Home Self-regulated learning and the future of diagnostic reasoning education
Article Publicly Available

Self-regulated learning and the future of diagnostic reasoning education

  • Alexander Goldowsky EMAIL logo and Joseph Rencic ORCID logo
Published/Copyright: December 8, 2022

Abstract

Diagnostic reasoning is a foundational ability of health professionals. The goal of enhancing clinical reasoning education is improved diagnostic accuracy and reduced diagnostic error. In order to do so, health professions educators need not only help learners improve their clinical reasoning, but teach them how to develop expert performance. An evidence-based learning strategy that is strongly associated with expert performance is self-regulated learning (SRL). SRL is the modulation of “self-generated thoughts, feelings, and actions that are planned and cyclically adapted to the attainment of personal goals”. At this time, there is little data on the use of SRL to improve diagnostic reasoning. However, there appear to be numerous opportunities to utilize SRL in novel ways to improve diagnostic reasoning given what is already known about this competency. Examples that are discussed include the role SRL can play in simulation, clinical experiences, assessment, and novel technologies such as virtual reality, artificial intelligence, and machine learning. SRL is central to the philosophy that health professionals are life-long learners, as it teaches learners “how to learn”. SRL has the potential to help achieve the goal of improved diagnostic accuracy and reduced diagnostic error.

Introduction

Diagnostic reasoning is a foundational ability of health professionals. Although clinical reasoning has been taught to students for millennia, the National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine (NASEM) has recently recommended enhancing clinical reasoning education [1]. The goal of enhancing clinical reasoning education includes improved diagnostic accuracy and reduced diagnostic error (i.e., clinical reasoning performance), as well as better resource utilization and improved patient experience. While perfect diagnostic accuracy is unrealistic, attaining a threshold such that treatment can be delivered without knowing a complete diagnosis (management reasoning) or having the knowledge that more information is needed (diagnostic reasoning) are key skills for the learner. There is an absence of randomized controlled trial (RCT) data that clinical reasoning education improves actual patient outcomes, but observational evidence that health professions learners’ diagnostic ability improves over their undergraduate and graduate medical education careers seems adequate to pursue improved clinical reasoning education.

To operationalize NASEM’s recommendation, health professions educators need not only help learners improve their clinical reasoning, but teach them how to develop expert performance (i.e., high diagnostic accuracy with a low error rate). Traditionally, the primary strategy used to teach clinical reasoning focuses on essential clinical reasoning knowledge acquisition, specifically illness scripts (i.e., diseases’ symptoms, signs, laboratory and radiological findings) and base rates of diseases (i.e., prevalence, incidence), as well as diagnostic frameworks (e.g., pre-renal, intra-renal, post-renal kidney injury), because knowledge is associated with diagnostic success [2]. The Competencies to Improve Diagnosis have described these as well as other curricular objectives for clinical reasoning [3]. However, this approach presupposes that students know the learning and behavioral strategies that lead to expert clinical reasoning performance. In fact, students need to both understand these frameworks, as well as the techniques for how to best learn them. Another approach to teaching diagnostic reasoning is low-fidelity simulation (e.g., paper-based, case-based or problem-based learning), with or without higher-fidelity simulation (e.g., simulated manikins). However, there is no definitive evidence on the most effective way to teach clinical reasoning. If health professions educators want to increase the likelihood of students developing expert performance in clinical reasoning, they need to teach and motivate students to use evidence-based learning strategies, particularly because life-long learning is necessary in the health professions where knowledge continuously expands and changes.

