Home Trustworthiness: Public reactions to COVID-19 crisis communication
Article Open Access

Trustworthiness: Public reactions to COVID-19 crisis communication

  • Eli Skogerbø ORCID logo EMAIL logo , Øyvind Ihlen , Jens Elmelund Kjeldsen and Anja Vranic
Published/Copyright: September 28, 2024
Communications
From the journal Communications

Abstract

In an unprecedented situation of uncertainty, the COVID-19 pandemic tested the public crisis communication capacity. Using focus group data, this study analyzes public reactions to COVID-19 policies in Scandinavia. In line with the “rally around the flag” hypothesis, trust in public health authorities remained high in all three Scandinavian countries throughout the COVID-19 pandemic. We asked how people assessed the trustworthiness of authorities, and how they discussed reasons for complying with regulations and recommendations. Our findings suggest that the trustworthiness of experts and leaders was continually negotiated, manufactured, and renegotiated, producing critical and conditional trust. Willingness to and reasons for complying with measures to curb the disease were expressed and justified by participants from Sweden, Norway, and Denmark, notwithstanding the national policies consisting of harsh regulations and mild pressure.

1 Introduction

In the wake of the global COVID-19 pandemic, questions about how citizens assessed the trustworthiness of their governments’ information and regulations have become essential (Offerdal et al., 2021). In the Scandinavian countries, known as “high-trust societies” long before the pandemic (see e.g., Andreasson, 2017), a majority declared that they were satisfied with the response of their governments, and expressed high levels of trust in the health authorities during the first stages of the pandemic (Nielsen and Lindvall, 2021). This was the case in all three Scandinavian countries—Denmark, Norway, and Sweden—even though the respective governments in the early stage of the COVID 19-crisis, Spring 2020, chose different strategies to curb the pandemic. Whereas the governments in Norway and Denmark implemented lockdowns and strict regulations on citizens’ movements, Sweden relied on recommendations and remained open (Nord, 2022).

To contextualize the study, the Scandinavian countries—Denmark, Norway, and Sweden—are small, stable, welfare states that were led by minority coalition governments in 2020: Social Democratic Prime Minister (PM) Mette Frederiksen in Denmark, Conservative PM Erna Solberg in Norway, and Social Democrat PM Stefan Löfven in Sweden. The Danish Cabinet and PM Mette Frederiksen fronted the crisis management through TV speeches, press conferences, and decision-making processes. A similar role was taken by the Norwegian government where the PM and the Minister of Health led the crisis management, advised and aided by expert institutions that did not always agree (Christensen and Laegreid, 2020; NOU 2021, p. 6). While Denmark and Norway implemented strict regulatory measures such as lockdowns and closing of schools and kindergartens from mid-March to April 2020, Sweden used an advisory strategy relying on voluntary compliance (Anderberg, 2021; Claeson and Hanson, 2021). In Sweden, the pandemic crisis management was—by law—delegated from the Cabinet to the Public Health Agency in Sweden (PHAS), and accordingly, the expert institution had a more visible role than the PM (Andersson and Aylott, 2020; Pierre, 2020). The different strategies led to radically different numbers of deaths and infections in Sweden compared to its Nordic neighbours in the first two years of the pandemic. As of February 21, 2022, the death tolls related to COVID-19 were approximately 1,656 per million in Sweden (16,897 deaths, population 10.2 million), compared to 669 per million in Denmark (3,884 deaths, population of 5.8 million), and 281 per million in Norway (1,549 deaths, population of 5.5 million)[1]. Despite national differences, trust in the national political and health authorities remained remarkably high and stable in all three countries (Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development, 2022).

Recent research has pointed to the “rally around the flag” hypothesis as an explanation of the tendency of citizens to support their governments in times of crisis (Esaiasson et al., 2021; Schraff, 2021). Such effects were far from being global phenomena during the COVID-19 pandemic (Kritzinger et al., 2021; Van Aelst and Blumler, 2021); however, in Scandinavia, there is substantial quantitative evidence that governments could rely on their citizens to comply with measures to curb the virus (Johansson et al., 2023). Likewise, survey studies show that not only health authorities, but also nationwide news media gained from rallying effects around the national media institutions (Knudsen et al., 2023). We take these quantitative findings as a starting point for this qualitative study that investigates how members of the public assess and negotiate the trustworthiness of health authorities. We seek to unpack why members of the public decide to trust and comply with the crisis measures imposed by authorities. By doing this, our study contributes to understanding the reasons why citizens “rally around the flag.” Building on insights of theories and studies of risk communication, rhetoric, and authority performances, we have formulated the research question as follows:

RQ1: How did members of the public assess the trustworthiness of health authorities?

RQ2: How did members of the public discuss and negotiate reasons for complying (or not) with policies and measures?

Below, we discuss the theoretical perspectives and previous studies we draw on, followed by a methods section that presents our data material and the methods we have used to analyze the focus group data.

