Abstract
Sustainability is generally conceptualised as comprising three interrelated aspects, or “pillars”, namely environmental, social, and economic dimensions (Purvis, Ben, Yong Mao & Darren Robinson. 2019. Three pillars of sustainability: In search of conceptual origins. Sustainability Science 14. 681–695). This conceptual framework is mirrored in the tripartite structure of Environmental, Social and Governance (ESG) reports used by pharmaceutical companies for annual disclosure. This paper focuses specifically on the first “pillar” – the “E” – the environmental aspect of ESG, with a view to exploring the “framing effects” employed in pharmaceutical discourse to construct an image of environmental awareness. The study draws on a corpus of ESG reports from eight pharmaceutical companies present on the stock exchange. A mixed-methods approach was adopted, combining quantitative analysis of lexical patterns with qualitative analysis of framing devices specific to the environmental section of the reports. This dual approach allows for a comprehensive examination of how language is used to frame environmental issues across different companies, while ensuring statistical robustness. Grounded in cognitive linguistics, and specifically in the analytic concept of frame and the theoretical notion of construal (Langacker 1987. Foundations of cognitive grammar: Volume I, theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press, 2007. Constructing grammar: Cognitive and contextual. Oxford: Oxford University Press; Talmy 1988. Frames of reference and the semantics of space. In Vyvyan Evans & Melanie Green (eds.), The cognitive linguistics reader, 21–44. London: Equinox, 2000. Toward a cognitive semantics: Volume I: Concept structuring systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), this paper examines the mental representations stakeholders are prompted to construct in response to specific lexical and grammatical choices. By analysing both lexical frequency and force-dynamic patterns in the ESG corpus, this study elucidates the role of framing devices in shaping stakeholders’ cognitive and emotional responses to environmental concerns within pharmaceutical corporate communication.
1 Introduction
Sustainability is generally conceptualised through three aspects, or “pillars”, encompassing environmental, social, and economic dimensions (Purvis et al. 2019: 681). This framework is reflected in the tripartite structure of Environmental, Social and Governance (henceforth ESG) reports used by pharmaceutical and other companies for annual disclosure. These documents integrate financial reporting with social responsibility communication and are crafted to comply with transparency regulations, offering stakeholders a comprehensive overview of the company’s practices. This research focuses on the first “pillar”, the “E” for environmental issues, and aims to explore how the discourse of pharmaceutical companies exploits “framing effects” to construct an image of ecological awareness and enhance the company’s reputation.
ESG reports follow a formal and standardized structure, typically including detailed sections on policies, objectives, and outcomes, supported by both quantitative and qualitative data. The narrative style varies from technical and informative to more persuasive, depending on the company’s communicative strategy. The language tends to emphasize successful activities and positive initiatives while presenting challenges in a manner that mitigates perceived negative impacts (Fuoli 2018). This strategic use of language not only frames environmental issues but also seeks to build a positive image of the company and bolster its credibility (Fuoli 2018; Higgins and Coffey 2016; Koller 2009).
In this study, quantitative analysis will support the initial hypothesis that the recurrence of certain linguistic choices relates to the activation of certain frames designed to shape stakeholders’ perceptions of how environmental issues are presented and addressed by pharmaceutical companies. According to Langacker (1991), linguistic structures impose conceptualisations on the “scene” at hand. Therefore, the lexical and grammatical choices in pharmaceutical discourse reflect purposeful framing through linguistic stimuli: “When messages are crafted, cognitive frames are used to select cues that can be expected to lead perceivers to reconstruct the intended frames. What is communicated is not the frame itself, but a set of cues that direct people toward the intended meaning” (Baden 2010: 29). These findings are further supported by theoretical frameworks proposed by Fillmore (1975), Talmy (1988, 2000), and Croft and Cruse (2004), which underscore the pivotal role of cognitive structures in shaping linguistic meaning.
Based on these insights, this study investigates how linguistic choices influence the portrayal of environmental issues and corporate responsibility in pharmaceutical discourse. The analysis will explore the strategic use of language in framing ecological concerns, highlighting how specific expressions and rhetorical strategies mould stakeholders’ perceptions and mental representations of corporate environmental engagement.
The paper is organized as follows: Section 2 outlines the theoretical-analytic framework of this study, focusing on the concepts of construal and frame, with a detailed discussion of Force Dynamics. Section 3 describes the data selection and collection process, detailing the corpus of ESG reports analyzed. Section 4 explains the methodological approach employed to identify and interpret FD patterns. Section 5 offers an in-depth analysis of how corporate environmental efforts are framed through FD patterns in ESG reports, examining the linguistic strategies used to construct trust and reliability. Section 6 further investigates psychological FD patterns, analyzing how pharmaceutical companies create a divided self to frame internal and external forces in their ESG discourse. Finally, the paper concludes with reflections on the broader implications of these framing strategies and suggestions for future research.
2 Theoretical-analytical framework
2.1 Cognitive linguistics and construal in ESG discourse
This study is situated within the framework of cognitive linguistics, focusing on “construal” – the way linguistic choices shape the conceptualization of a given “scene”, guiding the interpretation of discourse and influencing readers’ cognitive representations. Scholars such as Langacker (1987, 1991, 2007, 2008), Talmy (1988, 1998, 2000), Croft and Cruse (2004), and Ullman (2017) have extensively examined how linguistic structures impose specific perspectives on events. As Langacker (2008: 55) states, “an expression’s meaning is not just the conceptual content it evokes – equally important is how that content is construed”. This notion aligns with framing theory, which Fillmore (1975: 124) defines as the linguistic choices that activate specific mental models to structure an audience’s understanding of events. Frames function as cognitive shortcuts that guide interpretation and shape discourse reception (Croft and Cruse 2004; Lakoff 2001).
Within the context of ESG discourse, framing is particularly salient, often realized through conceptual metaphors that influence how sustainability and environmental responsibility are perceived (Larson 2011; Milne et al. 2006). These cognitive tools in corporate reporting not only illustrate environmental initiatives but also shape stakeholder perceptions. One especially effective metaphor is that of the “journey” (Milne et al. 2006), which presents sustainability as an ongoing process of growth and progress toward long-term goals. This imagery helps frame sustainability as a gradual and evolving effort, often making it more relatable and less urgent.
Another widely adopted framework in ESG discourse analysis is Systemic Functional Linguistics (SFL), particularly in studies focusing on evaluation, corporate credibility, and sustainability narratives (Fontaine and Li 2023; Li 2024; Piga 2023). Within SFL, the Appraisal Theory (Martin and White 2005) has been applied to analyze how companies construct credibility through lexical and grammatical choices, particularly focusing on how linguistic strategies are employed to convey environmental responsibility and shape a positive corporate image in sustainability reports (Cangero 2024). This includes the strategic use of positive semantic prosody and engagement devices to reinforce companies’ ecological commitment. In this regard, Fuoli (2012) explores evaluative language and trust-building devices in corporate sustainability reports, highlighting how these linguistic tools are used to establish trust and enhance the company’s relationship with its audience.
In addition to framing and evaluation strategies, Force Dynamics (Talmy 1988, 2000), rooted in cognitive linguistics, provides a cognitive-linguistic model for analyzing causal and counterforce relations in discourse. FD has been applied to modality, argumentation, and stance-taking (Sweetser 1990; Verhagen 2005) and to resistance and persistence in media and political discourse (Hart 2011, 2017). These studies show that force-dynamic structures shape how entities are positioned in relation to external pressures.
Despite FD’s relevance to corporate discourse, its application to ESG reporting remains underexplored. Since ESG reports describe corporate efforts to mitigate environmental impact, counter external constraints, and sustain long-term commitments, the force-dynamic model provides a valuable framework for analyzing how these processes are linguistically constructed. By combining Force Dynamics with existing approaches to framing and evaluation, this study addresses this gap by applying FD theory to ESG reports in the pharmaceutical sector, examining how specific lexical and grammatical choices shape the representation of corporate environmental action. This integrated approach offers insights into the rhetorical construction of environmental responsibility in a sector where credibility and responsibility are key communicative concerns.