Self-regulated learning to develop expert performance in diagnostic reasoning

An evidence-based learning strategy that is strongly associated with expert performance in other fields (sports, video gaming) is self-regulated learning (SRL) [4, 5]. SRL is defined as the modulation of “self-generated thoughts, feelings, and actions that are planned and cyclically adapted to the attainment of personal goals” (Table 1) [6, 7]. Another related strategy, deliberate practice, refers to a highly structured learning activity where learners set explicit learning goals to improve performance in a skill that they co-create with a coach who carefully observes performance to provide specific, meaningful feedback [7]. Performance improves through an iterative process of effortful, highly-focused practice, followed by assessment and targeted feedback [8]. Within medical education, deliberate practice has been linked to success via simulation training and procedural skills training [9, 10]. It should be noted, though, that studies on deliberate practice are contextually narrow, though elements can inform expertise in clinical reasoning. Regardless, both require some tolerance for repetition and “boredom”. While health professions educators are encouraging students to use SRL, there is no literature regarding its utility in improving diagnostic reasoning [11, 12].

Table 1:

The steps of self-regulated learning and current examples in health professions education.

Self-regulated learning step Definition Examples
Goal-and strategy-setting Setting a goal to compare progress to Pre-rotation/learning session surveys/standardized questionnaires asking learner goals prior to starting
Problem-based learning experiences in the classroom; ask learners what they want to achieve from the experience
Self-monitoring Process of keeping track of thoughts, motivations, behaviors, and environments Diagnostic reasoning case databases (ex. New England Journal of Medicine Healer)
Learner journaling of patient and diagnostic reasoning case experiences
Learner clinical portfolios Case-based repetition of diagnostic reasoning problems
Self-reflection loop Cycle of monitoring the effectiveness of the self-regulated learning process In-person structured diagnostic reasoning cases (OSCEs)
Formalized feedback documents detailing areas of success and improvement for the learner (in-person or virtual)
Regular learner feedback sessions (ex. Graduate medical education mid-year and end-of-year feedback)
Control Selection and adaptation of strategies used during the self-regulated learning process “Learning how to learn” courses Diagnostic reasoning bias education
Doctoring and diagnostic reasoning courses

Evidence shows SRL is both associated with expert performance and is a teachable competency that can be applied to medical education [13, 14]. Teaching and encouraging the use of SRL-related techniques appears to help students better utilize them in practice and promotes students’ learning [15], [16], [17]. Prior studies have shown associations between perceived SRL levels and better academic performance of medical students [12, 18]. There is also suggestion that SRL is important in clinical reasoning and life-long learning in medicine, which is especially beneficial to early-stage trainees, although the data are limited [17, 18]. Most have looked at learning outcomes associated with student self-report of SRL behaviors. A single-site study suggested that self-reported use of SRL leads to gains in self-efficacy [19]. A qualitative study of post-graduate trainees suggested that use of certain SRL strategies may aid in trainee success via improved self-confidence and autonomy [20]. It should be noted, though, that SRL’s impact on diagnostic accuracy and efficiency in clinical reasoning is lacking, as is its longitudinal impact on clinical reasoning skills.

Best practices for teaching self-regulated learning

SRL teaching should be done in a developmentally appropriate way based on learner level. For early-stage health professions students, it is particularly important that the educator and learner co-create learning goals due to the Dunning-Kruger phenomenon (i.e., students do not know what they do not know) [21, 22]. Strategies that emphasize learner goal-setting and self-efficacy have the greatest impact [23, 24]. In more advanced learners, metacognitive approaches (e.g., structured reflection on a differential diagnosis) are more ideal, as compared to novice learners, who might benefit from a more socio-cognitive approach (e.g., positive encouragement when a student formulates a proper differential diagnosis) [25], [26], [27], [28]. Using these best practices allows for improved learner ability to use SRL effectively.

We believe that early-stage trainees (medical students and residents/fellows) would benefit greatly from learning how to become “expert learners”. We recognize that such content will be a challenge to fit into already crowded health professions curricula. On the other hand, we believe that teaching SRL to health professions students is as important as any other content area, given the ever-changing landscape of medical knowledge [29]. SRL is a strategy that master diagnosticians use to learn. We believe that there are many opportunities to incorporate SRL into health professions education.

Given studies in other domains highlighting the critical nature of SRL in developing expert performance, we believe that health professions schools should teach students how to apply SRL and deliberate practice to their learning. Educators can provide learners feedback on their use of it to achieve NASEM’s goal of enhancing diagnostic reasoning education [30, 31].

