Home Linguistics & Semiotics Patrons sociolinguistiques chez trois générations de locuteurs acadiens
Chapter
Licensed
Unlicensed Requires Authentication

Patrons sociolinguistiques chez trois générations de locuteurs acadiens

  • Louise Beaulieu and Wladyslaw Cichocki
View more publications by John Benjamins Publishing Company
Historical Linguistics 2007
This chapter is in the book Historical Linguistics 2007

Abstract

Verbs in Acadian French mark third-person-plural subject-verb agreement with the traditional suffix -ont, that is realized by the nasal vowel /õ/ (les enfants i-jou-ont “the children play”), as well as by the zero morpheme that is generally found in contemporary French and that is represented in writing as -ent (les enfants i-jou-ent, les enfants jou-ent). This study reports on variation in the use of these forms by three generations of speakers from northeastern New Brunswick (Canada) and addresses Labov’s “transmission problem”: how children learn to talk differently from their parents, and yet in the same direction, over several generations. Data from a stratified corpus of 16 adult speakers show an ongoing change: the zero morpheme is replacing the traditional -ont suffix. The main external factor that conditions this variation is social network: speakers with closed affi liation networks tend to conserve the traditional variant while those who have open networks use the zero morpheme almost exclusively. Among closed network speakers there is a significant age-by-gender interaction: older males have the highest frequencies of occurrence of the traditional -ont variant followed by younger females; younger males and older females have the lowest frequencies. The main internal conditioning factor is verb class, where classes are arranged according to the number of bases or stems. Verb classes with a small number of bases (for example, arriver “to arrive”, appeler “to call”) are more likely to be associated with conservation of the traditional suffix, while those with larger numbers of stems (including verbs such as avoir “to have”, aller “to go”, faire “to do” that have suppletive forms) are less likely. The corpus of children’s data includes recordings made with 24 speakers in three age groups (3–5, 7–9 and 10–12 years of age). The distribution of the traditional -ont suffix with respect to social network is the same as that found among adults: as early as 3 to 5 years of age, children from families with closed networks use the traditional form almost exclusively while those from open network families use it infrequently, favouring the zero morpheme. However, among all children from closed network families, the frequency of use of the traditional variant is considerably higher than among adult speakers. Furthermore, unlike the pattern observed for adult speakers, there are no significant gender differences at any age level. With respect to the “transmission problem” the results show that the verb class constraint that is formulated by children is not identical with adults’ patterns, but it does resemble more closely the model of adult women speakers than that of adult male speakers. Interestingly, the process of re-structuring the internal constraint on the traditional –ont suffix variation begins relatively late (in the 10–12 year group) when compared with ages reported for the acquisition of constraints on phonological variation. The results of this study provide evidence for a complex picture of the acquisition of sociolinguistic competence; the acquisition of social and linguistic constraints on variation does not follow a clear linear order where social patterning is acquired before (or after) linguistic constraints.

Abstract

Verbs in Acadian French mark third-person-plural subject-verb agreement with the traditional suffix -ont, that is realized by the nasal vowel /õ/ (les enfants i-jou-ont “the children play”), as well as by the zero morpheme that is generally found in contemporary French and that is represented in writing as -ent (les enfants i-jou-ent, les enfants jou-ent). This study reports on variation in the use of these forms by three generations of speakers from northeastern New Brunswick (Canada) and addresses Labov’s “transmission problem”: how children learn to talk differently from their parents, and yet in the same direction, over several generations. Data from a stratified corpus of 16 adult speakers show an ongoing change: the zero morpheme is replacing the traditional -ont suffix. The main external factor that conditions this variation is social network: speakers with closed affi liation networks tend to conserve the traditional variant while those who have open networks use the zero morpheme almost exclusively. Among closed network speakers there is a significant age-by-gender interaction: older males have the highest frequencies of occurrence of the traditional -ont variant followed by younger females; younger males and older females have the lowest frequencies. The main internal conditioning factor is verb class, where classes are arranged according to the number of bases or stems. Verb classes with a small number of bases (for example, arriver “to arrive”, appeler “to call”) are more likely to be associated with conservation of the traditional suffix, while those with larger numbers of stems (including verbs such as avoir “to have”, aller “to go”, faire “to do” that have suppletive forms) are less likely. The corpus of children’s data includes recordings made with 24 speakers in three age groups (3–5, 7–9 and 10–12 years of age). The distribution of the traditional -ont suffix with respect to social network is the same as that found among adults: as early as 3 to 5 years of age, children from families with closed networks use the traditional form almost exclusively while those from open network families use it infrequently, favouring the zero morpheme. However, among all children from closed network families, the frequency of use of the traditional variant is considerably higher than among adult speakers. Furthermore, unlike the pattern observed for adult speakers, there are no significant gender differences at any age level. With respect to the “transmission problem” the results show that the verb class constraint that is formulated by children is not identical with adults’ patterns, but it does resemble more closely the model of adult women speakers than that of adult male speakers. Interestingly, the process of re-structuring the internal constraint on the traditional –ont suffix variation begins relatively late (in the 10–12 year group) when compared with ages reported for the acquisition of constraints on phonological variation. The results of this study provide evidence for a complex picture of the acquisition of sociolinguistic competence; the acquisition of social and linguistic constraints on variation does not follow a clear linear order where social patterning is acquired before (or after) linguistic constraints.

Chapters in this book

  1. Prelim pages i
  2. Table of contents v
  3. Foreword & acknowledgements ix
  4. Introduction 1
  5. Part I. Phonology
  6. Middle English vowel length in French loanwords 9
  7. Dental fricatives and stops in Germanic 19
  8. Dialect variation and the Dutch diminutive 37
  9. Part II. Morphology, syntax and semantics
  10. On the disappearance of genitive types in Middle English 49
  11. An asymmetric view on stage II in Jespersen’s cycle in the West Germanic languages 61
  12. Temporal reference and grammaticalization in the Spanish perfect(ive) 73
  13. (Un)-interpretable features and grammaticalization 83
  14. Imperative morphology in diachrony evidence from the Romance languages 99
  15. VO vs V(…)O en Français 109
  16. On the development of Recipient passives in DO languages 123
  17. The emergence of DP in the history of English 135
  18. A diachronic view of Psychological verbs with Dative Experiencers in Spanish and Romanian 149
  19. On the loss of the masculine genitive plural in Cypriot Greek 161
  20. The rise of peripheral modifiers in the noun phrase 175
  21. Wild variation, random patterns, and uncertain data* 185
  22. Part III. Sociolinguistics and dialectology
  23. Le changement linguistique dans la langue orale selon deux recherches sur le terrain séparées d’un siècle 197
  24. Patrons sociolinguistiques chez trois générations de locuteurs acadiens 211
  25. Change of functions of the first person pronouns in Chinese 223
  26. Vinderup in real time 233
  27. Variation in real time 245
  28. Part IV. Tools and methodology
  29. UNIDIA 259
  30. Visualization, validation and seriation 269
  31. Quantifying linguistic changes 285
  32. Historical core vocabulary: Spring and/or anchor 295
  33. Index of languages and terms 307
  34. Index of subjects and terms 309
Downloaded on 8.9.2025 from https://www.degruyterbrill.com/document/doi/10.1075/cilt.308.17bea/pdf
Scroll to top button