. https://doi.org/10.1111/jep.13998.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
59. Furfaro, D, Celi, L, Schwartzstein, R. Artificial intelligence in medical education; A long way to go. Chest 2023;165:771–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chest.2023.11.028.Search in Google Scholar PubMed
© 2025 the author(s), published by De Gruyter, Berlin/Boston
This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.