Contexts for developing and practicing SRL

One significant challenge of developing expert performance in clinical reasoning is the lack of iterative practice with multiple cases for a given problem or disease ranging from typical to atypical presentations. Other industries (e.g., aviation) have used simulation to fill in the gaps of inexperience to provide employees opportunities to develop critical knowledge and skills in safe environments [32]. Health professions pre-clinical and clinical curricula routinely uses low-fidelity simulation (e.g., paper-based, case-based or problem-based learning) to provide learners opportunities to practice clinical reasoning, but uptake of higher-fidelity simulation (e.g., simulated manikins) has been limited by the high cost, both time and money [33].

Furthermore, both low-and high-fidelity simulations are typically structured as “one and done” experiences (e.g., one case of myocardial infarction (MI) or one case of pulmonary embolism), rather than longitudinal curricula that help students build and maintain their clinical reasoning. The rationale for this approach is that students and trainees will “see” these problems and diseases in their clinical experiences and continue to develop their clinical reasoning in this way. Some schools require students to fill out patient logs to confirm they have cared for patients with curricularly “required” diseases and if not, they are assigned a case to make up for deficiencies. Again, most of these requirements are “one and done” approaches (e.g., “You didn’t see an MI on your clerkship, so you must complete this single assigned MI case and you will fulfill your requirement.”). It is known that repetition is key in the development of clinical reasoning abilities. It allows for pattern recognition and, with time, the ability of the learner to override thought-processing systems when faced with information that is not in agreement with known patterns [34]. Unfortunately, particularly in undergraduate health professions education, opportunities for repetition are often few.

Given these challenges and the limited educational benefits of “one and done” approaches to developing diagnostic reasoning expert performance, we recommend applying SRL to such exercises. The key to this approach is a large database of cases that cover common and cannot miss diagnoses in the common problems that health professionals will encounter, as well as meaningful assessment data. The former can be created with significant effort from textbooks, clinical problem-solving exercises published in journals, podcasts, and web-based resources/applications. Using the SRL cycle and principles of deliberate practice, learners should co-create appropriate developmental goals with evidence-based strategies for achieving them (e.g., spaced learning, interleaved practice) with a coach, ideally, to provide guidance and motivation (Table 2). Then, both learner and coach should monitor and reflect on performance to determine whether goals were achieved. An action plan should be developed to create new goals if they were achieved or to modify the strategies used if the goals were not achieved. If no coach is available, then feedback can be provided through the resources themselves and students can use this feedback to self-reflect and build their own goals, strategies, and action plans. It should be noted that in order for SRL to be successful, motivation on the part of the learner is a pre-requisite. This is one of the areas that can be regulated by the individual [35]. For this reason, SRL works particularly well in scenarios where the learner is highly motived to succeed, such as remediation. For SRL to work more generally in health professions education, it is vital to establish a learner’s motivation for completing a given clinical reasoning task.

Table 2:

Examples of typical educational goals based on stage of learning in health professions education.

Learning stage Examples
Early Learn diagnostic frameworks for common problems
Learn epidemiology and clinical findings for common and cannot miss diseases
Develop pattern recognition (non-analytic reasoning)
Develop analytic reasoning (Bayesian, pathophysiologic, etc.)
Develop clinical skills that enhance diagnostic ability
Advanced Generate a prioritized differential diagnosis including common and cannot miss diseases
Improve diagnostic accuracy
Improve diagnostic thoroughness (critical findings requested/total critical findings)
Improve diagnostic efficiency (critical findings requested/total findings requested)

To achieve the goals described in Table 2, we recommend the following elements be included in simulated cases: (1) an anatomical/pathophysiological diagnostic framework or an algorithmic approach to the problem, which provides the student with a useful knowledge organization structure to help build their own frameworks over time (e.g., in a case of syncope and dyspnea, begin with frameworks for each based on pathophysiology), (2) data selection opportunities or a question prior to revealing a patient’s clinical findings that encourages students to list findings that significantly increase or decrease the probability of diseases on the differential diagnosis (i.e., questions that foster a hypothesis-driven approach to data collection), and (3) prioritization and justification of the differential diagnosis (hypotheses) of at least 2–3 diseases, preferably focused on the most likely and cannot miss diseases [36].