2.2 Force dynamics
Leonard Talmy’s claim that “force dynamics figures significantly in language structure” (1988: 49) prompted us to focus on this specific framing. As a generalization of the linguistic notion of causation, Force Dynamics (henceforth FD) constructs a scene around an interaction of forces, considering both the participants in this exchange and their prominent force-related traits. Talmy specifically identifies two force-exerting “entities”:
One force-exerting entity is singled out for focal attention – the salient issue in the interaction is whether this entity is able to manifest its force tendency or, on the contrary, is overcome. The second force entity, correlatively, is considered for the effect that it has on the first, effectively overcoming it or not. Borrowing the terms from physiology where they refer to the opposing members of certain muscle pairs, I call the focal force entity the Agonist and the force element that opposes it the Antagonist (1998: 53).[1]
In FD schematizations, the Agonist is always central. This centrality should not be understood in grammatical or lexical-syntactic terms, as the Agonist is neither linked to specific parts of speech nor to thematic roles. Additionally, the opposition between Agonist and Antagonist can shift within and across sentences, sometimes multiple times. The key factor distinguishing the Agonist and ensuring its central role is an intrinsic force tendency that the Antagonist lacks. According to Talmy’s model, the Antagonist serves as an opposing counterpart to the Agonist, and the outcome of their interaction depends on their relative strengths. Figure 1 provides an overview of these fundamental FD concepts, and the symbols used to represent them.

Fundamental terminology and symbols for Force Dynamics (FD).
Four basic force-dynamic patterns (henceforth FDP) can be identified based on the Agonist’s tendency and the balance of strengths:
In (a), for example, an Agonist with an intrinsic tendency towards rest is clashing with a stronger Antagonist, resulting in action (the ball (•) kept rolling (–>–) because of the wind (+) blowing on it). Talmy notes that laterality is irrelevant in FD representations, meaning that FDP schemas can be represented either left to right or vice versa. We use UPPERCASE to differentiate an FDP from the lexico-grammatical structures that create the construal. For example, in (b) “The shed kept standing despite the gale wind blowing against it”, the DESPITE FDP is evoked by the despite construal. This graphic distinction is useful because an FDP can be triggered by different lexico-grammatical or syntactic constructions.
Talmy further categorizes FD interactions into Steady-state and Shifting patterns. The former represents the scenario just discussed, where the force impingement remains constant, while the latter involves changes in state, such as the introduction or removal of the Antagonist force (shift in state), or shifts in the balance of strengths.
Beyond the basic pattern, Talmy introduces a more complex FD configuration called “the divided self”, where “a basic FDP, applied in this case to intra-psychological force-like urges” (Talmy 2000: 69), precedes the external action and takes part in the creation of the forces involved. Talmy uses this internal force opposition to discuss psychological internal drive when “there is one part of the self that wants to perform a certain act and another part that wants that not to happen” (Talmy 2000: 69). However, we argue that this schematisation can be extended to understand how pharmaceutical companies frame their decision-making processes and consequent actions, particularly when they present internal conflicts between economic objectives and ethical or environmental responsibilities.
By analysing ESG discourse as a meaning-making process, we align with the notion of “psychological” not as referring to an individual’s mind but as a form of “collective psyche”. This approach emphasises the discursive image emerging from the texts rather than the empirical sender of the message. Consequently, we adopt the semiotic notion of “enunciator”, to underscore our focus on the text as enunciation – the linguistic act through which abstract values and ideas are manifested in discourse. While the concept shares similarities with the cognitive-linguistic notion of “conceptualizer” (Langacker 2008), “enunciator” emphasizes the textual and semiotic construction of the authorial voice, drawing on insights from discourse analysis and semiotics (Greimas and Courtés 2007)[2] Besides the message, the enunciator projects an image of itself and of the recipient, the enunciatee, into the discourse. In the act of reception, the enunciatee similarly constructs an image of the enunciator based on textual cues. Enunciator and enunciatee thus function as “textual avatars” or actants – abstract semiotic roles that, like empty slots, can be discursively realised in various ways.
3 Data
The corpus analyzed in this study consists of ESG reports from the year 2021 published by eight publicly traded pharmaceutical companies. For clarity, although the companies analyzed in this study are multinational, the U.S. regulatory context is particularly relevant. In the United States, ESG reporting is largely voluntary since neither the Sustainability Accounting Standards Board (SASB)[3] nor the U.S. Securities and Exchange Commission (SEC)[4] mandates comprehensive ESG disclosures; however, many companies choose to publish these reports to enhance transparency and meet investor and regulatory expectations (see Ioannou and Serafeim 2017).
These reports, available in PDF format on the companies’ official websites, were downloaded, converted into plain text, and stripped of paratextual elements such as headers, footers, and tables of contents to prevent distortions in lexical frequency analysis. The cleaned text files were then uploaded to the SketchEngine web app by Lexical Computing[5] for quantitative examination.
The reports vary significantly in length and structural organization. While they generally adhere to the tripartite ESG framework, environmental, social, and governance aspects are often discussed in an integrated manner, making it challenging to isolate the environmental sections exclusively. Restricting the analysis to explicitly environmental segments would risk omitting relevant material and reducing the corpus size, thereby weakening the statistical validity of the findings. To address this issue, a mixed analytical approach was adopted: qualitative analysis was conducted solely on textual instances related to environmental discourse, whereas the quantitative analysis encompassed the entire ESG corpus. This approach ensures a comprehensive investigation of framing strategies while maintaining statistical robustness.
Unless otherwise specified, the Reference Corpus (henceforth RC) used for comparative purposes is the English Web 2021 (enTenTen21) Corpus. Table 1 provides an overview of the ESG corpus composition. To avoid disclosing potentially sensitive or identifiable information, company names have been anonymised using generic labels (e.g., Company 1, Company 2, etc.).
The ESG corpus.a
Companies | Tokens | Types | % |
---|---|---|---|
Company1 | 21,408 | 17,976 | 6.8 |
Company2 | 78,781 | 66,152 | 25.1 |
Company3 | 14,103 | 11,842 | 4.5 |
Company4 | 79,959 | 67,141 | 25.5 |
Company5 | 17,631 | 14,804 | 5.6 |
Company6 | 56,283 | 47,260 | 18 |
Company7 | 33,089 | 27,784 | 10.6 |
Company8 | 12,164 | 10,214 | 3.9 |
Total | 313,418 | 263,173 | 100 |
-
aIn our analysis, a token is any instance of a sequence of characters treated as a unit by the software, while a type refers to each unique lexical item (or lexeme) identified in the text, regardless of how many times it occurs. For example, if the same word appears multiple times, it is counted only once as a distinct lexical item. The percentage (%) represents the proportion of a specific token relative to the total number of tokens in the corpus.
4 Methodology
This study employs a corpus-assisted qualitative and quantitative approach to investigate the linguistic encoding of force-dynamic patterns (FDPs) in pharmaceutical ESG reports. The quantitative analysis focuses on identifying recurring lexical items and phraseological patterns associated with force-dynamic conceptualizations. Special attention is given to verbs and verbal constructions that convey notions of obligation, control, restriction, and mitigation, which are central to the framing of corporate agency in sustainability discourse. Frequency data extracted from the ESG corpus are compared against the English Web 2021 (enTenTen21) reference corpus to assess the distinctiveness of these linguistic choices in corporate environmental communication.
In parallel, the qualitative analysis examines how force-dynamic expressions function within their textual environment. Instances of these linguistic patterns are manually extracted and analyzed to determine their role in shaping the representation of corporate environmental responsibility. The analysis considers both explicit force-dynamic markers (e.g., verbs and adjectives indicating necessity, resistance, or causation) and implicit constructions, such as nominalizations and metaphorical extensions reinforcing a specific stance on sustainability. Additionally, the study examines co-textual elements, including modality markers and agentive structures, to explore how pharmaceutical companies negotiate responsibility within their ESG narratives.
As mentioned in Section 3, to protect sensitive information and ensure confidentiality, company names have been anonymised using generic labels. Additionally, for the same privacy reasons, product names, dates, locations, partner organisations, and other recognisable details have been replaced with square brackets (e.g., [product name], [year], [location], [partner name], [other recognisable details]).