2 Trustworthiness and trust in public health authorities

In times of public health crises, such as the COVID-19 pandemic which forms the context of this study, securing the public’s trust in health authorities is considered essential for securing compliance with strict measures to curb the infection (Liu and Mehta, 2021; Majid et al., 2021; Johansson et al., 2023). Yet, in the literature, there are many definitions of trust. Sztompka (1999, p. 25) defines trust as “a bet about the future contingent actions of others.” Trust has also been seen as based on shared moral values with others resulting in the belief that the others will not take advantage of us (Uslaner, 2002). Rational choice theory sees trust as a form of encapsulated interest —“the potentially trusted person has an interest in maintaining a relationship with the trustor, an interest that gives the potentially trusted person an incentive to be trustworthy” (Hardin, 2006, p. 17). Here, we rely on a definition of trust from organizational research: “the willingness of a party to be vulnerable to the actions of another party based on the expectation that the other will perform a particular action important to the trustor, irrespective of the ability to monitor or control that other party” (Mayer et al., 1995, pp. 712). Organizational research commonly points to two antecedents of trust: trusting dispositions, and trustworthiness (Baer and Colquitt, 2018; Mayer et al., 1995; Rousseau et al., 1998). The former is a stable tendency to ascribe good intentions to other people, and, thus, a reliance of the trustor on what is said and done by others. It also implies how such dispositions might help explain trust in many situations, but also that trust might differ related to different topics and situations (Baer and Colquitt, 2018). Organizational scholars frequently discuss trustworthiness as a construct with the following three elements (Haynes et al., 2012; Mayer et al., 1995): ability (the trustor believes that the trustee has the needed knowledge, skills, expertise, and competence to perform x’ in a specific domain or to tackle some specific tasks) (van Dijck and Alinejad, 2020); integrity (the trustor believes that the trustee adheres to a set of values shared or accepted by the trustor, for instance, that there will be consistency between word and deed); and benevolence (the trustor believes that the trustee cares for the wellbeing of the trustor for their own sake, rather than the self-interest of the trustee). Baer and Colquitt (2018, p. 170) concluded that not only are these facets strongly correlated to trust, but trust is also “primarily and essentially a function of perceived trustworthiness.” The three elements found to increase trustworthiness are similar to the Aristotelian rhetorical tradition of seeing competence (ability), character (integrity), and goodwill (benevolence) as ways to strengthening ethos (Aristotle, ca. 340 B. C. E./2007; Fiskvik et al., 2023).

Previous studies of health communication during COVID-19 have shown that all three elements have been described as important. Van Dijck and Alinejad (2020, p. 2) singled out ability as crucial for the perceived trustworthiness of experts, whereas other studies have pointed to integrity, drawing attention to the perceived difference in trustworthiness between experts and politicians (Hendriks et al., 2023; Janssen et al., 2021). Public health authorities are often perceived as more trustworthy, presumably lacking a political agenda (Quinn et al., 2013). Another recent study pointed to how factors such as personal contact with experts, as well as the perceived independence from political elites, might increase perceptions of trustworthiness (Mihelj et al., 2022).

Walls et al. (2004) employed focus group data to develop the concept of critical trust, portraying trust as “provisionally conceptualized as multifaceted, potentially dynamic, and dependent upon a range of contextual variables” (p. 135). Their findings suggested that “people employ a range of rationales when assessing government” (p. 146), negotiating scepticism and reliance when they assessed the trustworthiness of public institutions for health and risk management. Many contextual factors played into the evaluations, and only rarely did the authors note expressions of naïve or blind trust, or the opposite, outright distrust among the participants (Walls et al., 2004).

Most citizens will gain information about political issues from different news and social media, and during the pandemic, when access to most social settings outside of the family were non-existent or limited, news media became overly important both as sources of information and manufacturers of trust (Knudsen et al., 2023). Koivunen and Vuorelma (2022, p. 394), offering a nuanced view of how trustworthiness is constructed in mediatized and networked contexts, argue that “instead of approaching trust through indexes that examine the level of citizens’ trust towards institutions, we treat it as a key element in authority performances.” The point in their reasoning is that the manufacturing of trust requires authorities to act and respond, but also to pay attention to the mediatized manufacturing of trust: “The media does not only report on and analyze authority performances of political actors but also themselves enact authority performances, which include manufacturing trust” (Koivunen and Vuorelma, 2022, p. 395). Authority performances were abundant in all phases of the COVID-19 pandemic in the Scandinavian countries, regularly enacted through governments’ press conferences and news reporting about crisis management. In a related but not similar vein, Bjørkdal and collegues (2021) argue that the pandemic could be seen as a set of rhetorical situations—a question inviting a response—and analyze the responses from the heads of state, the Danish, Norwegian and Swedish Prime Ministers (PMs) in the first stages of the crisis when they communicated to the people in daily press conferences broadcast in radio, TV, and online. The perspectives outlined above will be applied in the analysis of the data in the sections below.

3 Method and data

We used focus group interviews to research how group participants negotiated these dimensions and highlight the contingent character of trustworthiness, contributing to a nuanced in-depth understanding of how trustworthiness is situational (Delia, 1976; O’Keefe, 2016). Unlike survey data, our data does not yield statistically generalizable data, yet we aim for conceptual generalizability by employing an inductive, reflexive analytical approach (Polit and Beck, 2010; Tjora, 2019).

To collect data, we procured the services of the market analysis company Opinion to organize 12 focus groups across Scandinavia, four in each country, in May 2020. Opinion operates across the Scandinavian countries, and the company recruited participants from their pre-existing database according to the specifications of the project. Participants were rewarded with a gift card (DKK/NOK/SEK 500, approximately €50). Originally planned as physical focus groups, owing to the scale of the pandemic, they were turned into virtual groups and the conversations were online live chats. The chats were synchronous in text format with no visual communication.

Each group consisted of six to nine participants from four different age groups: Young without children (18–30); Families with small children (30–45); “Empty nesters,” i. e., persons without small children in the household (46–65); and “Seniors” aged 65 or older. The number of participants per group was set to secure that each group would allow for a certain diversity of opinions and beliefs, and, at the same time, allow enough time and space for all participants to actively take part in the conversation. Within the groups, there were variations between participants concerning jobs, state of employment, education, class, and cultural background. The selection of participants also considered that people had experienced the lockdowns and other measures differently, varying with life and job situations. For example, students were locked out from studies and part-time jobs and reported experiencing economic insecurity, whereas health and care workers had to work in high risk-environments.

Each live chat lasted approximately two hours and was led by professional moderators, one in each country, speaking Danish, Norwegian, and Swedish respectively. They opened the sessions by inviting the participants to present themselves and talk about how they initially heard about a global pandemic and how they had reacted to this information, explicitly noting that participants should not share confidential information leaving them vulnerable to harm. In the transcribed chats that were submitted to the research team, the participants were anonymized, except for first names, to fulfil research ethics requirements concerning privacy and anonymity.