We recommend a developmental approach to cases both in terms of fidelity and typicality. Early learners should train with typical presentations of common and cannot miss diseases using low-fidelity simulation (e.g., paper or web-based cases with words only) with faculty or scaffolding built into the case (e.g., algorithms) to aid them given that self-efficacy is a critical determinant of the SRL quality [20]. Low-fidelity simulations also reduce cognitive load, which can negatively impact student performance [37]. Once students master such cases, moderate to high fidelity simulation can be introduced, which require additional diagnostic competencies. For example, in a chest pain case caused by aortic stenosis, an audio file of the murmur, an ECG showing left ventricular hypertrophy, and a chest X-ray showing an enlarged cardiac silhouette can be provided, rather than the description. Alternatively, the entire case can be run in a simulation lab, where the learner elicits the history, performs the physical exam, and requests and interprets labs/studies. Once learners demonstrate competence in prioritizing and justifying the differential diagnosis, scaffolding can be reduced/removed and atypical presentations of common and cannot miss diseases can be introduced.

Regardless of format, meaningful feedback is essential for SRL and deliberate practice. In increasing order of resource utilization, feedback options include: (1) a gold standard note learners read or a video they watch that explains the clinical reasoning clearly, (2) peer feedback from co-students or final year students, (3) feedback from graduates, (4) faculty feedback, and (5) video review with self-assessment followed by faculty review of the video with synchronous or asynchronous feedback to the learner. The last option is likely most effective, as learners discover the gaps between their self-assessment and the observer’s assessment, which has the highest potential to improve their ability to monitor and self-reflect. An example of a case database that gives particularly strong feedback to learners is Healer from the New England Journal of Medicine [38]. We recognize there is no substitute for real clinical experience, but we believe simulation is an important complement that can enhance clinical reasoning knowledge acquisition and may be transferrable to actual patient care.

SRL and clinical experiences

Using SRL in the clinical experiences is essential to developing expert diagnostic reasoning performance as these learning experiences are most likely to transfer to future clinical encounters. We know that the transition to clinical experiences for health professions students leads to dynamic changes in how they learn. External motivation increased and use of metacognitive strategies decreased in one study [17]. Another showed that novice clinical students’ self-regulated learning is influenced greatly by others, including peers, particularly in goal-setting. This contrasts with more experienced clinical students, who are less apt to be influenced by a single individual and can more independently set goals and develop a clear learning trajectory [39]. We believe that this knowledge can be leveraged to better utilize SRL in the clinical experiences of health professions learners. Early emphasis on maintaining metacognitive approaches to learning is a key area that should be focused on, given the decrease in utilization of these strategies early in clinical experiences. Additionally, given the significant influence that peers and individuals at more advanced career/learning stages have on novice clinical students, this is a group that teaching on how to teach SRL techniques may be of great benefit, particularly how to co-create clinical reasoning goals. The aim is to create a more individualized clinical experience for the learner.

SRL and assessment

There is a dearth of information regarding the role of SRL in assessment in health professions education. However, in other fields, the effective use of SRL correlates significantly with performance [4, 5]. If a similar strong correlation can be demonstrated with diagnostic reasoning performance, SRL assessment could be an assessment game-changer because diagnostic reasoning is both content-and context-specific. Content specificity refers to the fact that diagnostic reasoning is highly dependent on clinical knowledge (i.e., a dermatologist may be an expert in diagnosing skin conditions, but a novice in diagnosing cardiac conditions) [40, 41]. Context specificity refers to the fact that clinicians’ diagnostic reasoning performances correlate poorly across different occasions even when the patient’s clinical findings and diagnosis remain the same [42, 43]. These two characteristics of diagnostic reasoning lead to the requirement for large sample sizes to assess the competence of clinical reasoning performance.