5 Framing corporate environmental efforts as force-dynamic patterns
We now turn to a set of FD schemas frequently invoked in ESG reports to structure a targeted conceptualisation of environmental situations and events. These patterns are prompted by linguistic elements, either grammatical structures or lexical items, which incorporate a force-dynamic component into their conceptual representation. The intended function is to build a relation of trust and construct a credible discursive image of the enunciator in terms of perseverance and reliability.
5.1 Continue to
A pivotal role in encoding FD relations is played by the frequently occurring trigger word continue, mainly in the form of the verbal group complex continue to + verbal group (VG), which accounts for 78 % of all occurrences.
Table 2 summarises the quantitative data for continue and its most frequent construction continue to in the ESG corpus. As shown, nearly 80 % of all continue instances (including the construct continue to) co-occur with the first-person plural pronouns (we/our/us), underscoring the strategic use of this construction to frame the company’s actions as persistent. The markedly higher relative frequency compared to the RC (537 %) confirms its strong function in ESG discourse.
Continue, quantitative data.
Occurrences | Corpus % | Reference corpus | Relative frequency | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Total | 348 | 0.11 % | 0.033 % | 333 % |
Continue to | 271 (78 %) | 0.086 % | 0.016 % | 537 % |
Co-occurrence of continue with we/our/us | 274 (79 %) | 0.087 % | Not relevant | Not relevant |
In Talmy’s terms, continue creates a FD pattern of the DESPITE type (Figure 2c), that is a situation where an expected force or obstacle is present but does not prevent a particular outcome. It highlights how an action or result occurs in spite of opposing forces or constraints (Figure 3). However, unlike the despite construction, which requires an explicit Antagonist, continue implies a constant force application while allowing the enunciator to omit the Antagonist. This is a case that Talmy (1998: 61) contemplates when he explains that not all the factors in an FDP must be mentioned explicitly:
All of the interrelated factors in any FDP are necessarily co-present wherever that pattern is involved. But a sentence expressing that pattern can pick out different subsets of the factors for explicit reference leaving the remainder unmentioned.

Basic steady-state FDPs (Talmy 2000: 415). (a) The ball kept rolling because of the wind blowing on it. (b) The shed kept standing despite the gale wind blowing against it. (c) The ball kept rolling despite the stiff grass. (d) The log kept lying on the incline because of the ridge there.

Shift-in-state FDPs (Talmy 2000: 418). (a) The ball’s hitting it made the lamp topple from the table. (b) The water’s dripping on it made the fire die down. (c) The plug’s coming loose let the water flow from the tank. (d) The stirring rod’s breaking let the particles settle.
Considering the widespread use of the DESPITE FDP, it is striking that despite as a word appears only 22 times in the whole corpus. We hypothesise that using continue rather than despite allows the enunciator to foreground its efforts, concealing the fact that the Antagonist forces are often part of the pharmaceutical companies themselves:
We continue to investigate innovative ways to reduce our environmental impact, including our [program name] in the [region] which allow for easy disposal of unused products. (Company3) |
We continue to pursue [certification type] certification or equivalents. (Company4) |
We continue to evaluate ways to reduce the environmental impact of our products through the use of new technology, application of green chemistry, and solvent recycling and reuse. (Company7) |
Conversely, using despite would require mentioning the opposing force(s) explicitly. By leaving the Antagonist slot empty, as indicated by a dashed line in Figure 4, the opposing force is backgrounded. This results in a DESPITE construal that emphasises the enunciator’s tendency towards action (>) and a positive balance of strength (+), thereby highlighting the company’s determination to operate despite some adverse forces.

Foregrounding of the Agonist’s role in the continue to construction.
By opting for hypotactic verbal group complexes (e.g., we continue to investigate/we continue to pursue/we continue to evaluate) rather than verbal group simplexes (e.g., we investigate/we pursue/we evaluate), the enunciator constructs a durative “time phase” (Halliday and Matthiessen 2007: 511–512). As examples 1–3 show, this type of construal exploits the semantic contribution of the primary verb continue by adding a durative aspect to the action described by the secondary verb, thereby underlining the ongoing commitment of the company in its efforts. The same framing effect is equally obtained with the adjectival form continued:
In [year], biodiversity presented many diverse opportunities despite continued challenges impacting multiple areas of our portfolio, our activities, farmers and our value chain. [Company2] has made significant efforts to develop science-based knowledge-sharing projects expanding resilience in cropping systems demonstrated through effective metrics. |
In the foregoing excerpt, multiple triggers come into play to achieve an FD framing effect: the initial sentence exhibits a sequence of recurring FD patterns of the DESPITE type triggered both grammatically (despite) and lexically (continued, impacting). The meaning construed is the value of resilience attributed to the company and made explicit in the subsequent sentence, which lexicalises the FD frame by first restating the efforts and then mentioning resilience itself – the ability to adapt and stand despite difficulties.
5.2 Commit
The pervasive use of commit and commitment is another revealing example of FD framing strategically evoked to build a reliable image of pharmaceutical companies. The quantitative analysis shows an overwhelming presence of commit* compared to the RC, and 17 times more frequent (Table 3).
Commit*, quantitative data.
Total occurrences | Corpus % | RC | Relative frequency |
---|---|---|---|
444 | 0.14 % | 0.0082 % | 1707 % |
The semantic complexity of commit* enables the enunciator to construe a frame for building trust by operating on three distinct but interconnected semantic levels discussed below.
5.2.1 Durative aspect of the commitment construal
The first, most superficial discursive effect of the commit construal is to foster trust by adding or emphasising a durative aspect to the enunciator’s actions. In this regard, it functions similarly to the continue framing, highlighting the company’s ability and willingness to engage in sustained efforts:
We share society concern for [ethical issue] and are actively committed to setting high standards for animal care. (Company1) |
We are committed to reducing our impact on the planet through the efficient, circular use of [resource] and other natural resources. (Company1) |
Interestingly, when considering the polysemic nature of the verb commit as defined in the Oxford Advanced Learner’s Dictionary (Hornby 2005: 302–303),[6] it is noteworthy that in ESG reports the term never occurs with the meaning of perpetrate. Instead, in this context, commit conveys the meaning of “promise”, thereby implying the presence of at least potential trust (see below).
Commit
[…]
3 ∼sb/yourself (to sth/to doing sth) [often passive] to promise sincerely that you will definitely do sth, keep to an agreement or arrangement, etc.
4 [VN]∼ yourself (to sth) to give an opinion or make a decision openly so that it is then difficult to change it.
Promise, verb
1∼ sth (to sb) | ∼ sb sth to tell sb that you will definitely do or not do sth, or that sth will definitely happen
(Hornby 2005: 302–303)
As the examples show, commit conveys much more than a general willingness to “keep doing”. Besides the durative aspect, the inclusion of definitely in the word definition reflects what Thompson (2004: 72) terms “high modal commitment”, thus incorporating the meaning of “promise”. By declaring its pledge to action through a commitment, the enunciator implicitly shifts the “epistemic gradient” (Enfield 2009) towards certainty without relying on explicit modal verbs or adjuncts. This observation aligns with other research (Fenice and Mocini forthcoming), which suggests a consistently low occurrence of modal verbs and adjuncts in ESG reports, along with a preference for less congruent and more metaphorical modality. This approach enacts a complex semiotic strategy that alternates between ostentation and concealment of the enunciator’s presence to manipulate trust and align stakeholders with the company’s values.
5.2.2 Perspectival shift in the construal of commitment
The pledging aspect adds another dimension to the framing device of commit* by leveraging the perspective inherent in the term, which shifts the focus from the actual execution of the action to the attempt and/or willingness to perform it. As Verhagen (2007: 8) explains, “perspective is a central part of the entire range of possible construal relations, in fact a definitional aspect of prototypical instances of construal”. This notion of perspective, which involves how actions are framed and understood, complements Talmy’s (1998: 73) identification of complex FD sequences through their “phase/factivity” patterns:
The first is what I will call ‘phase’: the location along the temporal sequence at which focal attention is placed. The second is ‘factivity’: the occurrence or non-occurrence of portions of the sequence and the speaker’s knowledge about this.