The interviews were guided by theory-based and detailed interview guides, translated, and adapted to the different national contexts. The common parts related to questions guiding the participants’ experiences, whereas the interview guides used specific Danish, Norwegian, and Swedish examples when asking about political leaders and governments, public health institutions, media reporting and measures for curbing the pandemic. Addressing aspects of trust and trustworthiness, the questions probed topics such as experiences of the pandemic, views on the public health information and crisis communication of political leaders and public health agencies, and experiences and views on the measures to combat the pandemic. They were asked about their personal experiences with the restrictions and recommendations set out by the health authorities to curb the pandemic, whether they found the measures reasonable, and whether and why they did or did not trust the representatives of the health authorities. The interview guides were designed to entice views, discussions, and negotiations on the credibility and efficiency of measures and the participants’ compliance—or lack thereof. In particular, the participants were invited to discuss and reason about how and why they assessed these actors as competent, displaying moral character, or showing goodwill. The moderators encouraged and asked participants to engage and discuss with each other and give reasons for their views.

The participants were also presented with one or two news stories and opinion pieces from major news media in each country: Dagbladet (N), Jyllands-Posten (DK), TV2 (DK), Svenska Dagbladet (S), and Aftonbladet (S). The pieces were selected as examples of the current public debates on the measures taken in each of the countries. The stories for Norway discussed criticism generated by the re-opening of schools and kindergartens; the pieces for Denmark illustrated disagreements on the strictness of the measures and sanctions for breaking them; and the items for Sweden showed the intense debate on the Swedish strategy of recommending rather than regulating and sanctioning public behavior. The media items were included to stimulate debate and discussions within the groups. The news stories’ veracity and the relevance of the opinion pieces were checked and discussed within the cross-national research team prior to being included as examples in the interview guides. The interview guide and stimuli are available on request.

The transcripts of the online chats were coded qualitatively in NVivo using a stepwise inductive-deductive approach (SDA; Tjora, 2019) involving many rounds of reading and analysis. Using an inductive-deductive approach means that the codes were initially inductively drawn from the transcripts of the chats in the focus groups. SDA has many similarities with a grounded theory approach (e.g., Corbin and Strauss, 2008), but is more deductive when drawing on previous theoretical insights in the analysis.

The discussions in the focus groups were not identical, and revealed different views and concerns related to the trustworthiness of political leaders and public health institutions in general as well as their spokespersons. The different group dynamics sometimes led to some aspects taking precedence over others, reflecting both different demographics, experiences of the participants, and national contexts. In the analysis of the transcripts, we delved into the expressions and negotiations between the focus group participants on the trustworthiness of health authorities concerning how they had experienced the first two months of the pandemic.

4 Results and analysis

We set out with two research questions asking how members of the public assessed the trustworthiness of health authorities (RQ1 and how they discussed and negotiated reasons for trusting and/or complying—or not—with the national COVID-19 measures (RQ2). The interviews and discussions show that the participants assessed and had various reasons for trusting and not trusting public health authorities. Most participants formed their impressions watching and listening to televised press conferences and media reporting; only a few mentioned that they also had access to relevant information through their jobs or family.

When asked a set of questions seeking to understand how participants assessed the trustworthiness and validity of the information and measures issued by health authorities, they replied by referring to several lines of reasoning based on their perception of political leaders and the spokespersons of public health agencies.

Ability

A first statement that was shared by participants was that leadership in crisis showed an ability to act: “I remember that I was very impressed when they locked down on 12 March. That decision commanded respect” (Line). Stine followed up: “They [i. e., health authorities] took it seriously and did what they could to protect us, even tried opening the “wallet.” Politicians and experts were both perceived as showing moral character and being trustworthy. Participants commented on the risks taken just by taking leadership in such difficult situations, as Gaute did: “I must commend Health Minister Høie, the NHD and the NIPH. Nakstad (the spokesperson for NHD) and colleagues managed to get the message through without scaremongering” (all quotes in this paragraph are from Families with small children, Norway, 4 May 2020). The belief that health authorities were trustworthy because they relied on the available expertise, even when it was scarce and contradictory, was repeated across the groups. However, there was no consensus among the participants that all measures were fully justified or correct. Anna F represents a sceptical but shared view across the countries and groups:

When everything started, I thought Tegnell (the then spokesperson for the PHAS) would kill us all. That was unfair of course as he was not alone in the PHAS. Nevertheless, I don’t think they are certain that Sweden’s method for handling the Corona crisis is the best or whether we should have locked down more like other countries did. (Anna F, Empty nesters, Sweden, 5 May 2020)

Participants had different views on how well public health authorities managed the COVID-16 crisis, yet they recognized that the situation was one where there was no secure knowledge, as Anna F did in the quote above. Although, initially, she was critical of the Swedish strategy, that is, not to regulate but recommend, she was open to the thought that it may have been right. We find similar assessments in both Norway and Denmark:

It may be a contradiction to state that I trust the authorities but still am not sure they have chosen the right strategy. On average, eleven people die every day in Norway. Now, it is fewer than the average number. It seems the measures have been too strict. (Svein, Seniors, Norway, 7 May 2020)

Svein’s view that the measures implemented were not always adequate was shared by others in the group. Participants did not think that the strategy chosen by their own government necessarily was the smartest, but they allowed for uncertainty.

Benevolence

Following from the recognition of risk and insecurity, was the widely shared willingness to believe that the health authorities did their best under the circumstances. References to the goodwill of governments and health authorities came up repeatedly in the negotiations on support for the governments’ measures to curb the spread of the pandemic, independent of the nationality of the participants, as Swedish Josefine said:

I do not regard myself as a person who does not trust others but in this crisis, I find it difficult to trust anyone at all. Nevertheless, I will believe that everyone attempts to do their best for all. I do not need to follow a leader or trust a specific authority or person. (Josefine, Young without children, Sweden, 5 May 2020)

In the group of young people in Denmark, in a long discussion on the Swedish as opposed to the Danish strategy, Jacob said: “In a way, I think it’s good that he [Anders Tegnell, PHAS] gives more responsibility to the Swedish people.” Lise added: “I think Anders is a brave man, and only time will show whether Søren [Brostrøm, then CEO of the Danish Health Authority] or Anders were the wiser one” (all quotes from Young without children, Denmark, 5 May 2020). Apart from the somewhat curious fact that young Danes and Swedes were on first name terms with the central public health spokespersons in both Denmark and Sweden, the willingness to allow for uncertainty and mistake, and to negotiate trust and trustworthiness was repeated in several groups. The participants emphasized the uncertainty and risk that underlaid the situation, and were open to an acceptance of strict measures: “My trust has not changed, that is, I do not have high trust but I am convinced that they [the health authorities] do their best and wish the best for all of us” (Malthe, Families with small children, Denmark, 5 May 2020); “It easy to be wise after the event, but I think the decisions made are right given the information they [the authorities] have at this time” (Hanna, Families with small children, Norway, 4 May 2020).