An example of an approach focused on performance process is self-regulated learning micro-analytic assessment (SRL-MAT). It has been used to generate information on the strategies learners’ used when faced with a diagnostic reasoning case [44]. In SRL-MAT, learners are asked directly about their strategies for diagnostic reasoning, motivations, and planning process while in the midst of a clinical case (Table 3). Based on this information and the results at the conclusion of the case, the teacher can give targeted feedback that the learner can apply to their learning and diagnostic process.

Table 3:

Self-regulated learning micro-analytic assessment process and example questions. Adapted from cleary, TJ et al. [41].

Self-regulated learning process Micro-analytic assessment question
Goal-setting What goal do you have for this activity?
Strategic planning What will you need to do to perform well on this activity?
Self-efficacy How confident are you that you will get the most-likely diagnosis on your first try?
Monitoring What have you been thinking about as you have been doing this activity?
Self-evaluation What percentage of lab tests that you ordered do you think an expert would say were essential for this activity?

A final assessment consideration is the nature of longitudinal, aggregated feedback provided to the learner, which is critical for learners to visualize their areas of growth and continued weaknesses. For diagnostic reasoning, this feedback should likely be separated by diagnostic problem and specific disease (e.g., dyspnea, pneumonia; chest pain, GERD), given content specificity. Under each, diagnostic accuracy, differential diagnosis accuracy, knowledge of diagnostic frameworks, data collection thoroughness, and efficiency can be listed, depending on the assessment. In addition, time on task, cognitive load, and confidence could be added to provide evidence of automatization of diagnostic reasoning (i.e., quicker to correct diagnosis with less cognitive effort signifies expert performance), as well as calibration (i.e., correlation of diagnostic accuracy and confidence). A recent development in the graphic depiction of such data is “learning curves”. They provide a visual answer to the question, “How well am I learning?” [44]. Validity evidence for learning curves exist in visual pattern-recognition skills, such as ECGs and radiograph interpretation [45, 46]. By using learning curves, learners and educators can visually identify improvement over time and determine whether competence has been achieved when compared to peers or consensus-based benchmarks. Learners can then modify their learning goals and/or strategies if they are not achieving diagnostic competency benchmarks at the target time intervals.

The use of SRL as an assessment tool has thus far been limited. However, the perceived benefits of its use are exciting and may allow for an improved ability to assess learners’ diagnostic reasoning capabilities. We view diagnostic reasoning as one of the pre-eminent pillars of medical education and life-long learning in medicine. Being able to view the learning trajectories of students and physicians in this realm via assessment is key and can serve as a means for early intervention for learners in any stage of their career who may be falling behind. It can also afford individuals on an accelerated learning curve the opportunity to push themselves further to becoming the “expert learner” that we strongly advocate for. SRL is uniquely suited to diagnostic reasoning assessment.

The future of SRL: novel technology

The technological innovations being implemented within medicine are growing by the day. We envision SRL being able to utilize them as a means of improving learner performance, and not just for their novelty. Virtual reality is being used as a method of instruction in numerous fields and is just now making its entry into medicine. Its primary uses have been in anatomy teaching, but the opportunities to use this in diagnostic reasoning are limitless [47]. Students or residents would have the ability to use programmed cases that place them in the middle of the hospital ward or clinic and not just looking at a computer screen. The immersive experience is more realistic. In addition, it would allow for a true preceptor to insert themselves within the simulation such that SRL can be engaged in with the learner. In this way, learners can talk through goals for a case, thought process, differential diagnosis, and ultimately what strategies worked and did not work.

As this technology becomes more refined, we hope SRL can be used to further individualize medical education. We are in an era where artificial intelligence (AI) and machine learning are en vogue. With further development, SRL-based simulated learning can adapt to the strengths and weakness of the learner. Such a learning environment could also adjust the case based on learner goals at the beginning of the SRL session. As the case advances, if the clinical scenario appears to be too simple for the learner, additional information could be given that would add to its complexity. The use of AI and machine learning is a fantastic opportunity to expose learners to the multitude of diagnostic reasoning strategies out there, and set them up for success as life-long learners.