The use of commit* indeed creates a perspectival shift, directing attention to the inceptive phase of a protracted activity rather than its effects or results. Claims like the following tend to obscure the results. Even when numerical data are provided. They lack factivity because it is not specified whether, or to what extent, the promise implicit in the initial temporal phase of the commitment has been fulfilled:
we have committed to achieve [percentage]% sustainable paper in secondary and tertiary packaging by the end of [year]. (Company1) |
we live into our commitment to operate increasingly more sustainably and significantly reduce the planetary impact of our operations and our products. (Company4) |
Both claims (7) and (8) focus on the commitment and the intent to act purposefully, yet they omit reference to actual achievements. This framing strategy results in a lack of communicative transparency, or rather, a deliberate opacity on the part of the pharmaceutical company.
5.2.3 “Interpersonal” layer of the commitment construal
Quantitative analysis of the corpus has revealed a notable preference for the term commit over try and attempt in ESG reports, highlighting a significant distinction in how commitment is framed. Specifically, commit appears frequently, while try and attempt are sparse, with only two and one occurrences, respectively. This suggests that the enunciator aims to utilize the TRY TO pattern but opts to use commit* as the FD trigger instead. This observation is supported by the fact that, even when combining the occurrences of commit (0.008 %), try (0.05 %), and attempt (0.013 %) in the reference corpus, they collectively account for only about half the occurrences of commit in ESG reports (0.071 % vs. 0.14 %). This marked preference for commit can be attributed to an additional semantic layer of the frame it evokes, which is absent in try. As just highlighted, quantitative analysis of the corpus suggests that the enunciator almost exclusively employs commit as a trigger word for the TRY TO force-dynamic pattern.
To clarify, while the terms commit, try, and attempt are not synonymous, their comparative analysis in this study is justified by the communicative intent behind their use. Unlike try and attempt, which denote uncertainty regarding the outcome, commit conveys a strong intention and a declared responsibility to achieve a goal. This linguistic choice reinforces the company’s image of determination and reliability in addressing environmental challenges. Specifically, commit is interpersonally tuned, engaging the enunciatee with the implicit promise contained in the act of commitment. According to Brandt’s (1992: 19) semiotic analysis, a promise involves a pathway consisting of four phases:
As Figure 5 illustrates, in an ideal-type scenario, following the promisor’s (S1) offer of a pledge (phase 1), the promisee (S2) responds to the promisor with a sign of acceptance and expectation, establishing trust. Consequently, the act of promising inherently fosters the development of trust (phase 2). Only then come the phases of realisation (phase 3) and evaluation of the promise (phase 4), although explicit mention of all these phases is not always necessary. The TRY TO perspective of commitment in ESG enunciation facilitates the backgrounding of outcomes through omission, a cognitive strategy known as “gapping”, which is part of the “window of attention” pattern (Evans and Green 2006: 527). This concept refers to the cognitive process where an enunciator selectively focuses the audience’s attention on specific aspects of a situation while deliberately ignoring or omitting others. By strategically “windowing” certain elements of the “scene” and “gapping” others, the enunciator can reap the benefits of making a promise, such as increased trust from the enunciatee, without fully addressing all potential outcomes or implications.

Promise pathway (Brandt 1992: 20, our translation).
5.3 The force of commitment
An additional aspect of the use of commit* which reinforces the hypotheses presented above warrants further discussion. The following chart illustrates the distribution of various forms of commit* within the ESG corpus (Table 4):
Distribution of commit* forms.
Form | Occurrences | % |
---|---|---|
Commitment | 261 | 59 % |
Be committed | 126 | 28 % |
Commit | 39 | 9 % |
Remain committed | 9 | 2 % |
Other | 9 | 2 % |
What immediately stands out is that only 9 % of all occurrences are in the active verbal form of commit, while 59 % are nominalisations, and the remaining 32 % are in the passive predicative constructions (primarily be committed, which accounts for 28 %). This distribution may be partly due to the typical construction of the commit verbal process, which, as noted in the dictionary definition mentioned above, often appears in the passive voice. Even after considering this, the quantitative analysis reveals an unusually high prevalence of be committed compared to commit. This trend persists, albeit to lesser extent, even when applying the “rough filter” of to.
Table 5 shows that the construction be committed (including the most frequently used we are committed) is used significantly more frequently in the ESG corpus than in the reference corpus. This trend also applies to the specific construction be committed to.
Prevalence of the construction be committed versus commit in the ESG and reference corpora.
% of be committed vs. total commit | % of be committed to vs. total commit to | |
---|---|---|
ESG corpus | 67 % | 69 % |
Ref. corpus | 29 % | 51 % |
Moreover, out of the 39 instances of commit VG used in the active voice, only 5 occur in the present tense. This raises the question: why does the enunciator significantly favour the COMMITMENT framing while almost entirely avoiding the phrase “we commit”, which appears in only 1 % of occurrences? One possible explanation may be found in the pragmatic meanings conveyed by the COMMIT construal and the varying force of the speech acts involved. We suggest that the ESG enunciator seeks to leverage the force of an illocutionary act without fully committing to a performative act. According to Austin (1962: 6), when an utterance that uses a verb in the first-person singular, present active tense, and performs the illocutionary act specified by the verb, it can be termed a performative utterance. However, in ESG reports, the commit construal rarely meets these criteria, as most occurrences appear in passive predicative constructions (be committed) rather than in the form of an explicit first-person performative (we commit). This structural preference suggests an attempt to convey commitment while mitigating the level of direct self-attribution of agency. Instances of such performative utterances remain exceedingly rare:
We also commit to reducing emissions related to upstream logistics by [percentage]% and business travel by [percentage]% by [year] from a [year] baseline, and to use our influence to catalyze similar reductions across our value chain. (Company7) |
We strongly commit to operating our business responsibly and disclosing our progress to stakeholders on an annual basis. (Company8) |
Commit is very seldom used in a performative utterance (i.e., as a true promise, in the first-person present tense and using an explicitly performative construction). In the overwhelming majority of cases, the enunciator describes a commitment that is already established (we are committed/our commitment). While these statements carry similar semantic strength, they function as declarative utterances rather than performative acts. This aligns with Brandt’s (1992: 19) analysis of a promise:
The performative requires the absence of any rhetoric. ‘P’ has exactly the meaning that ‘P’ is supposed to have. In ‘I declare the meeting open’, the verb is indispensable. […] When I say, ‘I promise…’, I am saying that this is a promise, that a promise is being made here and now. Perhaps this is why we do not say, ‘Here and now, I promise’ […] By saying, ‘Today I swear’, I am not swearing; I am describing what I am doing today. I am stating.[7]
According to Austin (1962: 6), performative utterances typically involve the first-person singular, as in the case of “I promise” or “I apologize,” where the utterance itself constitutes the performance of the act. However, in business discourse, instantiated here in pharmaceutical discourse, the first-person plural is more commonly used. While this may not align with Austin’s original examples, the use of “we commit” or “we promise” still carries a performative dimension, expressing the collective commitment of an organization. This plural form serves as a conventional means of conveying corporate responsibility and collective action. It allows companies to frame their commitments as institutional rather than personal, maintaining the performative effect described by Austin while adapting it to the corporate context.
If performativity does not occur in similar situations, it becomes even clearer when the enunciator makes claims about the past. For example:
we declared a commitment that by [year], we will reduce our total annual use of [material] in packaging weight by [percentage]% compared to [year]. (Company4) |
[Country name], where we have committed to plant [number] trees by [year]. (Company1) |
In other words, phrases like “we are committed”, “our commitment”, and even “we (have) committed” merely describe the present or past state of affairs. Only “we commit” carries the full force of a performative speech act, yet the enunciator appears to avoid this phrasing as much as possible. This avoidance does not imply that the claims about commitment are less valid or untrue, as they retain all the semantic force of the term. Instead, there is a shift in focus – what we might describe as a backwards perspective, from which the promise is presented as already having been made. This framing schema is similar to the one observed in the discussion of the TRY TO construal and allows the enunciator to present the enunciatee with the fait accompli – “we are [already] committed” or “we have [already] committed” – rather than performing the actual promise. Referring to Brandt’s diagram (Figure 5), it becomes clear that this strategy assumes the promisee’s trust, allowing the enunciator to bypass phase (2) of the promise, where trust is typically established. Consequently, the company can benefit from the “trust surplus” associated with commitment, without performing the formal speech act, which would necessitate “a sign of acceptation and expectations” in Brandt’s terms, achievable only with a present tense, performative “we commit”. To some extent, the enunciator is – we might say – forcing trust or, more precisely, constructing a frame where trust is presumed to already exist.