Stine referred to instances in which experts contradicted each other and there were conflicting messages from health authorities: “You must gather information from several places before deciding what to believe in. Make up your own mind, don’t believe blindly because they are experts or political leaders” (Stine, Families with children, Norway 4 May 2020). The willingness of participants to believe in the benevolence of authorities was in other words neither uncritical nor unanimous. There were plenty of critical comments to inadequate information, strategies and measures implemented and not implemented, and worries about deaths and serious illness.

Integrity

The quotes above not only point to participants’ perceptions of the benevolence of health authorities, but also illustrate that they appreciated their accountability, especially concerning uncertainty and admission of mistakes. Lars (Empty nesters, Denmark, 6 May 2020) stated: “It’s fair enough. Luckily, it’s the first time we experience things like this, so it’s fully understandable that they [the health authorities] sometimes also fumble a bit blindly.” The same position was taken by Alexandra (Young without children, Sweden, 5 May 2020): “I don’t think it’s strange. No one knows how to solve this or how it will turn out.”

Furthermore, when asked about why they still trusted health authorities, a repeated reply was that they chose to trust authorities, despite the knowledge that the decisions and strategies might be wrong, or even with experiences that the decisions had bad or unforeseen consequences: “I am confident that the government and the PHAS do their best. Still, I am worried about the virus being spread in the homes for the elderly. That people have died alone and in isolation because of this terrible disease” (Monica, Families with small children, Sweden, 6 May 2020). Participants maintained that they did not blindly trust either authority but that in this situation everything was new and uncertain, and that there was no correct answer. Josefine said: “I think it may be dangerous to trust authorities and even experts blindly” (Josefine, Young without children, Sweden, 5 May 2020). Norwegian Hanna said:

I believe nobody has the correct answer and everyone tries to do their best. I mostly trust those who have nothing to gain from the information they provide. Newspapers and other media profit from the clicks. I choose to trust the government and the experts, such as the Norwegian Institute for Public Health [NIPH]. (Hanna, Families with small children, Norway, 4 May 2020)

Experts know, politicians decide

Having established that participants placed much emphasis on benevolence and integrity, they, in all groups in all three countries, distinguished clearly between the roles of experts and politicians. Some, as Elias and Arvid, expressed that they had full confidence in the PHAS because they were experts (both from Young without children, Sweden, 5 May 2020). Danish Lise pointed out that different authorities did not have the same roles and responsibilities:

Oh, what can you say about the different authorities? They have different objectives. The health experts should provide expertise independent from the economy, whereas the politicians must balance health and economy, which cannot be easy. I do not distrust politicians; it is just that they have different roles. So, I think I trust SSI [Statens Serum Institut] and Søren [Brostrøm, CEO of Danish Health Authority—DHA] when it comes to health, but it is Mette [Frederiksen, the PM] who is responsible for the economy (Lise, Young without children, Denmark, 5 May 2020).

This was a returning theme, and the distinction was drawn regardless of the strategies that the governments were following, as exemplified in this statement from Denmark: “SSI and DHA have the expertise and competence, but the politicians decide. That is good” (Per, Families with small children, Denmark, 5 May 2020). As Per pointed out, differences in power and competence between political leaders and expert agencies were recognized and acknowledged.

The participants talked about both government representatives, such as the PMs and the Ministers of Health, and the experts on health, virology, and epidemics as being equally trustworthy but for different reasons. For instance, this Norwegian participant expressed:

I trust the government, the NDH [Norwegian Directorate of Health) and NIPH. The NIPH and NDH house the people with competence about health, viruses, spread of infection etcetera whereas the government has more to consider. It’s not necessarily easy to follow the advice from NIPH and the NDH because it can have major implications on other areas of society. Who should you sacrifice to save others? Those choices are difficult to make (Ida, Young without children, Norway, 4 May 2020).

Other participants pointed to the integrity of health authorities by emphasizing character traits of politicians and experts as being honest, calm, and positive amid the crisis management. They commented on the PMs as providing good information and being in control of the situation, whereas the expert institutions and agencies, and particularly their spokespersons in each country, were applauded for their ability to communicate clearly and understandably:

… [Deputy Director Espen Nakstad, NDH] is for me the big star … He appears trustworthy in the way he explains the situation. It is evident that he is a very good communicator. It is easy to understand his message (Ingri, Seniors, Norway, 7 May 2020).

I follow Tegnell [of PHAS]. There is a reason why he has his job, and he has been at work at this type of job for ages. He is pedagogical, he makes people listen due to his knowledge and calmness (Joel, Families with small children, Sweden, 6 May 2020).

The spokespersons for the agencies were commended because they did not use “war headlines” but made people listen because of their expertise, as Anne pointed out: “NIPH and NDH. They are bureaucrats without an agenda” (Families with children, Norway, 4 May 2020).

Balancing the costs of preventing the spread of the virus, loss of lives and burden on health care, and the costs of locking down as the governments had to do, were acknowledged across the countries. Kaya expressed her views like this: “I think that human lives have been valued incredibly high. There will be a large bill to pay at the other end. Still, I cannot see how it could be any different” (Families with children, Denmark, 5 May 2020). The willingness to accept that the governments and the experts navigated with limited knowledge and under great risk was shared by the group participants across borders. The public health agencies were regarded as competent on the disease and its consequences, whereas politicians should find the balance between saving lives and saving the economy.