Conclusions

Becoming an expert learner is no easy feat. It is one thing to say that all physicians are life-long learners, but it is entirely another to practice this on a regular basis. Self-regulated learning, combined with deliberate practice, is a competency that we believe is central to this philosophy as it teaches learners “how to learn”. The future of SRL is bright with opportunities for significant growth in simulation, assessment, and machine learning/artificial intelligence applications. We believe that SRL has the potential to help achieve the NASEM goal of improved diagnostic accuracy and reduced diagnostic error. We all can become expert learners. SRL is one effective way to get there.


Corresponding author: Alexander Goldowsky, MD, Division of Gastroenterology and Hepatology, Boston University School of Medicine, Gastroenterology Fellow, 85 E. Concord Street, 7th Floor, Boston, MA 02118, USA, E-mail:

  1. Research funding: None declared.

  2. Author contributions: All authors have accepted responsibility for the entire content of this manuscript and approved its submission.

  3. Competing interests: Joseph Rencic is a consultant for New England Journal of Medicine group Healer, an application that teaches clinical reasoning.

  4. Informed consent: Not applicable.

  5. Ethical approval: Not applicable.

References

1. Committee on Diagnostic Error in Health Care; Board on Health Care Services; Institute of Medicine; The National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. Balogh, EP, Miller, BT, Ball, JR, editors. Improving diagnosis in health care. Washington, DC, US: National Academies Press; 2015.Search in Google Scholar

2. Elstein, AS, Shulman, LS, Sprafka, SA. Medical problem solving: an analysis of clinical reasoning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1978.10.4159/harvard.9780674189089Search in Google Scholar

3. Olson, A, Rencic, J, Cosby, K, Rusz, D, Papa, F, Croskerry, P, et al.. Competencies for improving diagnosis: an interprofessional framework for education and training in health care. Diagnosis 2019;6:335–41. https://doi.org/10.1515/dx-2018-0107.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

4. Kitsantas, A, Zimmerman, BJ. Comparing self-regulatory processes among novice, non-expert, and expert volleyball players: a microanalytic study. J Appl Sport Psychol 2002;14:91–105. https://doi.org/10.1080/10413200252907761.Search in Google Scholar

5. Kleinman, E, Gayle, C, El-Nasr, MS. “Because I’m bad at the game!” A microanalytic study of self regulated learning in league of legends. Front Psychol 2021;12:780234.10.3389/fpsyg.2021.780234Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

6. Boekaerts, M, Pintrich, PR, Zeidner, M. Handbook of self-regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press; 2000.10.1016/B978-012109890-2/50030-5Search in Google Scholar

7. Ericsson, KA, Krampe, RT, Tesch-Römer, C. The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychol Rev 1993;100:363–406. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.100.3.363.Search in Google Scholar

8. Duvivier, RJ, van Dalen, J, Muijtjens, AM, Moulaert, VR, van der Vleuten, CP, Scherpbier, AJ. The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of clinical skills. BMC Med Educ 2011;11:101.10.1186/1472-6920-11-101Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

9. Issenberg, SB, McGaghie, WC, Petrusa, ER, Gordon, DL, Scalese, RJ. Features and uses of high-fidelity medical simulations that lead to effective learning: a BEME systematic review. Med Teach 2005;27:10–28. https://doi.org/10.1080/01421590500046924.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

10. Barsuk, JH, Ahya, SN, Cohen, ER, McGaghie, WC, Wayne, DB. Mastery learning of temporary hemodialysis catheter insertion by nephrology fellows using simulation technology and deliberate practice. Am J Kidney Dis 2009;54:70–6. https://doi.org/10.1053/j.ajkd.2008.12.041.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