In summary, the durative aspect of commit* fosters trust by emphasizing constancy and reliability, while the perspectival shift associated with the term redirects cognitive focus from the action’s results to the intention or willingness to act. Additionally, on an interpersonal level, commit* carries an implicit promise, imbuing the statement with a sense of performativity, even though true performative acts are often avoided by skipping the initial phase and presuming trust.
6 “Psychological” force-dynamic patterns in ESG discourse
In ESG discourse, companies frequently portray themselves not merely as rational actors responding to external demands, but as ethically engaged agents moved by inner forces. This rhetorical strategy aligns with what Talmy (2000) describes as “psychological” force-dynamic (FD) patterns, where action results from an internal exertion of will, often construed as one part of the self overcoming another. In such constructions, the pharmaceutical company is framed as a divided subject whose actions stem from moral responsibility or internal motivation, rather than coercion or external regulation. The following subsections examine how this psychological force-dynamic structure is deployed in ESG communication, beginning with internal divided-self construals, moving on to externally catalysed versions of the same schema, and finally considering intra-company divided-self framings.
6.1 Internal division of the corporate self
According to Talmy (2000: 71, 73),
the psychological component is normally included and understood as the factor that renders the [man] a stronger Agonist able to withstand the [crowd]. It accomplishes this by maintaining the expenditure of effort, that is, by a continuously renewed exertion, in which a goal-oriented part of the psyche overcomes a repose-oriented part so as to generate the output of energy […]. A more peripheral part of the [psyche] overcomes a more central part’s intrinsic repose to animate the otherwise inert [physical] component into overt force manifestation against a further external force entity.[8]
Aside from the references to a human individual enclosed in brackets, this definition describes a framing schema found during our exploration of highly recurrent FD activators, searching for more complex patterns beyond the basic ones. Some of those involving commit and continue, have already been examined, while other statistically significant trigger words will be discussed in this section.
The first component of a psychological FD schema is the separation of the self into a passive part – which is often left implicit and tends towards stasis and the preservation of the status quo – and an active, dynamic part of the self, which is ready for action and frequently foregrounded. The second component is a two-phase structure in which (1) the two internal forces clash, and the resultant is then (2) directed outwards, against an additional external force.
We feel all businesses – especially those like ours that are dedicated to human health – have a duty to minimize their impact on climate change and promote a long and prosperous future for all of Earth’s inhabitants. (Company3) |
At [Company5], we believe that it is our responsibility to grow our company in a way that protects the planet and minimizes our impact on the environment. (Company5) |
In (13), We feel…duty creates an internalisation of an external drive to action. On the one hand, it conceals the fact that this duty actually originates from the public or stakeholders, if not from an actual regulatory requirement. On the other hand, it evokes a psychological force-dynamic pattern (henceforth PsFDP), creating a state of divided self, where the force entity belonging to the enunciator is construed as having both central and peripheral parts, each acting in different roles. As a result, the enunciator frames its actions as caused by an internal, self-directed force, in a pattern where the Antagonist entity is a part of the divided self and compels the inactive Agonist into action. As Talmy (2000: 71) notes when discussing the psychological FD pattern:
This Antagonist represents a sense of responsibility or propriety and appears as an internalization of external social values. In effect, perhaps, a FD opposition originating between the self and the surroundings seems here to be introjected into an opposition between parts of the self.
In (14) both we believe and responsibility frame a push that comes from – as Talmy puts it – “external social values” as an internal drive. The construction evokes a frame where the enunciator gathers internal forces to counter an external one (to protect the planet). Moreover, this strategy also serves to disguise the fact that what should be the external force in the FD pattern is actually another internal force ( our impact on the environment).
Rather than reflecting a true corporate split self, this strategy suggests a reframing of forces, where external pressures are internalized as corporate values, and corporate actions are externalized as responses to external demand.
Likewise, the “protracted effort” framing analysed above can be seen as part of a psychological FD pattern, where the company spurs itself into action and improvement. Adding a further layer of complexity to the commitment construal, one may note that a commitment is essentially a self-directed force “in which a goal-oriented part of the psyche overcomes a repose-oriented part so as to generate the output of energy” (Talmy 2000: 71), i.e., a PsFDP of the EXERT as depicted in Figure 6:

The EXERT FDP (Talmy 2000: 431). aVP stands or Verbal Phrase.
For instance, in (15) the commitment can be schematised as a first phase, in which one part of the self impinges on another (i.e., we applied a force to ourselves):
To ensure the safe use of our crop protection products based on adequate research, we made an important voluntary commitment in [year]. (Company2) |
Thus, with the lens of psychological FD, we can gain further insight into the “prolonged effort” frame, which not only underlines the constant expenditure of energy, but also implies that this energy is necessary to oppose the other internal force – the lazy one – that is often omitted or backgrounded, since the enunciator does not wish to stress the fact that it belongs to the company itself.
As illustrated in Figure 7 below, the exertion in PHASE 1 results in an Agonist that has overcome internal attrition and has now progressed to PHASE 2 with an acquired tendency for action. This resulting force can then be directed towards solving external problems (to ensure…)

FD schematization of example (15).
The two-phase structure of Talmy’s psychological FD schematisation[9] is even more apparent in (16) below:
With the help of new processes, [emission type] emissions from [industry/activity] can not only be reduced, but can also be captured in the soil. (Company2) |
Since corporate processes are internal forces, this is a clear instance of a divided self. In the first phase, an internal exertion of forces, lexicalised by help, results in an ability to oppose an external antagonist (reduce gas emissions). Unlike example (15), where the outcome (whether real or expected) was foregrounded in the initial position through topicalisation, example (16) frames the situation as two consecutive phases: (PHASE 1) new processes help us → (PHASE 2) we use that resultant force to fight against gas emissions. This example illustrates a recurring schema in ESG discourse, where the aggregation of internal forces is directed towards addressing environmental challenges.
While identifying the individual entities involved in an FDP is useful, increasing sentence complexity can lead to a proliferation of entities, making coherent FD schematisation challenging. Therefore, it is necessary to use entity clusters, which group together all the FD entities belonging to the same category, as exemplified through the analysis of (17) below.
They [genetically modified crops] can help farmers to increase productivity despite difficult growing conditions by protecting harvests from [pests/weeds] while consuming fewer natural resources. (Company2) |
The sentence involves seven entities exerting forces:
Genetically modified crops
Farmers
Productivity
Difficult growing conditions
Harvests
Pests/weeds
Natural resources
These entities can be grouped into four clusters: (1 = E) belongs to the enunciator (Company2), (2 = S), (3 = S), (5 = S) refer to Company2’s stakeholders, namely the farmers; (4 = O) and (6 = O) are framed as part of the same cluster of forces opposing the farmers. Finally, (7 = N) introduces a fourth cluster, including only one entity: natural resources.
E, S, O, and N are typical entity clusters in ESG environmental discourse. This clustering helps identify logical connections between basic FD patterns by grouping the force-exerting entities according to their allegiance, as assigned by the framing strategy. Interestingly, the E cluster also includes the company’s partners, associated groups, institutions, and collaborative processes, accounting for the constant reference to collaboration, help, and so on, which usually construe a divided-self pattern, In other words, corporate partnerships not only evoke Talmy’s HELP FDP – i.e., Antagonist that removes obstacles – but also create a psychological two-phase structure, thus participating in the perspectival shift already discussed.
Through [collaboration type] collaboration, we are helping to address societal and health challenges - such as the pandemic response, health system resilience and [environmental issue] and are committed to making a sustainable impact. (Company1) |
Here, the initial prepositional phrase functions as an adverbial, introducing two force-exerting entities through lexicalisation (collaboration = HELP FDP). The first coordinate main clause expounds the HELP FDP lexicalised in collaboration, reinforcing the frame and assigning the active role of helper to the enunciator (we are helping). The second coordinate clause highlights that the role of the helper is often framed into the internal phase of exertion. In this example, the collaboration is part of the internal phase pertaining to the commitment, i.e., to the psychological exertion compound which will then be directed towards the external force, here lexicalised as (sustainable) impact. While this instance has been interpreted primarily as an example of the “removal of obstacle” pattern typical of Talmy’s HELP FDP, it is worth noting that in ESG discourse, helping may also reflect a “pushing-like” construal – where the enunciator actively supports or facilitates the Agonist’s progression toward a goal. This dual reading reflects the flexible rhetorical use of HELP FDPs in corporate communication, where both the alleviation of external constraints and the positive reinforcement of stakeholder action may coexist.