Summing up findings on RQ1, how members of the public assessed the trustworthiness of health authorities, our study brings out a multilayered picture. The focus group participants expressed both trust and scepticism. When political leaders were assessed as the main authorities there were often lower expectations concerning the competence of health experts. Health experts, on the other hand, were not expected to care for society as such, that would be the responsibility of the politicians. Throughout the material, there were references to the ability and integrity of the experts, and the benevolence of the political leadership, who had to make decisions under extreme conditions of risk and uncertainty. Among the participants, we find a few people who mis- or distrusted the information and/or governments and/or experts as such. The analysis accordingly provides us with some explanations for a perceived lowered trustworthiness of public health authorities. They did not follow national borders, rather they related to unclear communication and what was conceived of as cover-ups or dishonesty. A Danish participant referred to health agencies communicating conflicting information concerning face masks. The same concerns were voiced by Swedish Anna F:

I think, like several of the others [members of the focus group], that the government advocates double standards, for instance in the case of face masks, making it difficult to know who to believe and listen to. It goes in all directions; one day there is no reason for wearing masks and the other day they should be worn (Anna F, Empty nesters, Sweden, 5 May 2020).

In May 2020 there was a global shortage of facemasks, and they were not recommended for the public other than in specific situations in any of the Scandinavian countries. Another and well-known reason for distrust originated in political disagreement with the parties represented by the PMs. Participants pointed to the fact that politicians and parties are dependent on being re-elected and therefore may be less trustworthy than experts.

Compliance

Despite the examples above, participants maintained that they complied with recommendations even when said that they did not trust the authorities to have chosen the best strategies and measures. So, why did they? Anders is but one example of those who link compliance to loyalty to the political system: “That’s why we have democracy, why we elect representatives to govern us through this difficult time. If you don’t agree, you are free to say so, but you must do what is decided. In the next election you can vote them out” (Anders, Families with small children, Norway, 4 May 2020). The same views are found among the Danish and Swedish groups: Even though you may not like the rules—and several also mentioned that they did not support the ruling governments—you should be loyal to the decisions. A Norwegian participant pointed out that the cabinet led by PM Solberg did not have a majority in Parliament, meaning that Parliament could, and did, oversee and if need be, change rules and regulations. Other participants stressed that in a risky, uncertain situation there was no alternative but to trust the authorities.

The interviews show a diversity of reasons and experiences that explained why citizens complied with measures and recommendations. Although some of them distrusted authorities, some disagreed with the national strategies, or with some of the measures (e.g., the cabin ban in Norway), few participants stated that they did not follow recommendations and regulations. They linked compliance both to adherence to system—“otherwise, there will be anarchy,” as a participant argued with a smiley—and to the fear of infecting others and themselves if they did not comply. A discussion in the Swedish group of seniors (6 May 2020) highlights the deliberations:

Ulrika (Moderator): Who made you do this [e. g., comply with recommendations]?

Bjarne: Recommendations from PHAS, and other agencies.

Ann-Kristin: I don’t want to get sick.

Eva: I have a responsibility towards my children, my neighbours, and my family whom I don’t meet. I have the responsibility to keep myself alive! I am 72 now.

Anders: I have minimized social contacts, use no public transport and wash my hands continuously.

Agneta: PHAS and my husband.

This live chat exchange, which sums up the findings related to RQ2, refers to reasons for compliance as diverse as system adherence, self-protection, regard for close family members as well as for the community and society in general, and it all took place in Sweden where no legal measures were implemented at the time.

5 Discussion

In line with other studies, we find that focus group participants in Denmark, Norway, and Sweden in May 2020 assessed the trustworthiness of public health authorities according to the well-established concepts of ability, goodwill and integrity (Baer and Colquitt, 2018; Haynes et al., 2012). As expected, the ancient insights of how trustworthiness—ethos—is built through exposition of ability, integrity and benevolence go a long way to understand and interpret the replies and negotiations found in the focus group data. All three rhetorical components were pointed to by participants who found health authorities trustworthy. The political leaders, the governments, were commended for their ability to act under extreme and risky circumstances and for listening to and applying the knowledge of the experts, in line with what Hendriks et al. (2023) and Janssen et al. (2021) also found. The public health experts were seen as trustworthy for providing unbiased and independent expertise, as Milhelj et al. (2022) also pointed out, even in situations of uncertainty and scarcity of both knowledge and resources.

As shown above, there was no consensus either within the groups or between them that the measures chosen by their individual governments were the “best” or most “correct” measures for curbing the virus. Participants frequently stated that they did not trust authorities’ abilities, as shown when they expressed doubts about the national strategies, and which one would be the best in the long run. They often doubted that authorities, be they experts or not, had adequate competence to assess the effects of the measures they implemented (Denmark and Norway) or recommended (Sweden). Still, they chose to trust their goodwill and comply with regulations because of the high risks for themselves, their families, communities, and society connected to not doing so, or because of loyalty to the political system. The conversations and negotiations in the focus groups showed that the trustworthiness of authorities was contested, divided, and negotiated, particularly between the expert agencies and the political leaders. The participants distinguished between different types of competencies, the scientific expertise of the public health agencies, and the political competence of governments and ministers. Accordingly, our findings relate closely to the previous study of Walls et al. (2004), who coined the concept of critical trust reflecting that members of the public have a variety of reasons for trusting authorities, and that they weigh and balance different concerns against each other. Focus group participants expressed critical and, we may add, “conditional trust” towards political leaders and health experts. In their own words, they chose to trust authorities despite not believing that they necessarily implemented the best measures. They assessed some individual experts as more trustworthy than others on the basis of how their were perceived in the news and press conferences, echoing Koivunen and Vuorelma’s (2022) point that in the networked society, “authority performances” are integral to the production and maintenance of trust. Studies have analyzed how the press conferences became the main arenas both for informing the public and exercising authority (Young, 2022). The form and content were not similar in the three countries (Bjørkdal et al., 2021), yet this study adds to the understanding of how the public perceived and interpreted how public health authorities exerted leadership and expertise through the press conferences. Authority performances such as the press conferences and media reporting on authorities managing the crisis were situations in which trustworthiness and authority seemingly were produced and reproduced. As found in many previous studies (Cvetkovich and Lofstedt, 1999; Frederiksen, 2014; Gillman et al., 2023), displaying uncertainty and risk about the situation did not seem to weaken the trustworthiness of either experts or governments. Rather, openness and transparency play into building trustworthiness (Ihlen et al., 2022).