11. Gandomkar, R, Mirzazadeh, A, Jalili, M, Yazdani, K, Fata, L, Sandars, J. Self-regulated learning processes of medical students during an academic learning task. Med Educ 2016;50:1065–74. https://doi.org/10.1111/medu.12975.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

12. Song, HS, Kalet, AL, Plass, JL. Assessing medical students’ self-regulation as aptitude in computer-based learning. Adv Health Sci Educ 2011;16:97–107. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-010-9248-1.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

13. Ariel, R, Karpicke, JD. Improving self-regulated learning with a retrieval practice intervention. J Exp Psychol Appl 2018;24:43–56. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000133.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

14. Brydges, R, Butler, D. A reflective analysis of medical education research on self-regulation in learning and practice. Med Educ 2012;46:71–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2923.2011.04100.x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

15. Tanner, KD. Promoting student metacognition. CBE-Life Sci Educ 2012;11:113–20. https://doi.org/10.1187/cbe.12-03-0033.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

16. Turan, S, Konan, A. Self-regulated learning strategies used in surgical clerkship and the relationship with clinical achievement. J Surg Educ 2012;69:218–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsurg.2011.09.003.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

17. Demirören, M, Turan, S, Öztuna, D. Medical students’ self-efficacy in problem-based learning and its relationship with self-regulated learning. Med Educ Online 2016;21:30049.10.3402/meo.v21.30049Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

18. Cho, KK, Marjadi, B, Langendyk, V, Hu, W. Medical student changes in self-regulated learning during the transition to the clinical environment. BMC Med Educ 2017;17:59. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-017-0902-7.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

19. Kuiper, R, Pesut, D, Kautz, D. Promoting the self-regulation of clinical reasoning skills in nursing students. Open J Nurs 2009;3:76–85. https://doi.org/10.2174/1874434600903010076.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

20. Sagasser, MH, Kramer, AWM, van der Vleuten, CPM. How do postgraduate GP trainees regulate their learning and what helps and hinders them? A qualitative study. BMC Med Educ 2012;12:67. https://doi.org/10.1186/1472-6920-12-67.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

21. Kruger, J, Dunning, D. Unskilled and unaware of it: how difficulties in recognizing one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. J Pers Soc Psychol 1999;77:1121–34. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1121.Search in Google Scholar

22. Kornell, N, Bjork, R. Optimising self-regulated study: the benefits-and costs-of dropping flashcards. Memory 2008;16:125–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210701763899.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

23. Richardson, M, Abraham, C, Bond, R. Psychological correlates of university students’ academic performance: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 2012;138:353–87. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026838.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

24. Sitzmann, T, Ely, K. A meta-analysis of self-regulated learning in work-related training and educational attainment: what we know and where we need to go. Psychol Bull 2011;137:421–42. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022777.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

25. Dignath, C, Buettner, G, Langfeldt, HP. How can primary school students learn self-regulated learning strategies most effectively? A meta-analysis on self-regulation training programmes. Educ Res Rev 2008;3:101–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2008.02.003.Search in Google Scholar

26. Dignath, C, Büttner, G. Components of fostering self-regulated learning among students. A meta-analysis on intervention studies at primary and secondary school level. Metacogn Learn 2008;3:231–64. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-008-9029-x.Search in Google Scholar

27. Mamede, S, Schmidt, HG, Rikers, RM, Custers, EJ, Splinter, TA, van Saase, JL. Conscious thought beats deliberation without attention in diagnostic decision-making: at least when you are an expert. Psychol Res 2010;74:586–92. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-010-0281-8.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

28. Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. New York, NY: W. H. Freeman and Company; 1997.Search in Google Scholar

29. Prasad, V, Gall, V, Cifu, A. The frequency of medical reversal. Arch Intern Med 2011;171:1675–6. https://doi.org/10.1001/archinternmed.2011.295.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

30. Theobald, M. Self-regulated learning training programs enhance university students’ academic performance, self-regulated learning strategies, and motivation: a meta-analysis. Contemp Educ Psychol 2021;66:101976.10.1016/j.cedpsych.2021.101976Search in Google Scholar