In ESG discourse, the internal forces in the cluster can belong to the company itself or be activated by an external catalyst (partner), but the psychological framing device remains the same:
We are partnering with [four names of partner companies] and [research institution], to advance the sustainable development of new manufacturing processes. (Company1) |
Even though the enunciator and the partners are separate entities, if we consider them as belonging to the same cluster (E), the construal is identical to a PsFDP. What is happening is precisely framed as “a more peripheral part [of the psyche] overcomes a more central part’s intrinsic repose to animate the otherwise inert [physical] component into overt force manifestation against a further external force entity” (Talmy 2000: 73). In this scenario, the partners help the peripheral part of the company’s self to overcome its central, less active component which would tend towards stasis. The construal emphasizes the role of the external partners not just as co-agents, but as catalysts that enable the enunciator (E) to act, especially when analysed through Talmy’s psychological force-dynamic schema. In this light, the action is framed less as equal cooperation and more as the activation of the enunciator’s potential through external support, resembling a PsFDP scenario where the peripheral component gains enough momentum to overcome inertia.
Table 6 provides an overview of the PsFDP in ESG reports, with notable examples relating to environmental discourse and their relative frequency compared to the RC.
Two-phase structure of in the ESG psychological FD patterns.
Phase 1 | Phase 2 | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
FD definition | A goal-oriented part of the psyche overcomes a repose-oriented part so as to generate the output of energy. | Overt force manifestation against a further external force entity. | ||||||
General ESG schema | Internal gathering of forces | Opposing environmental challenges | ||||||
FD triggers | Intra-company PsFDP | FD trigger | Corpus % | RC | Rel.Freq. | |||
FD trigger | Corpus % | R. C. | Rel.Freq. | Minimize | 0.016 | 0.0023 | 695 % | |
Aim | 0.074 | 0.01 | 740 % | Maximise | 0.012 | 0.002 | 600 % | |
Target | 0.1 | 0.013 | 769 % | Reduce | 0.1 | 0.018 | 555 % | |
Objective | 0.018 | 0.0058 | 310 % | Increase | 0.1 | 0.035 | 286 % | |
Purpose | 0.037 | 0.013 | 285 % | Impact | 0.18 | 0.017 | 1,058 % | |
Goal | 0.098 | 0.017 | 576 % | Challenge | 0.064 | 0.019 | 336 % | |
Commit(ment)a | 0.14 | 0.0082 | 1707 % | Footprint | 0.026 | 0.001 | 2,600 % | |
PsFDP with external catalyst | ||||||||
FD trigger | Corpus % | Ref. c. | Rel.freq. | |||||
Help | 0.16 | 0.072 | 222 % | |||||
Partner(ship) | 0.17 | 0.02 | 850 % | |||||
Collaboration | 0.051 | 0.0046 | 1,108 % |
-
aWhile commit(ment) may not denote a discrete goal, it frequently acts as a trigger of internal mobilization in ESG discourse.
The psychological structure can be employed to implement complex framing strategies, such as the one illustrated in example (17) discussed above, here (20) annotated with FD entities (1–7) and FD patterns (a–d):
FD patterns: |
They [genetically modified crops] (1) can help farmers (2) |
to increase productivity (4) despite difficult growing conditions (3) |
by (1/2*) protecting harvests (5) from pests/weeds (6) |
while (?) consuming fewer natural resources (7) |
As shown in Figure 8a below, this sentence features a multi-layered structure of intertwining FDPs activated by specific FDP trigger words. The verbs help and protect, along with the preposition despite, facilitate the identification of three main FD patterns involving six entities exerting forces. However, the concept of ‘help’ can be interpreted in multiple ways within the FD framework. While the present analysis adheres to Talmy’s schematization, primarily framing HELP as the Antagonist removing an opposing force, it is also possible to view it as a pushing-like action, where the Antagonist supports the Agonist’s progress. Both interpretations are valid and may coexist, reflecting the complex interplay of forces in the discourse.

Force-dynamic structure of example (20), showing layered interactions in environmental discourse. (a) FD schematization of the HELP interaction and related patterns (DESPITE and PROTECT), highlighting the enunciator’s role in shifting the balance of forces. (b) Semantic ambiguity created by the non-finite clause while consuming fewer natural resources, which can refer to different parts of the preceding discourse.
The primary message conveyed, corresponding to the main clause (a), is E HELPS S. As illustrated in Figure 8a, Talmy’s framework depicts this HELP FD interaction as the Antagonist removing an opposing force. This interaction (a) is followed by a specification that answers “help to do what”. To interpret this second FD pattern (b), consider the subordinate clause as zooming in on the circle representing the Agonist farmers. This allows for the analysis of what Talmy (2000: 54) refers to as the “Agonist’s intrinsic force tendency towards action”, represented by a (>). This tendency results from the clash of forces depicted in (b), an FD schema of the DESPITE type where the Agonist is the stronger entity. However, this scenario remains hypothetical without the HELP provided by the enunciator, as described in the main clause (a). Specifically, the help is aimed at enhancing the Antagonist’s capacity to overcome challenges and achieve outcomes. Finally, a third layer is added by specifying how the help is provided (c). Notably, protect involves three participants (1/2, 5 and 6), a scenario not directly covered by Talmy’s FD schemas. This can still be explained in terms of FD patterns by dividing the interaction into different phases based on “the location along the temporal sequence at which focal attention is placed” (Talmy 2000: 73). Here, the focus is on the present and future (the help and its expected effects), while the past is left implicit. As shown in Figure 8a, the implied pre-existing conditions suggest that pests and weeds would otherwise undermine harvests. The enunciator’s intervention (HELP, interaction a) shifts the balance of strengths (+) from the Antagonist to the Agonist, changing the resultant force interaction from rest to action, thus aligning with the Agonist/stakeholder’s tendency. Consequently, protect creates an overarching narrative that unfolds over time, encompassing all the previous FD patterns and positioning the enunciator as the agent of a shifted balance of forces that enables the stakeholders to achieve their goals. Finally, (d) introduces a fourth FD layer: the non-finite clause following while can refer to any of the previous clauses: (a), (b), or (c). This creates ambiguity in the interpretation, as the ecological benefits mentioned earlier can be interpreted as valuable for both E and S, depending on the reading. Figure 8b displays these different possible interpretations.
Specifically, (a + d) attributes the value of environmental protection to the enunciator, (b + d) assigns it to the stakeholders and (c + d) to both. Therefore, a synergy between syntax and FD construal is employed to align E with S, potentially supporting a more effective trust-building strategy. In summary, this construal aims to position the stakeholders in the Enunciator’s role, without explicitly stating “we are on the same side”.
6.2 External catalysts and the framing of internal exertion
The following examples illustrate a construal strategy that shifts the reader’s perspective and diverts attention from inconvenient truths by portraying internal effort as driven by external forces. This is evident in (21) below:
In [year], our partnership with the non-profit organisation, [NPO name], continued to inspire a reduction in the environmental impacts of our lab practices. (Company1) |
Several preliminary considerations are necessary to frame the analysis. Firstly, the term partnership clearly lexicalizes the HELP FD pattern. Secondly, this schema implies multiple phases as suggested by the durative aspect of the actions described (the ongoing inspiration and the resulting reduction). Finally, due to its significance as an FD trigger, impact warrants specific attention (Table 7).
Impact, quantitative data.