Returning to our starting point, the unpacking of the “rally around the flag” hypothesis, that citizens will support their governments in states of crisis, our findings shed light on the reasons why citizens choose to be loyal. As pointed to above, citizens’ trust is both critical (Walls et al., 2004) and conditional. The COVID-19-crisis displayed many national contexts of low trust in public health authorities, e.g. the U. K., where Baker and Lilleker (2022) pointed to scandals and rule-breaking among the elites as reasons for mistrust. Our findings show that the public perceived public authorities as conditionally trustworthy, that is, those who chose to trust authorities did so despite uncertainty. Equally important, system loyalty weighed in as a separate and strong reason for compliance with regulations and recommendations. Even those who did not trust that the authorities had implemented the correct measures, and those not supporting the sitting government, argued in favor of complying with the COVID-19 measures.

Obviously, our findings do not, as they are qualitative, contribute to support or weaken the hypothesis. Rather, they add insights into what reasons may explain support or lack of support and contribute to refining the hypothesis. The rallying hypothesis brings to mind an all too simplified image of citizens blindly supporting their leaders in times of crisis, as Van Aelst and Blumler (2021) already questioned. Our study suggests that rallying effects should also be seen as conditional and linked to system adherence. As stated initially, the Scandinavian countries were high-trust democracies before the pandemic, and this context is likely to be an important explanation for the willingness of most respondents to comply with recommendations and regulations. Citizens expressed few reasons for distrust, but they were neither uncritical nor expressing blind loyalty. They simply seemed, individually and collectively, to agree that in the risky and insecure situation they experienced, they chose to trust the experts for expertise and the political authorities for balancing the choices of protecting and saving lives against the harms of shattered economies and other social problems.

Limitations of the study

The most obvious limitation of this study is the qualitative method and approach. First, the data does not yield statistically generalizable data; our findings about why people trust and comply should be seen as conceptual building blocks in the development of refined hypotheses on why or why not citizens support their governments in times of crises. Furthermore, the data collected for the study was limited to May 2020, and most certainly represents the specific situation existing in that time of the pandemic. A third possible limitation is that, for the sake of cost-effectiveness, the number of focus group interviews was preset, and not open for the addition of more interviews to ensure data saturation. Yet, as the replies from participants both within and across national settings often were repeating and overlapping, we believe that the method has provided us with solid data.

These limitations suggest avenues for further research. There is a need for studies that compare data over time so that we will know more about whether our findings are situational or valid across time and space. Other studies (Bengtsson and Brommesson, 2022; Nielsen and Lindvall, 2021) have shown waning but still comparatively high and stable trust figures in Scandinavia. Further, there are issues that the present study has just touched upon, such as the fact that trust in public institutions based on a perception of government competence, care, and openness may lead people to underestimate risks and thus reduce their belief in the need to take individual action to control the risks (Wong and Jensen, 2020).

6 Conclusion

The focus groups data were collected two months after the three Scandinavian governments declared the COVID-19 pandemic national crises, and the analyses generated two distinct insights. First, although we acknowledge that “rally around the flag” effects will often be present in crisis situations, this study suggests that the explanations for why they arise are not that citizens look for strong leaders or simple answers. Rather, citizens choose to believe in weakly founded and insecure knowledge because the consequences of not doing so imply major risks for both individual citizens and the social order. Second, the trustworthiness of experts and leaders was continually negotiated, manufactured, and renegotiated, producing critical (Walls et al., 2004) and conditional trust. Expressions of trust in spokespersons and leaders were not distinguished by national borders but by the credibility of messages and the authority performances of the leaders. The data from three countries tell a story of similarities between citizens of states that selected—at this stage—quite different strategies. Willingness to and reasons for complying with measures to curb the disease were expressed and justified by participants from Sweden, Norway, and Denmark, notwithstanding the national policies consisting of harsh regulations or mild pressure.

References

Anderberg, J. (2021). Flocken: Berättelsen om hur Sverige valde väg under pandemin [The flock: The story about how Sweden chose its path during the pandemic]. Albert Bonniers förlag.Search in Google Scholar

Andersson, S., & Aylott, N. (2020). Sweden and coronavirus: Unexceptional exceptionalism. Social Sciences, 9(12), 232. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci912023210.3390/socsci9120232Search in Google Scholar

Andreasson, U. (2017). Trust – the Nordic Gold. N. C. o. Ministers.10.6027/ANP2017-737Search in Google Scholar

Aristotle. (2007). On rhetoric: A theory of civic discourse (G. A. Kennedy, Trans.; Vol. 26). Oxford University Press. Original work published ca. 340 B. C. E.10.1080/07350190701419913Search in Google Scholar

Baer, M. D., & Colquitt, J., A. (2018). The Routledge companion to trust. Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Baker, T. A., & Lilleker, D. (2022). “Not one rule for everyone”: The impact of elite rule-breaking on public trust in the UK. In P. J. Maarek (Ed.), Manufacturing government communication on Covid-19: A comparative perspective (pp. 301–317). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-09230-5_1510.1007/978-3-031-09230-5_15Search in Google Scholar

Bengtsson, R., & Brommesson, D. (2022). Institutional trust and emergency preparedness: Perceptions of Covid 19 crisis management in Sweden. Journal of Contingencies and Crisis Management, 30(4), 481–491. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-5973.1239110.1111/1468-5973.12391Search in Google Scholar