31. Ejubović, A, Puška, A. Impact of self-regulated learning on academic performance and satisfaction of students in the online environment. Knowl Manag E-Learn 2019;11:345–63.10.34105/j.kmel.2019.11.018Search in Google Scholar

32. Demaria, AN. Medicine, aviation, and simulation. J Am Coll Cardiol 2011;57:1328–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jacc.2011.02.007.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

33. Haerling, KA. Cost-utility analysis of virtual and mannequin-based simulation. Simulat Healthc J Soc Med Simulat 2018;13:33–40. https://doi.org/10.1097/sih.0000000000000280.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

34. Yazdani, S, Hosseinzadeh, M, Hosseini, F. Models of clinical reasoning with a focus on general practice: a critical review. J Adv Med Educ Prof 2017;5:177–84.Search in Google Scholar

35. Pintrich, PR. A conceptual framework for assessing motivation and self-regulated learning in college students. Educ Psychol Rev 2004;16:385–407. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-004-0006-x.Search in Google Scholar

36. @CPSolvers. Friends, it’s time for another case, Tweetorial style. #Medtwitter #FOAMed #Clinicalreasoning; 2019.Search in Google Scholar

37. van Merriënboer, JJG, Sweller, J. Cognitive load theory in health professional education: design principles and strategies. Med Educ 2010;44:85–93. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2923.2009.03498.x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

38. Healer. New England journal of medicine; 2022. Available from: https://healer.nejm.org [Accessed 20 Aug 2022].Search in Google Scholar

39. Berkhout, JJ, Helmich, E, Teunissen, PW, van der Vleuten, CP, Jaarsma, AD. How clinical medical students perceive others to influence their self-regulated learning. Med Educ 2017;51:269–79. https://doi.org/10.1111/medu.13131.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

40. Elstein, AS, Shulman, LS, Sprafka, SA. Medical problem solving: an analysis of clinical reasoning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1978.10.4159/harvard.9780674189089Search in Google Scholar

41. Norman, GR, Tugwell, P, Feightner, JW, Muzzin, LJ, Jacoby, LL. Knowledge and clinical problem-solving. Med Educ 1985;19:344–56. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2923.1985.tb01336.x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

42. Eva, KW. On the generality of specificity. Med Educ 2003;37:587–8. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2923.2003.01563.x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

43. Konopasky, A, Durning, SJ, Battista, Artino, AR, Ramani, D, Haynes, ZA, et al.. Challenges in mitigating context specificity in clinical reasoning: a report and reflection. Diagnosis 2020;7:291–7. https://doi.org/10.1515/dx-2020-0018.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

44. Cleary, TJ, Durning, SJ, Artino, AR. Microanalytic assessment of self-regulated learning during clinical reasoning tasks: recent developments and next steps. Acad Med 2016;91:1516–21. https://doi.org/10.1097/acm.0000000000001228.Search in Google Scholar

45. Hatala, R, Gutman, J, Lineberry, M, Triola, M, Pusic, M. How well is each learner learning? Validity investigation of a learning curve-based assessment approach for ECG interpretation. Adv Health Sci Educ 2019;24:45–63. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-018-9846-x.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

46. Reinstein, I, Hill, J, Cook, DA, Lineberry, M, Pusic, M. Multi-level longitudinal learning curve regression models integrated with item difficulty metrics for deliberate practice of visual diagnosis: groundwork for adaptive learning. Adv Health Sci Educ 2021;26:881–912. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10459-021-10027-0.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

47. Pottle, J. Virtual reality and the transformation of medical education. Future Healthc J 2019;6:181–5. https://doi.org/10.7861/fhj.2019-0036.Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

Received: 2022-06-20
Accepted: 2022-11-08
Published Online: 2022-12-08

© 2022 Walter de Gruyter GmbH, Berlin/Boston

Downloaded on 11.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/dx-2022-0066/html
Scroll to top button