Total occurrences | Corpus % | RC | Relative frequency |
---|---|---|---|
574 | 0.18 % | 0.017 % | 1,058 % |
Being ten times more common in the ESG corpus than in the RC, the numeric relevance of impact should not be overlooked. Approximately one-third of its occurrences are in sentences containing the string environ*, with the collocation environmental impact appearing 74 times. There is no doubt that impact represents the lexicalisation of an FDP, defined as follows:
the act of an object hitting another; a: an impinging or striking especially of one body against another; b: a forceful contact or onset (Merriam-Webster Dictionary).[10]
Even in its metaphorical use, impact retains most of its physical connotation. According to the definition of the word and Talmy’s terminology, the most immediate FD schematisation should be a shifting-state FD pattern of the MAKE type, where an Antagonist enters the state of impingement, altering the state of the Agonist away from its original tendency (Figure 9).

FDP for impact (Talmy 2000: 418).
On closer inspection, environmental impact is not a sudden blow or an instantaneous forceful impulse but rather a gradual, long-term damaging force. Thus, it is schematised as a steady-state BECAUSE OF pattern, i.e. the environment changes BECAUSE OF human practices (Figure 10).

FDP for environmental impact.
Returning to the analysis of example (21), the externally catalyzed divided self can easily be identified, as the partner helps the company initiate a reduction in environmental impact. The sentence also illustrates a continuously renewed exertion, representing a prototypical psychological FD pattern. In the first phase, a goal-oriented part of the psyche overcomes a repose-oriented part generating energy. In the second phase, this resulting energy is directed against a force presented as external: the environmental impact of our lab practices (Figure 11).

FD schematization of example (21).
Framing the situation this way allows the company to foreground the internal phase, highlighting the proactiveness and inspired action while downplaying its own role as the Antagonist. In other words, the psychological FDP construes a situation where the energy resulting from the internal force components is directed towards an external force entity. In this case, however, the external force slot is occupied by the company’s own impact (our lab practices). Moreover, inspire suggests future prospects rather than present results. In Talmy’s terms, this shifts focal attention to the future phase, obscuring the implication of reducing: that in the present/past the environmental impact might be/have been too high. Thus, the emphasis is on the partnership and the reduction rather than the ongoing (albeit reduced) impact. Three temporal phases are described, but the specific framing allows the enunciator to avoid focusing on phases that cast a negative light on itself, particularly the first phase:
Our practices might have had a significant environmental impact (initial phase is gapped, only implied)
[NPO name] helped us overcome our stasis. The successful action is attributed to the (constant) force coming from their help (steady-state FD) (foregrounded psychological exertion)
Continued inspiration to reduce environmental impact (external Antagonist which is actually the company itself, backgrounded by the FD structure)
A second example of a catalyst framing device can be found in (22):
working with our partners to create a value change that minimizes impact to the environment. (Company5) |
A first phase involving the mobilization of internal forces, namely a lexicalised HELP FDP (partners), is followed by the redirection of the resulting energy towards an external force entity, i.e., the environmental impact. Notably, this two-phase framing of the divided self is reinforced by an explicit reference to the result of the first FD schema: Talmy’s internal phase, referred to as EXERTION, is framed as a value change. This value change is rhetorically framed as an autonomous agent within the ensuing clash of forces, signalled by the highly recurrent FD trigger word minimize. Indeed, the analysis of its occurrences confirms its strong connection to environmental issues (Table 8).
Minimize, quantitative data.
Total occurrences | Corpus % | RC | Relative frequency | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Minimize | 50 | 0.016 % | 0.0023 % | 695 % |
Maximise | 40 | 0.012 % | 0.002 % | 600 % |
Reduce | 313 | 0.1 % | 0.018 % | 555 % |
More than 70 % of the occurrences of minimize (37 out of 50) are found in sections relating to the environment. The use of minimize can be analysed through Talmy’s HINDER FDP, where one entity exerts a force that obstructs or impedes the actions or goals of another entity. This pattern is part of a specific series of schemas associated with a weaker Antagonist, which Talmy relates to the DESPITE pattern (Figure 12).

The HINDER FDP.
This pattern is also construed by reduce, which is more common than minimize but has a similarly high relative frequency compared to the RC. However, the use of minimize adds the implication that the enunciator is exerting maximal effort, suggesting that minimizing the impact requires the utmost exertion. Considering all this, the full FD schema of the sentence in (22) is as follows (Figure 13)

FD schematization of example (22).
Remarkably, the lexicalisation of impact construes a backgrounded FDP with an unnamed Antagonist, a gapping device the enunciator uses to distance itself from its actions. This becomes evident when compared to (6) and (14), where the enunciator’s role as Antagonist is explicitly windowed through expressions such as our impact.
6.3 Intra-company divided self
The two-phase perspectival strategy is also used in intra-company divided-self framing:
We limit our environmental impact by integrating sustainability best practices and adhering to operational excellence in our global operations. Our business is growing, and we continue to take actions across our operations to minimize our environmental footprint and ensure the highest standard of [workplace well-being and safety]. (Company8) |
In this example, the Agonist’s and Antagonist’s forces belong to the same entity, the enunciator. The HINDER FDP is applied in the main clause of the first sentence (We limit), framing the interaction as an internal clash. This frame is reinforced and extended in the second sentence, where continue to adds a durative aspect. Once again, a temporal perspective is evident, with a focus on the present situation. While the company might have had a greater environmental impact in the past, this fact is omitted. In the present, the company exerts an internal force to counteract its own impact, leading to a reduced environmental footprint. Moreover, the lexicalisations used for the HINDER FD pattern indicate an increase in the Antagonist’s force (with terms like limit evolving to minimize or highest), creating a crescendo of effort that implies an intensified commitment, with a clear promotional intent.
The following example presents an even clearer psychological construal, reinforced through strategic lexicalisation:
[Company4] is marshalling resources, expertise and convening power to positively impact planetary health while also ensuring the resilience of our business for generations to come. This commitment includes continually striving to improve the environmental footprint of our operations, our products and our value chain. |
Here, the verb marshalling acts as a trigger for the first phase, where an intra-company divided self is “volitionally” (Talmy 2000: 447) mobilizing its strengths against external forces. This construal evokes a military frame, further reinforced by the phrase convening power, which highlights strategic coordination. The conflict is lexicalised in the dual challenges of impact and resilience. The analysis reveals two FDPs: HELP (i.e., we positively impact planet) and DESPITE (i.e., we will resist despite adversities). Both FDPs enhance the narrative of trustworthiness across different aspects of the ESG (namely Environmental and Governance). Additionally, the second sentence reinforces the framing by introducing a double trigger for a steady-state FDP: the constant internal effort implied by commitment aligns with the external continually striving, giving both phases a unified aspectual meaning and implying a consistent effort over time. This strategy narrativizes one of the company’s core values: trustworthiness.
7 Conclusions
This study has sought to illuminate the complex discursive strategies that pharmaceutical companies employ in ESG reports to frame their environmental impact. By examining ESG reports specifically, this research makes a significant contribution to understanding how companies in this sector strategically shape their discourse to engage stakeholders and build a credible narrative about their environmental actions. It reveals how these companies not only present their environmental efforts but also strategically craft their discourse to engage stakeholders and create a nuanced, credible narrative.
The concept of “construal”, central to cognitive linguistics, has been pivotal in understanding how linguistic expressions shape mental representations and influence stakeholders’ perceptions. The application of FD theory to ESG reports demonstrates how cognitive linguistics can deepen our understanding of corporate discourse, especially in a high-stakes sector like pharmaceuticals. The deliberate selection of FDPs articulates the interaction between the company and various environmental challenges, reflecting a sophisticated approach to narrative construction. Trigger terms like continue, commit, and impact underscore a proactive stance, promoting a perception of responsibility and engagement in environmental stewardship.
Furthermore, the notion of the “divided self” reveals how companies manage the inherent challenges and contradictions of their operations. The use of this concept demonstrates how linguistic strategies can simultaneously mitigate negative perceptions while promoting a more favourable image of corporate sustainability efforts. By shifting the narrative focus from past impact to future-oriented exertion and partnerships, pharmaceutical companies enhance their image but also build stakeholders’ trust, aligning corporate actions with broader environmental goals.
In addition to its core insights, this study lays the groundwork for further research. Upcoming studies could explore how similar discursive strategies function across different industries and cultural contexts, and assess their influence on actual environmental practices and corporate accountability. Given the societal relevance of ESG reporting, it is also important to investigate how these linguistic strategies influence not only corporate reputation but also real-world sustainability outcomes.