Bjørkdahl, K., Kjeldsen, J. E., Villadsen, L. & Vigsø, O. (2021). Argumentum ad solidaritatem: Rhetorical Leadership Strategies in Scandinavia During COVID-19. In: Lewis, M., Govender, E. & Holland, K. (eds) Communicating COVID-19. Palgrave Macmillan. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-79735-5_910.1007/978-3-030-79735-5_9Search in Google Scholar

Christensen, T. & Lægreid, P. (2020). Balancing Governance Capacity and Legitimacy: How the Norwegian Government Handled the COVID-19 Crisis as a High Performer. Public Admin Rev, 80(5) 774–779. https://doi.org/10.1111/puar.1324110.1111/puar.13241Search in Google Scholar

Claeson, M., & Hanson, S. (2021). COVID-19 and the Swedish enigma. The Lancet, 397(10271), 259–261. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(20)32750-110.1016/S0140-6736(20)32750-1Search in Google Scholar

Corbin, J. M., & Strauss, A. L. (2008). Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and procedures for developing grounded theory. Sage.10.4135/9781452230153Search in Google Scholar

Cvetkovich, G., & Lofstedt, R. E. (Eds.). (1999). Social trust and the management of risk. Routledge.Search in Google Scholar

Delia, J. G. (1976). A constructivist analysis of the concept of credibility. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 62(4), 361–375. https://doi.org/10.1080/00335637609383350.10.1080/00335637609383350Search in Google Scholar

Esaiasson, P., Sohlberg, J., Ghersetti, M., & Johansson, B. (2021). How the coronavirus crisis affects citizen trust in institutions and in unknown others: Evidence from ‘the Swedish experiment’. European Journal of Political Research, 60(3), 748–760. https://doi.org/10.1111/1475-6765.1241910.1111/1475-6765.12419Search in Google Scholar

Fiskvik, J. T., Bjarkø, A. V., & Ihlen, Ø. (2023). Trustworthiness Over Time on Twitter: Three Critical Periods for the Norwegian Health Authorities and Political Leadership During the COVID-19 Pandemic. Social Media + Society, 9(2). https://doi.org/10.1177/2056305123117968910.1177/20563051231179689Search in Google Scholar

Frederiksen, M. (2014). Trust in the face of uncertainty: A qualitative study of intersubjective trust and risk. International Review of Sociology, 24(1), 130–144. https://doi.org/10.1080/03906701.2014.89433510.1080/03906701.2014.894335Search in Google Scholar

Gillman, A. S., Scharnetzki, L., Boyd, P., Ferrer, R. A., Klein, W. M. P., & Han, P. K. J. (2023). Perceptions and tolerance of uncertainty: Relationship to trust in COVID-19 health information and vaccine hesitancy. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 46(1), 40–53. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-022-00302-910.1007/s10865-022-00302-9Search in Google Scholar

Hardin, R. (2006). Trust. Polity.Search in Google Scholar

Haynes, A. S., Derrick, G. E., Redman, S., Hall, W. D., Gillespie, J. A., Chapman, S., & Sturk, H. (2012). Identifying trustworthy experts: How do policymakers find and assess public health researchers worth consulting or collaborating with? PLOS ONE, 7(3), e32665. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.003266510.1371/journal.pone.0032665Search in Google Scholar

Hendriks, F., Janssen, I., & Jucks, R. (2023). Balance as credibility? How presenting one- vs. two-sided messages affects ratings of scientists’ and politicians’ trustworthiness. Health Communication, 38(12), 2757–2764. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2022.211163810.1080/10410236.2022.2111638Search in Google Scholar

Ihlen, Ø., Just, S. N., Kjeldsen, J. E., Mølster, R., Offerdal, T. S., Rasmussen, J., & Skogerbø, E. (2022). Transparency beyond information disclosure: strategies of the Scandinavian public health authorities during the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Risk Research, 25(10), 1176–1189. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2022.207741610.1080/13669877.2022.2077416Search in Google Scholar

Janssen, I., Hendriks, F., & Jucks, R. (2021). Face masks might protect you from COVID-19: The communication of scientific uncertainty by scientists versus politicians in the context of policy in the making. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 40(5–6), 602–626. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X21104451210.1177/0261927X211044512Search in Google Scholar

Johansson, B., Sohlberg, J., & Esaiasson, P. (2023). Institutional trust and crisis management in high-trust societies: Rallies around the Nordic flags during the Covid-19 pandemic. In Ihlen, Ø., Johansson, B., Lindholm, J. & Blach-Ørsten, M. (Eds.), Communicating a pandemic: Crisis management and Covid-19 in the Nordic countries (pp. 285–301). Nordicom. https://doi.org/10.48335/9789188855688-13Search in Google Scholar

Johansson, B., Ihlen, Ø., Lindholm, J., & Blach-Ørsten, M. (2023). Introduction: Communicating a pandemic in the Nordic countries. In In Ihlen, Ø., Johansson, B., Lindholm, J. & Blach-Ørsten; M. (Eds.), Communicating a pandemic: Crisis management and Covid-19 in the Nordic countries (pp. 11–30). Nordicom. https://doi.org/10.48335/9789188855688-1Search in Google Scholar

Knudsen, E., Nordø, Å. D., & Iversen, M. H. (2023). How rally-round-the-flag effects shape trust in the news media: Evidence from panel waves before and during the covid-19 pandemic crisis. Political Communication, 40(2), 201–221.10.1080/10584609.2023.2168322Search in Google Scholar

Koivunen, A., & Vuorelma, J. (2022). Trust and authority in the age of mediatised politics. European Journal of Communication, 02673231211072653. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267323121107265310.1177/02673231211072653Search in Google Scholar

Kritzinger, S., Foucault, M., Lachat, R., Partheymüller, J., Plescia, C., & Brouard, S. (2021). ‘Rally round the flag’: The COVID-19 crisis and trust in the national government. West European Politics, 44(5–6), 1205–1231. https://doi.org/10.1080/01402382.2021.192501710.1080/01402382.2021.1925017Search in Google Scholar

Liu, B. F., & Mehta, A. M. (2021). From the periphery and toward a centralized model for trust in government risk and disaster communication. Journal of Risk Research, 24(7), 853–869. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2020.177351610.1080/13669877.2020.1773516Search in Google Scholar

Majid, U., Wasim, A., Truong, J., & Bakshi, S. (2021). Public trust in governments, health care providers, and the media during pandemics: A systematic review. Journal of Trust Research, 11(2), 119–141. https://doi.org/10.1080/21515581.2022.202974210.1080/21515581.2022.2029742Search in Google Scholar

Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., & Schoorman, F. D. (1995). An integrative model of organizational trust. The Academy of Management Review, 20(3), 709–734. https://doi.org/10.2307/25879210.5465/amr.1995.9508080335Search in Google Scholar

Mihelj, S., Kondor, K., & Štětka, V. (2022). Establishing trust in experts during a crisis: Expert trustworthiness and media use during the COVID-19 pandemic. Science Communication, 44(3), 292–319. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547022110055810.1177/10755470221100558Search in Google Scholar

Nielsen, J. H., & Lindvall, J. (2021). Trust in government in Sweden and Denmark during the COVID-19 epidemic. West European politics, 44(5–6), 1180–1204. https://doi.org/10.1080/01402382.2021.190996410.1080/01402382.2021.1909964Search in Google Scholar

Nord, L. (2022). No lockdown please, we are Swedish: How the middle way country became an extreme case of government communication. In P. J. Maarek (Ed.), Manufacturing government communication on Covid-19: A comparative perspective (pp. 107–121). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-09230-5_610.1007/978-3-031-09230-5_6Search in Google Scholar

NOU 2021:6 Myndighetenes håndtering av koronapandemien [The Norwegian authorities’ handling of the COVID-19 pandemic]. Statsministerens kontor. [Office of the Prime Minister]. https://www.regjeringen.no/no/dokumenter/nou-2021-6/id2844388/Search in Google Scholar

Offerdal, T. S., Just, S. N., & Ihlen, O. (2021). Public ethos in the pandemic rhetorical situation: Strategies for building trust in authorities’ risk communication. Journal of International Crisis and Risk Communication Research, 4(2), 247–270. https://search.informit.org/doi/10.3316/INFORMIT.09904720764490910.30658/jicrcr.4.2.3Search in Google Scholar

O’Keefe, D. J. (2016). Persuasion: Theory and research (3rd ed.). Sage.Search in Google Scholar

Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development. (2022). Building trust to reinforce democracy: Main findings from the 2021 OECD survey on drivers of trust in public institutions. OECD Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1787/b407f99c-en10.1787/b407f99c-enSearch in Google Scholar

Pierre, J. (2020). Nudges against pandemics: Sweden’s COVID-19 containment strategy in perspective. Policy and Society, 39(3), 478–493. https://doi.org/10.1080/14494035.2020.178378710.1080/14494035.2020.1783787Search in Google Scholar

Polit, D. F., & Beck, C. T. (2010). Generalization in quantitative and qualitative research: Myths and strategies. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 47(11), 1451–1458. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2010.06.00410.1016/j.ijnurstu.2010.06.004Search in Google Scholar

Quinn, S. C., Parmer, J., Freimuth, V. S., Hilyard, K. M., Musa, D., & Kim, K. H. (2013). Exploring communication, trust in government, and vaccination intention later in the 2009 H1N1 pandemic: Results of a national survey. Biosecurity and Bioterrorism: Biodefense Strategy, Practice, and Science, 11(2), 96–106. https://doi.org/10.1089/bsp.2012.004810.1089/bsp.2012.0048Search in Google Scholar

Rousseau, D. M., Sitkin, S. B., Burt, R. S., & Camerer, C. (1998). Not so different after all: A cross-discipline view of trust. Academy of Management Review, 23(3), 393–404. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.1998.92661710.5465/amr.1998.926617Search in Google Scholar

Schraff, D. (2021). Political trust during the Covid-19 pandemic: Rally around the flag or lockdown effects? European Journal of Political Research, 60(4), 1007–1017. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/1475-6765.1242510.1111/1475-6765.12425Search in Google Scholar

Sztompka, P. (1999). Trust: A sociological theory. Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Tjora, A. H. (2019). Qualitative research as stepwise-deductive induction. Routledge.10.4324/9780203730072Search in Google Scholar

Uslaner, E. M. (2002). The moral foundations of trust. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO978051161493410.1017/CBO9780511614934Search in Google Scholar

Van Aelst, P., & Blumler, J. G. (Eds.). (2021). Political communication in the time of coronavirus. Routledge.10.4324/9781003170051Search in Google Scholar

van Dijck, J., & Alinejad, D. (2020). Social media and trust in scientific expertise: Debating the Covid-19 pandemic in the Netherlands. Social Media + Society, 6(4), 2056305120981057. https://doi.org/10.1177/205630512098105710.1177/2056305120981057Search in Google Scholar

Walls, J., Pidgeon, N., Weyman, A., & Horlick-Jones, T. (2004). Critical trust: Understanding lay perceptions of health and safety risk regulation. Health, Risk & Society, 6(2), 133–150. https://doi.org/10.1080/136985704200021978810.1080/1369857042000219788Search in Google Scholar

Wong, C. M. L., & Jensen, O. (2020). The paradox of trust: perceived risk and public compliance during the COVID-19 pandemic in Singapore. Journal of Risk Research, 23(7–8), 1021–1030. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2020.175638610.1080/13669877.2020.1756386Search in Google Scholar

Young, S. (2022). Rituals, reassurance, and compliance: Government communication in australia during the COVID-19 pandemic. In P. J. Maarek (Ed.), Manufacturing government communication on Covid-19: A comparative perspective (pp. 147–174). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-09230-5_810.1007/978-3-031-09230-5_8Search in Google Scholar

Published Online: 2024-09-28

© 2024 the author(s), published by De Gruyter.

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.

Downloaded on 10.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1515/commun-2023-0132/html
Scroll to top button