In summary, this research provides insights into the framing mechanisms used in ESG reports and the role of language in shaping corporate environmental discourse. By drawing on cognitive linguistics, it contributes to a deeper understanding of how pharmaceutical companies manage their narratives and points to the need for further investigation into the effectiveness of these strategies across various contexts. Ultimately, this study underscores the significance of ESG reports as a distinct genre with far-reaching implications for corporate accountability and societal impact.
-
Author contributions: The paper is the result of joint work. Renzo Mocini contributed primarily to Sections 1, 2, and 5; Andrea Fenice to Sections 3, 4, 6, and 7.
-
Research funding: The research presented in this article was carried out within the framework of the PRIN project ‘Communicating transparency: New trends and insights for professional and intercultural settings’ (PRIN Prot. 2020TJTA55), funded by the Italian Ministry of University and Research (MUR).
-
Data availability: In accordance with ethical considerations and to preserve company anonymity, all examples used in the paper have been anonymised. However, the original sources and documents can be provided by the corresponding author upon reasonable and justified request.
References
Austin, John L. 1962. How to do things with words. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Baden, Christian. 2010. Communication, contextualization, and cognition. London: Palgrave Macmillan.Search in Google Scholar
Benveniste, Émile. 1971. Problems in general linguistics. Translated by Mary Elizabeth Meek. Coral Gables, FL: University of Miami Press.Search in Google Scholar
Brandt, Per Aage. 1992. Per una semiotica della promessa. Urbino: Centro Internazionale di Semiotica e Linguistica.Search in Google Scholar
Cangero, Fabio. 2024. From green hype to green action: An integrated discourse analysis of sustainability reports. Paper presented at the International Conference on Enhancing Sustainability: Bridging Corporate Practices with Academic and Popular Discourse. University of Naples Federico II. Available at: https://www.iris.unina.it/handle/11588/991108.Search in Google Scholar
Croft, William & D. Alan Cruse. 2004. Cognitive linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511803864Search in Google Scholar
Ducrot, Oswald. 1984. Le dire et le dit. Paris: Éditions de Minuit.Search in Google Scholar
Enfield, Nick J. 2009. Epistemic dimensions of polar questions. In Jan Peter de Ruiter (ed.), Questions: Formal, functional and interactional perspectives, 193–221. New York: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781139045414.014Search in Google Scholar
Evans, Vyvyan & Melanie Green. 2006. Cognitive linguistics: An introduction. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Fenice, Andrea & Renzo Mocini. Forthcoming. The ‘transparency effect’ in pharmaceutical ESG reports. Iperstoria 2025(25).Search in Google Scholar
Fillmore, Charles J. 1975. An alternative to checklist theories of meaning. In Proceedings of the first annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 123–131.Search in Google Scholar
Fontaine, Lise & Ke K. Li. 2023. The discursive legitimation of corporate ecological identity in Chinese sustainability discourse. Text & Talk 44. 3–26. https://doi.org/10.1515/text-2022-0053.Search in Google Scholar
Fuoli, Matteo. 2012. Assessing social responsibility: A quantitative analysis of appraisal in BP’s and IKEA’s social reports. Discourse & Communication 6(1). 55–81. https://doi.org/10.1177/1750481311427788.Search in Google Scholar
Fuoli, Matteo. 2018. Building a trustworthy corporate identity: A corpus-based analysis of stance in annual and corporate social responsibility reports. Applied Linguistics 39(6). 846–885. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amw058.Search in Google Scholar
Greimas, Algirdas Julien & Joseph Courtés. 2007. Semiotica: dizionario ragionato della teoria del linguaggio. Milan: Bruno Mondadori.Search in Google Scholar
Halliday, Michael A. K. & Christian M. I. M. Matthiessen. 2007. An introduction to functional grammar, 3rd edn. London: Hodder Arnold.Search in Google Scholar
Hart, Christopher. 2011. Critical discourse analysis and the politics of resistance in corporate communications. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.Search in Google Scholar
Hart, Christopher. 2017. Cognitive linguistic critical discourse studies. In John Flowerdew & John E. Richardson (eds.), The Routledge handbook of critical discourse studies, 138–151. London: Routledge.10.4324/9781315739342-6Search in Google Scholar
Higgins, Colin & Brian Coffey. 2016. Improving how sustainability reports drive change: A critical discourse analysis. Journal of Cleaner Production 136. 18–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclepro.2016.01.101.Search in Google Scholar
Hornby, Albert S. (ed.). 2005. Oxford advanced learner’s dictionary of current English. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Ioannou, Ioannis & George Serafeim. 2017. The consequences of mandatory corporate sustainability reporting. Harvard Business School Working Paper 15-083. Boston: Harvard Business School.Search in Google Scholar
Koller, Veronika. 2009. Corporate self-presentation and self-centredness: A case for cognitive critical discourse analysis. In Hermine Pishwa (ed.), Language and social cognition: Expression of the social mind, 267–287. Berlin: De Gruyter.10.1515/9783110216080.2.267Search in Google Scholar
Lakoff, Robin T. 2001. The language war. Berkeley: University of California Press.10.1525/9780520928077Search in Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald W. 1987. Foundations of cognitive grammar: Volume I, theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald W. 1991. Foundations of cognitive grammar: Volume II, descriptive application. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald W. 2007. Constructing grammar: Cognitive and contextual. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald W. 2008. Cognitive grammar: A basic introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195331967.001.0001Search in Google Scholar
Larson, Brendon. 2011. Metaphors for environmental sustainability: Redefining our relationship with nature. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.10.2307/j.ctt5vm557Search in Google Scholar
Li, Yuanhua. 2024. A corpus-based transitivity analysis of the corporate image constructed in CEO statements from the CSR reports of China Mobile and Vodafone. International Journal of Advances in Social Sciences and Humanities 3(3). 194–203. https://doi.org/10.56225/ijassh.v3i3.339.Search in Google Scholar
Martin, James R. & Peter R. R. White. 2005. The language of evaluation: Appraisal in English. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.Search in Google Scholar
Milne, Markus J., Kate Kearins & Sara Walton. 2006. Creating adventures in wonderland: The journey metaphor and environmental sustainability. Organization 13(6). 801–839. https://doi.org/10.1177/1350508406068506.Search in Google Scholar
Piga, Antonio. 2023. Systemic functional linguistics in the rhetorical strategies of persuasion: A longitudinal study of transitivity and ergativity in the rhetoric of Saras’ sustainability reports. International Journal of English Linguistics 13(4). 24–36. https://doi.org/10.5539/ijel.v13n4p24.Search in Google Scholar
Purvis, Ben, Yong Mao & Darren Robinson. 2019. Three pillars of sustainability: In search of conceptual origins. Sustainability Science 14. 681–695. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11625-018-0627-5.Search in Google Scholar
Sweetser, Eve. 1990. From etymology to pragmatics: Metaphorical and cultural aspects of semantic structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511620904Search in Google Scholar
Talmy, Leonard. 1988. Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cognitive Science 12(1). 49–100. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1201_2.Search in Google Scholar
Talmy, Leonard. 1998. Frames of reference and the semantics of space. In Vyvyan Evans & Melanie Green (eds.), The cognitive linguistics reader, 21–44. London: Equinox.Search in Google Scholar
Talmy, Leonard. 2000. Toward a cognitive semantics: Volume I: Concept structuring systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/6847.001.0001Search in Google Scholar
Thompson, Geoff. 2004. Introducing functional grammar. London: Hodder Arnold.Search in Google Scholar
Ullman, Stefanie T. 2017. Conceptualising force in the context of the Arab revolutions: A comparative analysis of international mass media reports and Twitter posts. Discourse & Communication 11(2). 160–178. https://doi.org/10.1177/1750481317691859.Search in Google Scholar
Verhagen, Arie. 2005. Constructions of intersubjectivity: Discourse, syntax, and cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar
Verhagen, Arie. 2007. Construal and perspectivization. In Dirk Geeraerts & Hubert Cuyckens (eds.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive linguistics, 48–81. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar
© 2025 the author(s), published by De Gruyter, Berlin/Boston
This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.